Anda di halaman 1dari 31

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/234130999

Motivation theory in educational practice: Knowledge claims, challenges, and


future directions

Chapter · January 2012


DOI: 10.1037/13274-007

CITATIONS READS
31 9,570

3 authors:

Avi Kaplan Idit Katz


Temple University Ben-Gurion University of the Negev
87 PUBLICATIONS   6,060 CITATIONS    21 PUBLICATIONS   685 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Hanoch Flum
Ben-Gurion University of the Negev
35 PUBLICATIONS   1,177 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Motivation and Identity View project

Bootstrapping Achievement and Motivation in STEM: An Integrated Cognitive-Motivational Intervention to Improve Biology Grades View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Avi Kaplan on 25 August 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


CHAPTER 7

MOTIVATION THEORY IN
EDUCATIONAL PRACTICE:
KNOWLEDGE CLAIMS, CHALLENGES,
AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Avi Kaplan, Idit Katz, and Hanoch Flum

One important goal of scientific work in educational because these assumptions have practical as well as
psychology is the application of theoretical under- moral implications.
standing to educational practice. The domain of Moreover, even when a motivational theory has
motivation is particularly relevant to this goal been carefully evaluated and selected, it is very often
because it is concerned with the processes underly- the case that theory-guided interventions in educa-
ing the initiation, maintenance, and quality of peo- tional settings encounter obstacles of various sorts
ple’s action (Brophy, 2004; Schunk, Pintrich, & and conclude with less-than-optimal results. Indeed,
Meece, 2008; Stipek, 1998; Wentzel & Wigfield, the application of theory to educational practice is
2009; see Volume 1, Chapter 13, this handbook). itself a motivated action that involves the same com-
Motivation theory and research in educational set- plex processes in which it attempts to intervene and
tings have dealt with a wide range of phenomena, hence constitutes a motivational phenomenon wor-
including the intensity and quality of student engage- thy of conceptualization and investigation in its own
ment in schoolwork; learning and performance; off- right. Unfortunately, it is not often that motivational
task behavior such as avoidance of engagement, theorists, researchers, policymakers, or administra-
disruptive behavior, cheating, and procrastination; tors attend to the systemic motivational implications
and teacher satisfaction, engagement in teaching, and of the recommendations for applying motivation
burnout (Schunk et al., 2008; Wentzel & Wigfield, theory to educational practice (Urdan & Turner,
2009). Hence, the application of motivational theory 2005). This inattendance is most clearly the case,
in educational settings is of much interest to teach- for example, when interventions aiming to change
ers, principals, counselors, psychologists, parents, instructional practice to promote students’ motiva-
curriculum designers, and policymakers (Wentzel & tion disregard the motivation of the school adminis-
Wigfield, 2007). tration and of the teachers who are to implement the
However, the application of theoretical under- new practices.
standings to educational practice is not a straight- A decade into the 21st century, the relevance of
forward affair. There are multiple motivational theory-driven motivational recommendations for
frameworks, with different underlying assumptions educational practice is seriously questioned along
concerning the nature of motivation. Those who theoretical, methodological, practical, and moral
seek to apply motivational theory to motivate others terms (Berliner, 2006; Blumenfeld, 1992; Brophy,
must carefully and critically evaluate these assump- 2005; Hickey, 2003; Turner, 2010; Urdan & Turner,
tions along with their own beliefs, values, and goals 2005). As we argue in this chapter, whereas research
and in relation to the particular educational setting of the past decades contributed tremendously to the

We thank Tresa Grauer for her valuable comments on various drafts of this chapter.

DOI: 10.1037/13274-007
APA Educational Psychology Handbook: Vol. 2. Individual Differences and Cultural and Contextual Factors, K. R. Harris, S. Graham, and T. Urdan (Editors-in-Chief)
Copyright © 2012 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
165
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

knowledge of motivational mechanisms, these chal- Throughout the history of the scientific study of
lenges do seem to call for a reconsideration of some motivation, numerous theoretical constructs and
underlying assumptions concerning the way theo- models were developed in the attempt to explain
rists, researchers, and practitioners have conceptual- some or many of these phenomena (Brophy, 2004;
ized, investigated, and applied understandings about Schunk et al., 2008; Stipek, 1998; see Volume 1,
motivation in educational settings. Chapter 13, this handbook).
We begin our chapter with a critical perspective Because the theorists and researchers who devel-
on contemporary knowledge claims regarding the oped and continue to develop motivational theoreti-
application of motivational theory to educational cal models have different ideological views and have
practice. Our aim here is not to review the theories. been working within different zeitgeists and cultural–
Extensive reviews of motivational theories, their historical contexts, the theories themselves represent
assumptions, and their applications to educational different sets of assumptions concerning motivation,
practice can be found in other excellent sources its operation, and its valued outcomes (Berliner,
(e.g., Brophy, 2004; Schunk et al., 2008; Stipek, 2006; Hickey & McCaslin, 2001; Kaplan, 2008;
1998; Wentzel & Wigfield, 2009; see Volume 1, Weiner, 1992). Different theoretical assumptions—
Chapter 13, this handbook). Rather, for the purpose for example, concerning the stability, locus, dimen-
of this chapter, we highlight how assumptions that sionality, and specificity of motivation—have
underlie contemporary motivational perspectives important implications for understanding human
guide both theoretical explanations of motivational motivation and for the practical application of theo-
phenomena and the approach to and principles for retical understandings to practice.
applying these theoretical understandings to prac- For example, different theoretical assumptions
tice. We follow this discussion by pointing to episte- about the stability of motivation have guided different
mological assumptions about motivation and attempts to intervene in these motivational processes.
educational research shared by several of the cur- For instance, in implicit need theory (McClelland,
rently dominant motivational theories and by listing Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1953; Schultheiss &
theoretical, methodological, practical, and ethical Brunstein, 2005), and in some perspectives within the
challenges to these assumptions that can undermine achievement goal approach (Elliot, 2005), motivation
the application of these motivational theories in is conceptualized as being based in very stable per-
educational settings. Finally, we end by suggesting sonality attributes. The assumption of high stability
possible directions for addressing these challenges provides researchers little hope for changing students’
and for developing general principles for conceptu- motivational patterns. Accordingly, the implication
alizing, investigating, and applying motivational for practice in these perspectives involves the princi-
theory in educational practice. ple of matching different environments or incentive
structures to students with different attributes (cf.
Barron & Harackiewicz, 2001; Durik & Harackie-
CONTEMPORARY MOTIVATIONAL
wicz, 2003; Schultheiss & Brunstein, 2005).
PERSPECTIVES AND THEIR APPLICATION
In comparison, theoretical perspectives that view
TO EDUCATIONAL PRACTICE
motivation as malleable allow more hope for chang-
Motivation theory and research in education concern ing people’s motivation. Behaviorists, for example,
phenomena that are central to the educational proj- assume that motivation is based in associations
ect. Motivational theories aim to answer questions between environmental stimuli and individuals’
such as “Why do some students engage deeply, reactions that are malleable and that are continu-
cooperate with others, enjoy learning, perform well, ously being shaped through classical and operant
and thrive in school, whereas others procrastinate, conditioning (B. F. Skinner, 1935; Thompson &
avoid or fail to learn, disrupt the lesson, and drop Zeiler, 1986). Hence, application of behaviorist the-
out?” These questions have spurred the development oretical understandings to practice involves short-
of theory and programs of research for centuries. and long-term environmental interventions that

166
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

employ discriminative use of reinforcements and that focus on changing the environment with the
punishments that aim to increase the likelihood of aim of nurturing the motivational resources of the
desired behavior and diminish the likelihood of person (Reeve, 2004, 2009). Finally, there are moti-
undesired behavior (Codding & Poncy, 2010; vational perspectives that consider the individual
Ertmer & Newby, 1993; Vargas & Vargas, 1991). and the environment as integrated in motivated
Similarly, social–cognitive perspectives that view action. The sociocultural perspectives on motivation
motivation as based in perceptions, beliefs, and (Hickey, 2003; McCaslin, 2009; Turner, 2001), and
goals, such as self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997), expec- some cultural perspectives within the achievement
tancies and task values (Eccles-Parsons et al., 1983; goal approach (Kaplan & Maehr, 2002; Maehr &
Wigfield & Eccles, 1992), and achievement goals Nicholls, 1980), view motivation as being based in
(Ames, 1992; Maehr, 1984; Nicholls, 1989), con- people’s situated participation in communities of
sider these motivational variables to be malleable practice. Applying this view to practice includes
and to be affected by social contexts and interac- interventions that aim to modify norms and proce-
tions. Hence, application of these theories to prac- dures for participation that increase opportunities
tice commonly involves shaping the social for adaptive modes of participation (Ames, 1992;
environment in ways that are meant to promote Hickey & Zuiker, 2005; McCaslin, 2009).
adaptive cognitions such as higher perceived effi- Similarly, differences in theoretical assumptions
cacy (Usher & Pajares, 2008), higher task value concerning the dimensionality (i.e., viewing motiva-
(Wigfield & Tonks, 2002), and mastery goal orien- tion as a unidimensional quantity as opposed to
tation (Maehr & Midgley, 1996). consisting of multiple dimensions that vary in qual-
Different assumptions about the primary locus of ity) or specificity of motivation (i.e., focus on dis-
motivation—whether it is based in the individual, crete actions vs. general patterns of reactions) have
the environment, the interaction of the two, or their had consequences for the application of theoretical
integration—also result in different practical appli- understandings to educational practice.
cations. For example, the behaviorist assumption A final, noteworthy difference among motivational
that motivation resides primarily in the environmen- theories that is relevant to application to practice con-
tal reward system leads to applications that target cerns their ideological basis, which involves assump-
the environment, creating effective systems of con- tions with ethical implications. For example, some
tingent rewards (Ertmer & Newby, 1993). In com- motivational perspectives, such as the behaviorist
parison, certain sociocognitive perspectives, such as approach, are based in mechanistic metaphors of
attribution theory (Dweck, 1999; Weiner, 1986, human action that imply that motivational interven-
2010), assign the primary role to the individual’s tions involve manipulating people like machines (Teo,
cognitive characteristics. The consequent applica- 2009). Other perspectives, such as self-determination
tions are interventions such as attributional retrain- theory, are grounded in humanistic ideology and met-
ing, which focus on transforming the individual’s aphors that imply that motivational interventions
cognitive–motivational system (Perry, Hechter, should attend first and foremost to people’s human
Menec, & Weinberg, 1993; Wilson, Damiani, & needs (Reeve, 2004).
Shelton, 2002). Thus, epistemological, ontological, and ideologi-
Other motivational perspectives conceptualize cal assumptions concerning human action guide the
motivation as an interaction between the individu- conceptualization and investigation of motivation and
al’s motivational system and environmental affor- consequently the principles for application to educa-
dances. Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, tional practice. In some cases, these different assump-
1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000), for example, views moti- tions lead to quite different, at times contradictory,
vation as an interaction between the individual’s recommendations for practice. This is the case, for
organismic needs of autonomy, competence, and example, in the recommendations emanating from
relatedness and the environmental support for these self-determination theory, which focus on autonomy
needs. This assumption guides application efforts support and avoidance of external controls

167
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

(Reeve, 2009), as opposed to recommendations ema- guidelines for promoting students’ motivation. Yet,
nating from the behaviorist approach, which focus despite the decades of research and the seeming
solely on systems of contingent rewards (Ertmer & commonality of these recommendations across theo-
Newby, 1993). However, a review of the motivational retical perspectives, many concerns remain about
literature suggests that, with the exception of the the relevance of theory-guided recommendations for
behaviorist approach, theory and empirical research practice to motivational phenomena in schools and
from different motivational perspectives seem to other educational settings. Indeed, after listing the
point to significant agreement concerning general preceding recommended practices, Urdan and
principles of educational practice that are expected to Turner (2005) went on to question the validity or
promote adaptive motivation. Summarizing the rec- practicality of each and every one of them. Urdan
ommendations of a variety of social–cognitive moti- and Turner’s reservations join other challenges to
vational theories, Urdan and Turner (2005) noted the the current knowledge claims concerning the appli-
following as shared by different perspectives: cation of current theoretical motivational under-
standings to educational practice.
1. Develop and assign academic tasks and activities
that are personally meaningful and relevant to
students. CHALLENGES TO CONTEMPORARY
2. Develop and assign moderately, or appropriately, KNOWLEDGE CLAIMS ON APPLICATION
challenging tasks and material. OF MOTIVATION THEORY TO
3. Promote perceptions of control and autonomy EDUCATIONAL PRACTICE
by allowing students to make choices about
Motivation theory and research of the past decades
classroom experience and the work in which
have contributed tremendously to scientific knowl-
they engage. Also, encourage students to view
edge concerning mechanisms and processes of
intelligence, learning, and performance as per-
human motivation and engagement. However, in
sonally controllable by attributing performance
matters of application to educational practice, moti-
to controllable factors such as effort and strategy
vation theory—indeed, educational psychology
use. Avoid controlling or coercive language and
more generally—is in a rather dismal state. As Ber-
instructional practices.
liner (2006) poignantly noted in addressing the rela-
4. Encourage students to focus on mastery, skill
tions of educational psychology research and
development, and the process of learning rather
educational practice,
than just focusing on outcomes such as test
scores or relative performance. Irrelevance, sadly, may also be essence for
5. Help students develop and pursue proximal, educational psychology . . . the scientific
challenging, and achievable goals. work for which we are justifiably proud
6. Infuse the curriculum with fantasy, novelty, vari- seems not to translate easily into practice.
ety, and humor. For whatever reasons, we do not affect the
7. Provide accurate, informational feedback focused lives of teachers in the ways we had hoped.
on strategy use and competence development Our scientific work often has implications
rather than social–comparative or simply evalua- for classroom practice, but does not end
tive feedback. up often changing practice. (p. 6)
8. Assess students’ confidence, attributional ten-
There are likely many reasons for the relative fail-
dencies, and skill levels to help meet their
ure of scientific educational psychological theories
preferences for challenge and to help students
to effectively change educational practice. In this
approach tasks with realistic expectations and
section, we list a few observations that we believe
cope with difficulties adaptively. (pp. 306–307)
constitute significant challenges to the relevance of
These recommendations provide logical, theoret- current theoretically driven recommendations for
ically coherent, and empirically supported specific applying motivational theory to educational settings

168
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

(see also Turner & Meyer, 2000; Turner & Patrick, However, during the last 2 decades of the 20th
2008). The challenges are related to each other; century, research that sought to investigate these
however, for purposes of presentation, we divide our predictions began to pose challenges to this para-
discussion into challenges anchored in theory, meth- digm. One such challenge emerged as researchers
ods, practice, and ethics. interested in different aspects of educational experi-
ences (e.g., teachers, peers, parents, culture) began
Theoretical Challenges: The Mismatch to identify more and more antecedents to the central
of Theoretical Assumptions and motivational constructs, which led to the construc-
Motivational Phenomena in tion of models with a very large number of influ-
Educational Settings ences on motivational processes, and in turn on
Most contemporary motivational theories were outcomes (e.g., E. A. Skinner, Kindermann, Con-
developed during the 1970s and 1980s. The guiding nell, & Wellborn, 2009, p. 232). A review of recent
scientific paradigm during that period relied on literature has suggested, for example, that influences
assumptions similar to those of Newtonian mechan- on motivational processes are likely to include vari-
ics: linearity and determinism (Radford, 2008). ous different personality attributes (e.g., needs for
With the exception of the sociocultural perspective achievement, beliefs about intelligence), ethnicity,
(Hickey, 2003; Stetsenko, 2008), motivational theo- gender, age, previous educational experiences, prior
ries were developed along these assumptions. This knowledge, prior achievement, neighborhood char-
paradigm guided theorists to identify only a small acteristics, school characteristics, classroom charac-
number of variables (e.g., self-efficacy, controllable teristics, activity characteristics, the domain of
attribution, task value) and to specify their linear study, teacher characteristics (e.g., gender, ethnicity,
relations with outcomes for the purposes of predic- expertise, epistemological beliefs, motivation), par-
tion, control, and replication. Whereas contempo- ent characteristics, peer characteristics, and their
rary theorists abandoned the notion of modeling interactions (Wentzel & Wigfield, 2009). The para-
motivational processes with linear mathematical digm that relies on assumptions of linear relations
formulas—an important goal for the earlier genera- implies that all of these operate simultaneously,
tion of motivational theorists (Graham & Weiner, albeit with different strengths of influence, and
1996)—they nevertheless aimed at specifying a few should be accounted for when attempting to under-
predicted directional relations that are generalized stand and intervene with motivational processes in
and hence replicable across situations, contexts, and educational settings. Yet, although conceptually
populations. Examples of such predictions may be acknowledging the complicated nature of motiva-
“higher self-efficacy leads to higher effort,” “higher tional processes, these models are untenable meth-
mastery goal orientation is associated with more odologically and practically. It is simply not feasible
adaptive self-regulation strategies,” and “external to capture all these relations in an empirical study
regulation is associated with negative emotions.” that seeks to assess each of them separately. Because
The proliferation of motivational constructs and such complicated theoretical models cannot be fully
models during this period also led researchers to tested, they cannot be validly informed by research
pose different predicted relations against or along- findings. Therefore, almost by definition, current
side each other (e.g., learning goals vs. performance motivational theories present a very partial picture,
goals, autonomous vs. controlled regulation), to test with questionable validity, of motivational processes
the relative strength of constructs in predicting in actual educational environments.
motivational phenomena and outcomes (e.g., mas- Even more challenging to the underlying assump-
tery goals relative to self-efficacy), or to compare tions of linearity and determinism, and to the goals of
various linear combinations of predictive relations— prediction, control, and replicability, are inconsistent
specialized, additive, or interactive—between differ- research findings concerning the predicted linear
ent motivational constructs and outcomes (e.g., relations between motivational variables and out-
multiple achievement goals). comes under different circumstances. For example,

169
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

research has suggested that the relations of self- predictions were found for other motivational con-
efficacy and indicators of adaptive motivation are structs such as control beliefs and perceived choice
not always positive. The findings suggested that (Pintrich, 2003).
these relations might be different under different cir- Over the years, theorists and researchers also
cumstances and for different outcomes and might began to realize that attempts to resolve competing
sometime even be negative (e.g., Salomon, 1984). assumptions and hypotheses of different motiva-
Divergent findings on self-efficacy were apparent, for tional perspectives about the nature of motivation—
example, under circumstances of poor calibration for example, as conscious or nonconscious, fixed or
between students’ perceived self-efficacy and actual malleable, residing in the individual or emanating
skill level for the task, when students had different from the environment—were not contributing to
levels of prior knowledge in the content, and when comprehensive understanding of motivation. Moti-
perceptions of self-efficacy were assessed at different vational processes manifest all of these characteris-
units of analysis (e.g., situated task, curricular unit, tics. However, the guiding paradigm underlying
domain of study, college; Pintrich, 2003). A compre- current motivational theory and methods seems lim-
hensive review of research findings concerned with ited in its ability to support models that integrate
perceptions of self-efficacy among students led these dichotomies.
Schunk and Pajares (2004) to acknowledge the limi- Acknowledging this limitation, Pintrich (2003)
tations of linear predictions from self-efficacy theory called for the application of motivation theory mod-
to account for students’ motivation and achievement els from related fields (e.g., cognitive connection-
in actual educational settings: ism) that operate under very different assumptions
than the “modern Newtonian” paradigm. Connec-
Schools are complex environments in
tionist models of cognition are one example of a
which a multitude of sociocultural fac-
more general scientific paradigm that has been
tors operate that can affect students’
developing in the natural and social sciences for the
performance independently of their self-
past few decades and that has been referred to as
efficacy. Thus, highly efficacious and
complexity science, complexity theory, self-organizing
skillful students who desire friendships
systems, dynamic systems theory, and emergent sys-
with less-talented peers may purposely
tems (e.g., Fischer & Bidell, 2006; Lewis, 2000;
perform at a lower level to compare more
Morowitz, 2002; Prigogine & Stengers, 1984; Wal-
favorably with their peers and not appear
drop, 1992). Although it is still emerging, the com-
superior in ability. Students low in self-
plexity paradigm has already been applied to address
efficacy may nevertheless work diligently
challenges in a variety of social domains, including
to avoid being criticized by their teachers
psychology and human action (e.g., Fischer &
and thereby lose privileges. Students who
Bidell, 2006; Juarrero, 1999; Vallacher & Nowak,
believe that their teachers perceive them
1997), cultural systems (e.g., Strauss & Quinn,
as lacking skills may work only as hard
1997; Urry, 2005), organizational behavior (e.g.,
as they must to meet the teachers’ stan-
Axelrod & Cohen, 2000; Stacey, 2001; Weick,
dards, regardless of how self-efficacious
2005), and education1 (e.g., Jörg, Davis, & Nick-
the students feel. (p. 131)
mans, 2007; McMurtry et al., 2004; Osberg, 2005).
In natural educational settings, “conflicting The complexity paradigm relies on several basic
sociocultural pressures make direct application of assumptions that are markedly different from
self-efficacy theory problematic” (Schunk & Pajares, the currently dominant paradigm in motivation
2004, p. 116). It is perhaps not surprising, then, that research. The assumptions relevant to our discus-
similar patterns of results that diverge from the sion concern the view of psychological and educa-
expected ubiquitous linear, acontextual, replicable tional phenomena as complex and emergent

1
Interested readers may find the Complexity and Education website useful (http://www.complexityandeducation.ualberta.ca/index.htm).

170
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

systems, which have different characteristics than possibility of the creation of new subnetworks
simple systems and complicated systems through the process—means that the behavior of
(McMurtry, 2008; Osberg, 2005; Radford, 2008; complex systems cannot be predicted. Moreover, it
Rowland, 2007). cannot be influenced in a deterministic manner,
A central assumption of the complexity paradigm although some external elements may have more
is that a complex system—for example, language, weight in influencing the emergence process (e.g.,
economy, human society, personality, consciousness, a charismatic teacher; a strongly enforced rule of
meaning, learning, and, arguably, motivation—is behavior) and may have different influence at
made up of parts or elements; however, the parts different times in the life of a system (Newman &
alone do not determine its nature and characteristics. Newman, 2007).
The brain is not simply an aggregation of neurons; a Additionally, any system is, itself, an element in
classroom is not simply an aggregation of an adult, its environment—in effect, a part of a complex sys-
children, and materials; and an individual’s motiva- tem of a higher unit of analysis (e.g., a person in the
tion is not simply the aggregation of the numerous social dynamics in the situation)—and is coupled
singular antecedents listed earlier. Rather, the prop- with the larger system in the simultaneous, mutually
erties of the complex system are a function of the influencing dynamic change. In this way, the para-
parts as well as of their interrelationships, which digm allows a conceptualization of collective moti-
involve “multiple interdependencies with varying vation (e.g., the motivation of a team) that is made
types and strengths of interconnections and complex up of, but different from, the motivation of each of
patterns of positive (deviation amplifying) and nega- its members or their simple summative aggregate
tive (deviation reducing) feedback, which defy sim- (Juarrero, 1999; McMurtry, 2008).
ple cause–effect logic” (Rowland, 2007, p. 10). Complex systems are different from simple sys-
Another important characteristic of complex tems in that simple systems are characterized by a
systems is that their “behavior” (e.g., a student’s sit- small number of parts and few interconnections that
uated engagement in a task; a student’s domain- are assumed to have cause–effect relations (e.g., a
specific self-perception of ability; a classroom’s doorbell). Simple systems are easy to comprehend, to
autonomy-supportive culture) is in a perpetual pro- predict, and to control. Complex systems are also dif-
cess of emergence—continuous, dynamic, and con- ferent from complicated systems; although both are
text-sensitive change. Hence, a complex system is characterized by many parts, in the former the inter-
forever in the process of becoming. The emergence connections among the parts are relatively few and
involves the continuously changing interrelations are also characterized by cause–effect relations (e.g.,
among the system’s constitutive parts. Every turn an airplane). Such systems are more difficult to com-
in the emergence process is based in the system’s prehend; however, with enough computational
history—the earlier state of the system. The emer- power, they can be modeled and tested, and their
gence is, therefore, a “bottom-up” process to which behavior can be predicted and controlled. Current
the various constituting parts contribute, albeit with theoretical models in motivation tend to treat motiva-
different weights, by activating their interrelation- tional processes as complicated systems. Most moti-
ships. The interrelationships change dynamically as vational research has operationalized motivational
the system responds to external (i.e., environmen- processes as simple systems. However, research find-
tal) and internal (i.e., within-system) events. Hence, ings as well as attentive observation and experience
to understand a system, it is imperative to be famil- seem to question these assumptions and practice.
iar with both its history and its context (McMurtry, The complexity paradigm may be able to repre-
2008). However, although complex systems can be sent empirical findings concerning motivational pro-
investigated and learned, the nature of the emer- cesses better than models that are based on linearity
gence process—the idea that the system is in an and determinism. Moreover, the complexity para-
almost continuous mode of dynamic change, with digm may be able to guide researchers in conceptu-
shifting weights of the interrelations, and with the alizing and investigating motivational phenomena in

171
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

ways that are more compatible with their manifesta- of the complexity paradigm and have the humility of
tions in educational settings. In calling for the appli- uncertainty rather than the overconfidence of pre-
cation of complexity assumptions to motivational diction and control in their attempts to intervene
theory, Pintrich (2003) expressed the hope that it with teachers’ and students’ motivation.
would allow the “build[ing of] integrated models Acknowledging the limits of the current underly-
of the cognitive—motivational—affective self- ing assumptions of motivational theories for
system . . . that transcends some of the traditional explaining and informing practice regarding natu-
false dichotomies between stable-changeable, ratio- rally occurring motivational phenomena is a first
nale-irrational, consistent-inconsistent, conscious- step in the development of a complexity paradigm of
unconscious, controlled-automatic, and agentic- motivation that could organize current knowledge
routinized descriptions of the individual” (p. 680). claims in motivation and integrate the different
Applying complexity theory to motivational phe- research findings into more comprehensive,
nomena may also help to “bridge the current gap dynamic, contextual, and practice-relevant theoreti-
between social-cognitive and situated models of cal models.
motivation that differentially emphasize the individ-
ual or the context” and “to link our more psycholog- Methodological Challenges: The
ical models to the biological and physiological Mismatch of Prevalent Research Methods
processes involved in cognition, motivation, and and the Nature of Educational Settings
affect” (p. 680). Such models may also be able to The paradigm underlying current motivational theo-
account for other motivational phenomena, such as ries has also guided the methods by which motiva-
intentional but inconsistent behavior and serendip- tional theories have been empirically tested. In
ity (Bandura, 1982; Merton & Barber, 2004) that correspondence with the assumptions of linearity
current theoretical models find hard to incorporate. and determinism, the most prevalent research meth-
It is important to note that the complex science ods in the domain of motivation have been experi-
paradigm is still changing and taking shape. There ments and correlational studies. In recent years,
are also multiple and differing ideas within this body researchers have raised questions concerning the
of work (cf. Witherington, 2007). Moreover, com- relevance of findings from studies using these meth-
plexity science emerged from the natural sciences, ods to educational settings (e.g., Berliner, 2006; Bro-
and its translation to understanding human phe- phy, 2005; Urdan & Turner, 2005). The critics have
nomena has been primarily metaphorical, although argued that recommendations for application that
some applications have been tried (see the NetLogo are based on these findings may not be relevant to
website: http://ccl.northwestern.edu/netlogo/), even educational settings because the experimental meth-
in educational research (Maroulis et al., 2010). odology and conditions are very different from the
Researchers still need to develop language that complex contexts of classrooms and schools. Theo-
would be relevant to psychological and educational rists have noted that lab simulations and experi-
processes (Rowland, 2007) and to develop and test ments commonly represent simple systems, which
complex models to account for empirical phenom- are far less common in the world than complex sys-
ena (Human-Vogel, 2008). Some more and less suc- tems, which are much more prevalent (Rowland,
cessful attempts have been taken (Jörg, Davis, & 2007; Strogatz, 2003). In relation to self-efficacy, for
Nickmans, 2008; Jörg et al., 2007; Osberg, 2009), example, Schunk and Pajares (2004) noted that
and more are needed. However, as some theorists “schools are a far cry from controlled laboratory set-
have argued, the risk to conceptual understanding tings, which makes the operation of self-efficacy
and application to practice of mistakenly treating a more variable and its mediational and predictive
simple system as complex is far smaller than treating power more complex” (p. 131).
a complex system as simple (Rowland, 2007). By definition, experiments involve short-term sit-
Hence, arguably, it would be better for motivational uations that are designed to control for the myriad
researchers, and for educators, to adopt assumptions influences that exist in natural settings to focus on

172
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

one particular motivational process. In regular edu- 2005, p. 310). Indeed, research that looks at what
cational settings, such control does not occur. Fur- naturally occurs in classrooms has often found some
thermore, those influences that in experiments are differences between the a priori theoretical assump-
considered disturbances or errors to be controlled tions investigated with self-report surveys and the
for constitute inherent and highly relevant processes motivational processes that manifest in these class-
in regular educational settings. Whether one adopts rooms (e.g., Patrick, Anderman, Ryan, Edelin, &
a complexity system perspective on classrooms or Midgley, 2001; Turner et al., 2002; Urdan, Kneisal, &
not, it is clear that, unlike experimental conditions, Mason, 1999).
classrooms involve personal, social, and organiza- Another aspect of self-report survey methodolo-
tional histories; short- and long-term perspectives; gies that poses a methodological challenge to current
and multiple, sometimes conflicting perceptions, motivation theories and their recommendations for
values, goals, and practices, which are interrelated practice is the fact that these methods are, and are
and relevant to teachers’ and students’ motivation perceived to be, intrusions on regular classroom
(Urdan & Turner, 2005). This observation does not activity. It is not uncommon for researchers who
invalidate the contribution of basic experimental request class time for administering surveys to meet
research that seeks to understand the operation of a administrators’ and educators’ concerns that the
particular motivational mechanism. Rather, it high- research will take too much time away from educa-
lights that there is an important difference between tional activities. The discrepancy itself provides addi-
research that focuses on the operation of a particular tional strength to the validity concerns mentioned
motivational mechanism and research that focuses earlier. In addition, the sense of intrusion and the
on the same motivational mechanism within the boring experience of filling in a survey are likely to
complex context of human social action. An analogy influence the motivation of the teachers and the stu-
may be the difference between a focus on the seman- dents who are asked to provide the data. The solu-
tic definition of a word in a dictionary and a focus tion is to monitor and seek incidents that indicate
on the word’s pragmatic meaning in an actual con- inappropriate reporting and to conduct analyses of
versation, or the difference between research focus- reliability and validity. Yet, these practices are less
ing on understanding the mechanical operation of than optimal to address data biases that are a result
the human heart valve and research focusing on the of low motivation for participation. Perhaps more
adaptive procedures for a human heart transplant— important, the fact that research on motivational pro-
related, but different. cesses in education is itself incongruent with class-
The other main methodology that has contrib- room life, intrusive, and unmotivating constitutes a
uted to the development of motivational theories challenge to researchers as scientific practitioners.
and to the recommendations for application has
been correlational self-report studies. Most often, Practical Challenges: The Mismatch
such research uses measures that are researcher gen- of Recommendations for Practice and
erated and that organize students’ responses to Classroom Life
reflect the researcher’s notion of what classroom life In addition to the theoretical and methodological
is about. Moreover, the common analytic proce- challenges to the current knowledge claims concern-
dures for correlational data rely on linear statistical ing application of motivation theory in educational
modeling (e.g., hierarchical linear modeling, struc- settings, researchers have also pointed to practical
tural equation modeling), which subscribes to the challenges to such recommendations for practice.
linear, deterministic paradigm that is incompatible One important challenge comes from the realization
with classroom life. Hence, recommendations for that, in the same classroom, different students may
application that are based on theoretical under- be motivated by different processes:
standings developed through correlational research
“may offer a distorted view of the classroom” and be Some students may be motivated and
of little relevance to educators (Urdan & Turner, sustained through their self-efficacy

173
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

beliefs, whereas others are motivated to 2005, p. 306). Urdan and Turner (2005) noted,
try hard, persist, and achieve because however, that in the current educational culture,
of their goals, their personal interests, with its emphasis on testing and grades, many stu-
their value beliefs, or contextual factors dents would be likely to resist tasks of moderate
that motivate, support, and direct their challenge because they pose a risk for failure and for
behavior. (Pintrich, 2003, p. 671) lower achievement. Moreover, Urdan and Turner
noted that teachers are not very good at assessing
Another important challenge is the complexity of students’ differential skills and designing or select-
processes involved even in a particular motivational ing appropriate tasks for them.
construct. For example, Skaalvik and Skaalvik Another recommendation made by theorists
(2002) noted the immense complexity of the pro- from various perspectives is that teachers “develop
cesses by which students combine multiple internal and assign academic tasks and activities that are per-
and external sources of information that vary with sonally meaningful and relevant for students”
personal attributes and preferences, age, grade level, (Urdan & Turner, 2005, p. 306). However, as Hidi
classroom composition, school system, and educa- and Renninger (2006) noted, students’ interests are
tional practice, which are interpreted along different idiosyncratic and diverse. Teachers would find it
frames of reference and which may be providing extremely difficult to succeed in matching task con-
conflicting information, for constructing their aca- tent to students’ meaningful interests and goals.
demic self-concept. This complexity prevents Moreover, Urdan and Turner (2005) pointed to the
straightforward assessment of and intervention in common curricular constraints that teachers experi-
students’ motivation. ence and that would limit their ability to cater to
At least as important is the realization that simi- students’ interests. The pressure teachers and stu-
lar educational settings may actually provide differ- dents experience in terms of curricular guidelines
ent motivational contexts to different students, and emphasis on testing would also limit teachers’
with some motivational processes such as exercis- ability to provide students choice and control and
ing choice, decision making, and control more support their autonomy. This mismatch between the
available to some students than to others (McCas- theory-driven recommendations and the practicality
lin, 2009; McCaslin & Lavigne, 2010). The poten- of classroom life constitutes another challenge to the
tial for such diversity of motivational processes in a ecological validity of motivational theories and their
single classroom poses serious practical and moral application to educational settings.
challenges to the application of motivational theory
in educational settings. Such an understanding sug- Moral Challenges: The Ethical Dilemmas
gests that no set of recommendations for practice Inherent in Influencing Others’ Actions
from a particular motivational theory would pro- Moral and ethical considerations are another impor-
mote adaptive motivation among all the students in tant domain of challenge to the application of moti-
the context. vation theory to educational practice. Researchers
One possible solution may be to target recom- and educators aiming to apply motivational pro-
mendations for practice that are common to various cesses in educational practice need to reflect on their
motivational theories (e.g., Urdan & Turner, 2005, role in influencing others’ lives. Not the least of
pp. 308–309). However, the practicality of these these concerns is the way the interventionist is per-
recommendations also has to be questioned. For ceived. McClelland (1978) worried that “the motiva-
example, one shared understanding by several tional psychologist has acquired in some quarters
motivational theories is that moderate challenge is the negative image of someone who can use infor-
motivating. Hence, motivational theorists and mation to motivate people to do something they
researchers have recommended that teachers don’t want to do” (p. 201). This moral and practical
“develop and assign moderately, or appropriately, issue should be of concern to researchers and practi-
challenging tasks and material” (Urdan & Turner, tioners who seek a collaborative relationship with

174
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

the people they are trying to study and affect—a on intrinsic motivation (e.g., Cameron, 2001; Deci,
desirable and perhaps necessary state of affairs, both Koestner, & Ryan, 1999a, 1999b, 2001; Deci, Ryan, &
motivationally and morally, and one recognized Koestner, 2001; Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996;
even by behaviorist psychologists (e.g., Fantuzzo & Eisenberger, Pierce, & Cameron, 1999; Lepper,
Atkins, 1992). Henderlong, & Gingras, 1999). Notably, the world-
Clearly, the moral issues involved in changing view held by some scientists that science should
others’ motivation go much beyond the concern focus on discovering objective empirical truth and
with perceptions of the interventionist. Motivational be devoid of ideology and values is itself, paradoxi-
researchers and practitioners should seriously con- cally perhaps, an ideological stance with serious
sider the moral implications of the goals and the moral implications.
means of applying motivational theory in educa- The understanding that the development of sci-
tional settings. For example, noting the possible ence and of scientific theories is infused with ideo-
implications of adopting a behaviorist approach, logical debates cautions against viewing theoretical
Bruner (1990) quoted Wolfgang Kohler’s hypotheti- assumptions about what educational goals are wor-
cal worry about a situation in which the postman thy and desirable as ontological truths. Hence, the
and the prime minister would also come to make confidence that goals and means that are based on
decisions and treat others on the basis of the belief these assumptions would necessarily improve peo-
that “human nature is . . . nothing but the concate- ple’s lives is tentative and should be the subject of
nation of conditioned reflexes, associative bonds, deliberation. Whereas such tentative confidence
[and] transformed animal drives” (p. 31). may not be completely irreconcilable with the orien-
But the most significant moral dilemmas in this tation of motivational theorists working with the
domain concern people’s actual lives—those people current perspectives, it does stand at odds with the
whose motivation is the target of the study and underlying scientific paradigm that assumes deter-
intervention and others who are in their social net- minism and seeks prediction and control.
works. The core dilemma can be phrased in terms of More important, entertaining the understanding
the level of confidence that the researchers or practi- that the goals and the means of motivational change
tioners have that their goals and means of studying are not absolute is far from advocating a relativist
and influencing motivational change will indeed outlook on ideology and values or suggesting inac-
improve people’s lives and not cause them harm. As tion (cf. Bruner, 1990). Scientists should be highly
we emphasized earlier, the goals and means of theory- reflective of their values and strive to explicitly align
driven motivational interventions are inextricably them with their research questions, methods, and
coupled with underlying ideology and assumptions interpretation. However, in addition, scientists
regarding what is worthy and desirable. The history should consider (a) the likelihood that the people
of science generally, and of social science and educa- whom they are trying to influence may not share
tion particularly, are rich with ideological debates their ideas about the worthy goals of education and
and paradigmatic shifts that positioned against each development; (b) how the different values held by
other values and goals relating to the issues under those people might reflect valid and important cir-
investigation, as well as to the scientific project itself cumstances in their lives; and (c) how the interven-
(Latour, 1987). The domain of motivation theory in tion might need to change to accommodate these
education is no exception and is clearly manifested, values. If ignored, the practical problem of intrusion
for example, in the debates around the policy and into classrooms may pale relative to the potential
practical implications of empirical findings regard- implications of interventions that may harm teachers
ing the relations of performance-approach goals and and students by inadvertently limiting their current
student outcomes (e.g., Elliot & Moller, 2003; Har- or future opportunities or by facilitating discrimina-
ackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich, Elliot, & Thrash, 2002; tion or differential treatment and access to resources
Kaplan & Middleton, 2002; Midgley, Kaplan, & despite the most noble intentions (Delpit, 1995;
Middleton, 2001) and the effect of extrinsic rewards McCaslin, 2009; McCaslin & Lavigne, 2010).

175
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

At least according to the democratic principles However, once again, such practices seem
hailed by many if not most educators in the Western incompatible with the scientific paradigm that val-
world, the principles and practices that foster adap- ues acontextual and generalized theoretical knowl-
tive motivation should not be incompatible—and edge claims. How may researchers incorporate their
desirably should coalesce—with principles and own theory-driven ideas about what is valued into
practices that foster tolerance, appreciation of differ- potentially conflicting ideas held by the practitioners
ence, and self-determination of the “other” (cf. and students in the context? For example, what
Gates, 1995). However, any practice provides higher might be done if researchers aim to promote auton-
legitimacy to certain behaviors and experiences and omy and relatedness, whereas teachers and students
less legitimacy to others (Giroux, 1985). Theory- value teachers’ authority and students’ conformity
driven motivational interventions explicitly aim at (cf. Delpit, 1988; Triandis, 1989)?
creating experiences, subjective perceptions, identi-
ties, and behaviors of certain kinds—the kinds Summary
favored by the theorists. A moral dilemma exists The guiding paradigm on which most contemporary
when considering the possibility that although pro- motivational theories were founded and developed
moting some valued motivations, the interventions is facing some serious challenges. The modern-era
might delegitimize the actualization of different Newtonian assumptions of linearity and determin-
teacher and student experiences, particularly those ism and the goals of prediction and control that
who traditionally have been suppressed. stand at the foundation of this paradigm can be chal-
Solutions to these challenges would include lenged on theoretical, methodological, practical, and
becoming deeply familiar with the culture and con- moral grounds. Whereas the paradigm calls for spec-
text of those whose motivation is to be influenced, ifying a few central variables and their linear, cause-
attending particularly to those domains of experi- and-effect relations, the reality of motivation in
ence that might come into conflict. Additionally, educational settings may be more compatible with
motivation researchers and practitioners should complexity models that assume that phenomena
involve the voices of those whose motivation is to be such as motivation continuously emerge from con-
changed in a constructive negotiation over the val- text-sensitive and dynamic interrelations among a
ues and goals of the program. Teachers and students very large number of elements. Whereas contempo-
should be made aware of the decision-making pro- rary theories were developed primarily with experi-
cesses and the underlying reasons for developing mental and correlational methods, experimental
practices, and they should be taught skills for criti- conditions are quite different from characteristics of
cally evaluating these reasons, goals, and processes educational environments, and researcher-generated
(cf. Giroux, 1985). Teachers and students should self-report measures may provide a distorted picture
become critics and coconstructors of the motiva- of processes occurring in natural settings. Recom-
tional change (e.g., Colsant, 1995; Delpit, 1988; Lee, mendations derived from contemporary motiva-
1995; Nicholls, 1989; Nicholls & Hazzard, 1995; tional theories are found to be impractical or
Sarason, 1990; Thorkildsen & Nicholls, 1991). As unfeasible to implement in real classrooms. Finally,
several of the motivational theories already pro- the underlying paradigm guiding contemporary
claim, students’ opinions and decision making play motivational theories seems to be limited in its abil-
an important role in their engagement in academic ity to address moral considerations that call for only
activities, as well as in their well-being in school. tentative endorsement of theoretical convictions and
Becoming aware of and listening to students’ inter- for practices such as incorporating participants’ per-
pretations and voices in designing motivational spectives on the goals and means of motivational
interventions would make the programs more change programs into those programs.
democratic as well as more meaningful to students The challenges suggest different assumptions as
(Corbett & Wilson, 1995; Flum & Kaplan, 2006; guides for developing motivational theory and research
Sarason, 1990). that would be relevant to educational settings. In the

176
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

next section, we consider a few of these assumptions more user inspired (cf. Stokes, 1997). Schoenfeld
and their implications for motivational theory, called for breaking the distinction between basic and
research, and educational practice. applied research to create a synergy between theory
and practice. He argued that “educational research
has evolved to the point where it is possible, much
ADDRESSING THE CHALLENGES:
of the time, to conduct research in contexts that are
DIRECTIONS FOR PROMOTING
of practical import, working on problems whose
APPLICATION OF MOTIVATION THEORY
solutions help make things better and contribute to
IN EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
theoretical understanding” (p. 5). Schoenfeld con-
The challenges we have listed highlight assump- tended that “finding and working on such problems
tions that would be conducive for developing is a high-leverage strategy for making a difference in
motivational theory and research more relevant for the years to come” (p. 5) and called for the pursuit
application in educational settings and practice. In the of research questions that are context sensitive, such
final section of this chapter, we propose how some of as “What does it take to really be able to do X?” with
these assumptions may be translated into guidelines X being a specific skill or “How do teachers manage
for the development of practice-relevant motivational to do what they do, ‘on line,’ in the classroom?”
models and research. We anchor the assumptions in (p. 6). Moreover, he urged educational researchers
guidelines for the core practices of empirical research: to construct their research questions along the fol-
generating research questions, adopting a research lowing principles: “How can the issues under inves-
orientation, and selecting and using a specific tigation be framed so that the contributions to
research methodology. We believe that these guide- fundamental understanding are as large as possible?
lines could, and arguably should, frame future scien- And, how can we situate this work so that the
tific action in the domain of motivation in education. contributions to practice are as large as possible?”
(p. 9). To this, we may add, “And how can this
Practice-Relevant Research Questions investigation be sensitive to the life conditions of
Commonly, theoretical developments under the cur- the participants?”
rently dominant motivational paradigm follow theo- Other theorists and researchers have similarly
ry-driven research questions that aim at the central emphasized the great need for scientific research
theoretical constructs rather than at the naturally that is conducted in natural settings and that
occurring motivational phenomena. Such questions addresses practice-relevant research questions. In
constrain empirical investigations and theoretical his review of the state of the field of educational psy-
interpretation and keep them within the boundaries chology, Berliner (2006) pointed to a resurgence of
of the theory. This is one reason for the very partial interest in practice as a valid and important anchor
capacity of current motivational theories to compre- for psychological theory building. He perceived this
hensively depict the complexity of motivational phe- trend of practice-relevant research as a turning point
nomena in educational settings. Therefore, one in the field of educational psychology, saying,
critical understanding for developing practice-relevant
It is not just respect for practice that makes
motivational theory is that practice-relevant theory
this turn of our field so exciting. It is the
emerges from practice-relevant research. In turn,
understanding that theory building in situ,
practice-relevant research begins with practice-
in the real world, constitutes our greatest
relevant research questions. Hence, the first guide-
scientific challenge and the most exciting
line for conducting practice-relevant research is to
opportunity to affect practice. (p. 22)
generate practice-relevant research questions.
A little more than a decade ago, in his presiden- Berliner argued that what educational psychologists
tial address at the American Educational Research should focus on is “psychologizing the problems of
Association annual meeting, Schoenfeld (1999) practice, that is, making them more often the source
noted the need for educational theory to become of our inquiries” (p. 24).

177
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

Endorsing the guideline of generating practice- of research questions, there will need to be a change
relevant research questions means a significant shift in the epistemological perspective regarding the
to a focus on practical concerns from the currently nature and purpose of empirical research. The
normative practice of generating research questions understanding that motivational phenomena are
that focus on theoretical constructs. For example, highly context bound and complex and that engag-
instead of a theory-centered research question such ing in research and intervention has important
as “What is the relationship between utility task moral consequences points to several assumptions
value and self-regulated learning?” the researcher about the research practice itself that should frame a
may ask a question that focuses on educational practice-relevant orientation to motivational theory
action in context, such as “What needs to occur for and research.
students in this educational context to engage in For example, the understanding that motiva-
self-regulated learning?” Instead of a construct- tional phenomena are context bound calls for adopt-
focused question such as “What is the effect of ing the assumption that each investigation is a case
maintained situational interest on persistence in the study. Regardless of whether the study is an experi-
face of difficulty?” the researcher may ask a practice- ment conducted in the computer laboratory of an
relevant question such as “What may promote per- Ivy League university with 90 undergraduate stu-
sistence in the face of difficulty among students in dents taking Introduction to Psychology, an obser-
this context?” Instead of a correlational question vation study conducted in five science kindergarten
such as “What is the relationship between teachers’ classrooms, or a survey administered to a nationally
achievement goals for teaching and students’ representative sample of 14,000 high school stu-
achievement and well-being?” the researcher may dents, each set of data represents the characteristics
pose a practice-relevant question such as “What of the context, the historical period, and the particu-
teachers’ attitudes and actions would promote stu- lar participants—a case (cf. Cronbach, 1982). Under
dents’ achievement and well-being in this educa- the current dominant paradigm, findings from these
tional context?” As noted before, research that studies (and in practice, unfortunately, most com-
follows theory-centered research questions makes monly from the findings on undergraduate students)
important contributions that advance knowledge would be interpreted as indicating acontextual gen-
about motivational constructs and processes such as eralized psychological mechanisms. In contrast, the
utility task value, situational interest, and achieve- assumption that each study is a context-bound case
ment goals. However, this knowledge may have little highlights the fact that the data are unique. Follow-
relevance for practice if these particular motivational ing the practice of case-study research, empirical
processes are not the ones that meaningfully capture findings would not be generalized to others in the
the motivational phenomena in the particular con- population but to the theory, while taking account
text. From a practice-relevant perspective, acontex- of the unique characteristics of the case (Yin, 2008).
tual theoretical knowledge concerning the operation When the case-study approach is combined with
of particular motivational constructs could and practice-relevant research questions about contextu-
should inform analysis, interpretation, and educa- alized motivational action, the findings can be gen-
tional practice if and when it is found to be relevant eralized to motivational theory, provide relevant
to the phenomena in question. Yet, this relevance information to practitioners and students, and
can only be determined through analysis of rich, inform practice in the particular context.
context-sensitive, and practice-relevant data that are Another important understanding from the chal-
generated through research guided by a practice- lenges posed that should frame the researchers’ ori-
relevant research question. entation is that motivational processes—and the
educational research process itself—are complex,
Practice-Relevant Research Orientation emergent systems and hence cannot be fully pre-
Another insight from the challenges outlined in the dicted or controlled. The traditional orientation
previous section is that alongside the change in type toward research seeks to predict and control all

178
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

aspects of the study, from schedule through partici- context and for informing practice. Clearly, the par-
pants, data collection, and analysis to the hypothe- ticipants themselves often do not have all of the rele-
sized motivational outcomes. In contrast, the vant information, or know which information is
understanding that motivation and the educational important. Yet, without the participants’ expertise
research process are complex systems implies that and intentional collaboration, the research will not
expectations for prediction and control need to be produce the ecologically valid understandings of the
relinquished. More important, relinquishing the motivational processes most relevant to the context.
assumption of predictability and controllability of There is also a motivational rationale for partici-
the research process and its outcomes does not pants’ full involvement in practice-relevant research.
imply passivity or helplessness. On the contrary, After all, it is the participants—the principals, teach-
constituents of complex systems may indeed influ- ers, coaches, and students—not the researchers,
ence their emerging process, sometimes with unex- who will be applying understandings to educational
pected ferocity. Whereas complex processes may practice. They need to endorse the value of the
counter the intended effects of any action, highly inquiry and its relevance to their practice to apply
informed actions are more likely to influence the its understandings. On the basis of experiences in
system in the intended direction. Thus, whereas it is motivational interventions, David McClelland (iron-
impossible to fully predict the effect of action, it is ically, the scholar most associated with a view of
possible to anticipate it (cf. Geertz, 1973, p. 26). An motivation as an unconscious, stable personality
important difference between researchers who attribute) observed that
expect to control and predict and researchers who
it is extraordinarily difficult to get people
anticipate is that the former perceive unexpected
to do what they don’t want to do. If you
events as problems and unpredicted outcomes as
want to change what they want to do, you
failure, whereas the latter perceive unexpected
had better let them in on the desires you
events and unanticipated outcomes as natural occur-
want them to have—and that, of course,
rences to be integrated into theoretical understand-
gives them the freedom to refuse your
ing of the motivational processes in the context. The
assistance. (McClelland, 1978, p. 209)
derived assumption for a practice-relevant research
orientation may be that the research process and its Forming a collaboration between researchers and
outcomes may be anticipated but not predicted. participants in which participants assume an equal-
A third important understanding about research- status role in negotiations and decision making
ing and intervening with motivational phenomena regarding the study—from posing the research ques-
that emerges from the challenges posed earlier is that tions to the design of data collection and its analysis
the researcher is not the ultimate authority on what to the interpretation and translation of findings to
takes place in the educational context—practically, practice—is likely to support their autonomy, com-
methodologically, or even conceptually (Turner, petence, and relatedness (Deci, 2009) and to
2010). Researchers have much expertise—in theo- increase the anticipation that they internalize the
retical knowledge, systematic data collection, value of the study, endorse its findings, and apply
rigorous analysis of data, and interpretation and its implications.
conceptualization of findings. However, these skills Finally, the crucial role of participants’ voice in
are acontextual. The researcher has little or no the design and conduct of the research is not only
knowledge of the characteristics of the context: the for purposes of efficient data collection, ecological
history and characteristics of participants and of the validity, and motivated involvement and implemen-
organization, their values and goals, and the actual tation, it is also a moral imperative. When partici-
and perceived constraints on their decision making pants take an authoritative role in the research
and action. It is the participants who have expertise design, the ethical convictions regarding support of
in this information, which is crucial for understand- participants’ autonomy are exercised and concerns
ing the motivational processes taking place in that about potential harm to participants are somewhat

179
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

alleviated. Therefore, the implication is the assump- incidents). Moreover, some quantitative analytical
tion that research should be grounded in research- techniques are compatible with constructivist and
ers’ and participants’ equal-status collaborative nondeterministic epistemological assumptions about
relationships (cf. Turner, 2010). the phenomenon measured (e.g., Guttman, 1968;
Rasch, 1960/1980; although see Trendler, 2009).
Practice-Relevant Research Methodology Ultimately, research should collect the data that are
Practice-relevant research questions and orienta- relevant to the research question—whether they be
tions should be pursued with a compatible method- qualitative, quantitative, or both.
ology. Clearly, theory-driven laboratory experiments This understanding highlights the distinction
or self-report correlational methods would not suf- between different methods (e.g., surveys, observa-
fice. What is required is a methodology that would tions, interviews) for collecting different types of
be flexible enough to provide a rich outlook on the data (i.e., quantitative and qualitative) and the
complex dynamic of action in natural settings. Not methodologies within which these methods are used
surprisingly, various kinds of qualitative research (e.g., experiments, ethnography, case studies, self-
methods come to mind. Several motivational theo- report correlational studies, interventions). The
rists who have found experimental and correlational incompatibility of laboratory experiments and self-
research to be limited have also made this observa- report correlational studies with practice-relevant
tion (e.g., Kaplan & Maehr, 2007; Patrick & Mid- theory and research lies not only in the type of data
dleton, 2002; Skaalvik & Skaalvik, 2002; Turner & they generate but in the epistemological assump-
Meyer, 2000). For example, in noting the limitations tions concerning the purpose of research that guide
of current theory and research to understanding the the methodology: the overall approach to collection,
complexity of self-efficacy processes, Usher and analysis, and interpretation of the data. Therefore,
Pajares (2008) commented, the challenges to contemporary motivational theory
and research posed earlier call for reconsidering the
Qualitative investigations hold great
general methodological approach to research meth-
promise for providing a rich under-
ods that has been normatively used (rather than just
standing of the genesis of students’
the data collection instruments). What practice-
self-efficacy beliefs, as they have the
relevant research should use are methodological
potential to describe the heuristic tech-
approaches that are compatible with the theoretical,
niques students use to attend to, weigh,
methodological, practical, and ethical assumptions
and appraise the degree of influence the
and guides that are highlighted by the challenges—
sources have on their self-efficacy. . . .
methodologies that correspond with a broad case-
Qualitative approaches such as grounded
study approach that pursues practice-relevant
theory, ethnography, classroom obser-
research questions through nondeterministic
vations, interview techniques, and case
equal-status collaborations of researchers and
studies, particularly when conducted
practitioners.
over time, would offer new perspectives
Arguably, the most direct way to investigate a
about the low or high self-efficacy beliefs
practice-relevant research question is to enact the
students come to hold. (p. 784)
practice in question and evaluate it, which points to
However, whereas qualitative methods have a interventions as a methodology of choice. Interven-
clear advantage in their ability to richly portray tions aim to apply and test the consequence of practice
complex processes, quantitative data should not be in a natural setting. However, interventions are an
ignored. Such data may, in fact, be imperative for umbrella category for many different types of research.
describing certain phenomena that can only be cap- Not all interventions are compatible with the assump-
tured with numbers (e.g., trends in students’ grades, tions and guides we have highlighted. For example,
amount of time spent on different activities, preva- some intervention studies that are set to test very spe-
lence and frequency of different types of behavioral cific researcher-generated, theory-focused questions

180
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

and hypotheses; do not involve an equal-status collab- aims to explain and improve. In its authenticity, this
oration between researchers and practitioners in the type of research can be promoting scientific knowl-
research process; use quasi-experimental designs that edge while at the same time constructively critiqu-
are similar to experiments in their focus on a single ing the day-to-day social practices and inviting
motivational process for a short period of time; are the agents-in-context to see themselves in the scien-
deterministic in their expectation of controlling dis- tific portrait that they effectively draw together
turbances; and target statistical significance of differ- (cf. Bruner, 1990, pp. 31–32).
ence between the experimental and the control groups Such a research methodology would be quite dif-
to support theoretical predictions, with little insight ferent from the traditional methodology commonly
into the actual psychological processes taking place used in psychological research. Yet, researchers
during engagement. As such, these types of interven- interested in pursuing this type of practice-relevant
tions do not constitute adequate approximations of research do not need to invent the methodological
natural educational environments, and their findings wheel. There are methodologies that correspond
will be of limited relevance to practice. As Schoenfeld with the assumptions and guides we have listed and
(1999) noted, the methodology that should guide that cater specifically to research that aims to make
practice-relevant research is significant contributions to theory as well as to prac-
tice. Two such methodologies are design-based
not a pre-determined comparison study
research and participatory action research.
in which one randomly selected group
of students receives Treatment A and
Design-based research.2 The terms design experi-
another receives Treatment B, but a good
ments or design-based research methods (Brown, 1992;
faith effort that draws upon available
Collins, 1992; Kelly, 2003; Sandoval & Bell, 2004)
knowledge to make needed change in a
refer to a rather diverse group of research methods
real world context. (p. 12)
that share the methodological approach of using
Taking this conclusion seriously means that design principles for developing theoretical under-
research that addresses the challenges and is rele- standings about adaptive learning in context and
vant to educational practice is research that is inte- about the instructional technology, curricula, and par-
grated with the educational practice itself. As ticipation structures that support such learning. The
Schoenfeld (1999) emphasized, “This kind of premise of design-based methods is that researchers
approach does not represent a weak alternative to “can learn important things about the nature and
conducting controlled experiments, but a different conditions of learning by attempting to engineer and
option altogether. Sometimes the only way you can sustain educational innovation in everyday settings”
understand complexity is to study complex things” (Bell, 2004, p. 243). Perhaps the essence of design-
(p. 12). In such research, the dichotomy between based methods can be captured by what Schoenfeld
research and practice breaks down (Schoenfeld, (1999) considered to be the “maxim that lies at the
1999). It is research that uses the natural educa- heart” of the approach: “Sometimes you have to build
tional practice as the foundation for investigating something to see if it will work . . . and then you have
how that practice in context gives rise to motiva- to study the hell out of it” (p. 12).
tional phenomena (cf. Hickey, 2003). Such research Whereas the roots of design-based research can
methodology values the authenticity of the practice be traced to the traditions of program evaluation
because it is this authenticity that provides for the and curricular design, the impetus for the contem-
simultaneous pursuit of ecologically valid theoreti- porary approach to design-based research has been
cal knowledge as well as high relevance for practice. the work of Ann Brown, Allan Collins, Roy Pea, and
This type of research methodology, and the theory Jan Hawkins (Kelly, 2003). These researchers’ grow-
that it informs, speaks to those people whose lives it ing frustration with the ability of traditional research

2
See Tabak (2004) for a more elaborate discussion on how design-based methods may address some of the challenges to contemporary motivational research.

181
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

methods to represent learning processes in complex of hypotheses in one cycle leads to the generation
environments led them to develop alternative of new theoretical understandings and hypotheses
research procedures (Brown, 1992; Collins, 1992). that guide the next cycle.
Their work spurred the development of several 5. Finally, the theoretical understandings that
research programs that focus on the investigation of result from design-based research are practice
theories of learning through the design of learning based—they “do real work” (Cobb et al., 2003,
activities in authentic educational contexts (Kelly, p. 10) and are easily communicable to practitio-
2003; Sandoval & Bell, 2004). ners. As such, “the theoretical products of design
Despite differences in various strands of design- experiments have the potential for rapid pay-off
based methods in educational research (Bell, 2004; because they are filtered in advance for instru-
O’Donnell, 2004), Cobb, Confrey, diSessa, Lehrer, mental effect. They also speak directly to the
and Schauble (2003) pointed to five cross-cutting types of problems that practitioners address in
features (p. 9) that, in combination, distinguish the course of their work” (p. 11).
these methods from other forms of research:
Design-based methods are guided by research
questions that originate in a theoretical idea and are
1. Design-based methods aim to investigate and
commonly focused on relatively specific instruc-
develop theory (diSessa & Cobb, 2004; San-
tional actions. Design researchers see this focus as
doval, 2004). The research process begins with
providing an anchor that helps in coping with the
a theoretical idea about affecting an educational
messiness of the educational environment (Joseph,
process, progresses through the investigation
2004). The following research steps, however, posi-
of the theoretical idea in a particular natural
tion and investigate the focused theoretical hypothe-
educational context, and results in more devel-
sis within the complexity of authentic practice. For
oped theoretical understandings that can be
example, a concern with the motivation of urban
generalized to other contexts. Each design, and
middle school students in the domain of world his-
the processes it aims to promote, “are viewed
tory may lead to the following hypothetical research
as instances of broader classes of phenomena”
phases:
(Cobb et al., 2003, p. 10)—in effect, cases with
unique characteristics that can inform a more 1. Design: Researchers will use the model of interest
general theory. development (Hidi & Renninger, 2006) to design
2. Design-based research is centered on an interven- software with features that are hypothesized to
tion that concerns particular functional actions. trigger and maintain urban middle school stu-
More specifically, in design-based research, the dents’ situational interest in world history.
research team enacts actions in the educational 2. Enactment: The researchers would then collabo-
context that are theoretically hypothesized to rate with history teachers in an urban middle
promote desirable educational processes. school in implementing the software as part of
3. The implementation of the design and the inves- the world history curriculum.
tigation of its effect are both “prospective and 3. Analysis: The collaboration would also involve
reflective” (Cobb et al., 2003, p. 10). The imple- collecting multiple forms of longitudinal, multiple-
mentation is guided by the prospect of affecting level, qualitative, and perhaps also quantitative
the hypothesized processes, and the data collec- data about the broad processes involving trigger-
tion and analysis are guided by reflective inves- ing and maintaining students’ situational interest
tigations of the hypotheses concerning both the and about the use of the software in the class-
target processes and the means to affect them. rooms. These rich data may range from microge-
4. The research process involves iterative cycles of netic engagement with the software of individual
“invention and revision” (p. 10) or “design, enact- students to more macro-level classroom dis-
ment, analysis, and redesign” (Design-Based course taking place throughout the curricular
Research Collective, 2003, p. 5), in which refutation unit to students’ continuing engagement in

182
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

world history beyond the classroom context example, a National Research Council committee
(cf. Cobb et al., 2003). Analysis of these data that examined research practices in education with
would support or refute the initial hypothesis, the aim of recommending those that provide high-
but, in addition, would provide insight into quality scientific evidence criticized the ability of
the broader, naturally occurring motivational design-based methods to provide scientific “war-
processes taking place while the teachers and rants” (Shavelson, Phillips, Towne, & Feurer,
students were using the software. These analyses 2003). The committee members criticized the narra-
would inform the theoretical underpinnings of tive form that commonly frames the findings of
triggering and maintaining situational interest in design-based research and questioned the ability of
world history among urban middle school stu- such research that lacks “more traditional controls
dents, as well as the features of the software that on extraneous variables” (p. 27) to produce valid,
could effectively be changed for this purpose. warranted, replicable, and generalizable causal
4. Redesign: The results of the analysis would then claims. The committee members recommended that
lead to the revision of the software design so that design-based researchers introduce randomized
it fits better with the emerging theoretical under- experiments in appropriate stages into the design
standings and the characteristics of the particular research with the aim of testing specific causal
educational context. hypotheses.
5. Additional cycles: New hypotheses would be An extensive discussion of current challenges
generated about the effect of the software, and and criticisms of design-based research methods is
another cycle of enactment and data collection beyond the scope of this chapter. These issues not-
would ensue. Further iterations would take withstanding, more important to the current project
place until a decision was made to stop—either is that even the critics realize the potential of this
because the researchers and teachers reached the research approach to address contemporary chal-
conclusion that the software was in a state that lenges to traditional educational research:
satisfied the need of motivating the students or
The strengths of design studies lie in
because of other constraints (e.g., the funding
testing theories in the crucible of prac-
ended).
tice; in working collegially with practi-
6. Summative analysis: A retrospective analysis
tioners, coconstructing knowledge; in
would then trace the multiple forms of longitudi-
confronting everyday classroom, school,
nal data collected through the research to gener-
and community problems that influ-
ate “rigorous, empirically grounded claims and
ence teaching and learning and adapt-
assertions” (Cobb et al., 2003, p. 12) concerning
ing instruction to these conditions; in
the use of the software for the triggering and
recognizing the limits of theory; and in
maintenance of situational interest in world his-
capturing the specifics of practice and
tory among urban middle school students. More
the potential advantages from iteratively
important, theories that are developed through
adapting and sharpening theory in its
design-based research are humble, “not merely in
context. (Shavelson et al., 2003, p. 25)
the sense that they are concerned with domain-
specific learning processes, but also because they Participatory action research. Action research is
are accountable to the activity of design” (p. 10). another broad category referring to a very diverse
This is also their power—they are able to explain set of research methods aimed at investigating prac-
the operation of instructional actions on psycho- tice in natural settings. The action research category
logical processes in complex settings. includes approaches to inquiry that vary both in
label (e.g., action science [Argyris, Putnam, & Smith,
The methodological domain of design-based 1985; Friedman, 2001], action inquiry [Torbert,
research is still emerging (O’Donnell, 2004). It has 2001], appreciative inquiry [Ludema, Cooperrider, &
also encountered challenges and criticism. For Barrett, 2001]) and in emphasis on the primary

183
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

purpose and methods of inquiry (e.g., a primary promotion of well-being and social justice (Heron &
emphasis on description vs. transformation of prac- Reason, 2001). Accordingly, action research is most
tice, the centrality of the purpose to promote social often guided by research questions that concern
justice, the balance in the voice of researchers and social challenges and aim to improve the human
practitioners, the salience of theory building as a condition. Thus, unlike the researcher-generated,
goal, the emphasis on rigor in methods; Cassell & theory-guided research question and innovative
Johnson, 2006; Chandler & Torbert, 2003; Dick, design that are common in design-based methods,
Stringer, & Huxham, 2009; Heron & Reason, 2001; action researchers begin with an open-ended and
Rapoport, 1970; Zeichner, 2001). However, similar to wide-ranging research question that is based in prac-
design-based research, action research “seeks to bring titioners’ concerns with naturally occurring social
together action and reflection, theory and practice” phenomena of practice.
(Reason & Bradbury, 2008, p. 4). Moreover, action Furthermore, as part of its value-based founda-
research is also centered on an iterative cyclical pro- tion, action research is guided by the premise “that
cess of inquiry and intervention, which includes the human systems could only be understood and
investigation of actions through the collection of changed if one involved the members of the system in
multiple-form, longitudinal, rich data whose analy- the inquiry process itself” (Brydon-Miller et al., 2003,
sis generates theoretical understanding and revision pp. 13–14). Thus, rather than a pragmatic necessity,
of action that lead to a new action research cycle. the equal-status researcher–practitioner collaboration
Indeed, when broadly defined, the variety of design- is an integral and highly valued aspect of participa-
based methods can be viewed as representing specific tory action research, and it involves both researchers
types of action research. and practitioners in all stages of the research process,
In this discussion, we focus on a particular type from generating the research question to deciding on
of action research labeled participatory, collabora- the research methods and collection of the data, to
tive, or cooperative action research (Heron & Reason, the analysis and interpretation of the findings, to the
2001; McIntyre, 2008; Whyte, 1991). Unlike some design of the intervention in all cycles of action and
other types of action research (e.g., practitioners’ research (Heron & Reason, 2001).
self-study), and similar to design-based methods, For example, teachers’ concern with the motiva-
participatory action research involves the collabora- tion of urban middle school students in the domain of
tion of researchers and practitioners who apply their world history may lead to the following hypothetical
combined expertise to the investigation of authentic action research phases (cf. Heron & Reason, 2001):
practices in natural contexts. However, whereas par-
ticipatory action research and design-based methods 1. Creating a collaborative inquiry team and concep-
share many features, they also differ in some ways tualizing a research question: Researchers and
that seem to be based in their different historical tra- practitioners form a collaborative inquiry team
ditions. The action research traditions, which in which the roles of researcher and practitioner
include the work of psychologists and educators are somewhat blurred. People from both groups
such as John Dewey, Kurt Lewin, members of the bring to the inquiry process their expertise in the
Tavistock Institute for Human Relations, Paulo conceptualization of motivation, instructional
Freire, and Gregory Bateson, framed action research practice, and knowledge of the context. The team
as inquiry with the mission of “generating knowl- decides on the domain of inquiry and frames a
edge that is both valid and vital to the well-being of research question, such as “How can we motivate
individuals, communities, and for the promotion of urban middle school students in this context to
larger-scale democratic social change” (Brydon- engage deeply in the domain of world history?”
Miller, Greenwood, & Maguire, 2003, p. 11; Dick This question then leads to a conversation about
et al., 2009). Hence, action research is embedded in the meaning of motivation and deep engagement
an epistemology that views any scientific action as in world history in the particular context, with
expressing values and that explicitly pursues the the issues of equal opportunity for success of

184
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

both teachers and students taking an important taking place in the context. The multiple perspec-
role. The conversation would involve decision tives and the multiple forms of data contribute
making about the unit of analysis for the study. to the validity of the interpretations. Validity is
Depending on various contextual opportunities enhanced when different ways of knowing—for
and constraints, the team may decide to keep example, experiential, theoretical, and practical
the question at the initial unit of analysis of the (cf. Heron & Reason, 2001)—converge. When
school or focus the question on a more specific desired, people external to the inquiry group may
context (e.g., particular student group, specific be brought in to propose conceptualization of the
curricular unit, certain teaching practice). data for further validation. However, the funda-
2. Choosing data collection methods: The team would mental premise is that validity is tested in practice.
then decide on instruments and procedures for Therefore, the team would modify instructional
collecting open-ended, multiple-form, rich data and other types of practice on the basis of the new
on the focal contextual practice of world his- understandings. The team would also make deci-
tory instruction. The methods for data collec- sions about the instruments and procedures by
tion would include a mix of instruments such as which new data on the enactment of the new prac-
demographic characteristics of school, teachers, tices would be collected.
and students; quantitative and qualitative data on 5. Additional cycles: Another cycle of action and
students’ products and achievement; examina- research would ensue. Further iterations would
tion of curricular demands and materials; longi- take place until a decision was made to stop.
tudinal observations of lessons and of teachers’ 6. Summative analysis. A reflective analysis would
team meetings; focus groups with students and be written, highlighting the theoretical under-
teachers; interviews with administrators, teach- standings from the particular case about the
ers, parents, and students; and diaries or jour- motivation of urban middle school students for
nals of teachers. The selection of these methods world history, as well as about the organizational
would take into account contextual opportuni- transformation process in practice and how both
ties and constraints. led or did not lead to the promotion of teachers’
3. Enacting practice and collecting data: The data col- and students’ motivation.
lection tasks would be divided among the inquiry
team members as appropriate, and data would Notably, whereas the inquiry process just
be collected on the focal practices. This phase described depicts rather linear steps that clearly dis-
may involve smaller unit-of-analysis cycles of tinguish between phases of action and reflection, in
formative evaluation as a result of the heightened reality the phases may be more or less distinct,
awareness of the practitioner–researchers enact- depending on the approach of the inquiry team
ing the instructional practice and participating (Heron & Reason, 2001). Indeed, some researchers
in data collection (Heron & Reason, 2001). This have considered a more diffused process as better
heightened reflection on and transformation of compatible with complexity perspectives on human
practice is an integral and desirable element of systems and practice (Reason & Goodwin, 1999).
the process and should be documented through The action research approach has been criticized
the data collection procedures. as unscientific, as lacking methodological rigor,
4. Analysis and reflection: The data would then be and for being no different from consultation (Bask-
organized, coded, and interpreted collaboratively erville & Wood-Harper, 1996). For example, as with
by the inquiry team. All members suggest themes, design-based methods, action research was criticized
concepts, and processes that they perceive to be for failing to generate replicable and generalizable
useful for understanding patterns in the data. causal knowledge claims. As discussed earlier, these
Multiple data sources would be used and triangu- criteria for social science are incompatible with
lated to arrive at rich, multilevel, and sophisticated emerging understandings of the nature of human
understandings of the motivational processes systems and action. Nevertheless, because science is

185
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

concerned with the development of valid knowledge practice-relevant research methods that involve
that has relevance beyond the research context, practitioners as equal-status collaborators in the
action researchers have developed criteria for such investigations of their own practice. This scientific
transfer that are more compatible with the epistemo- approach is better suited to generating theoretical
logical assumptions of the approach. For example, knowledge that is practice relevant because it
Checkland and Holwell (1998) suggested that action encourages researchers to adopt an “insider” per-
researchers adopt a “recoverable” criterion: “enact a spective through their collaborative dialogue with
process based on a declared-in-advance methodol- the agents of action in the context. Moreover, it
ogy (encompassing a particular framework of ideas) encourages practitioners to adopt an “outsider”
in such a way that the process is recoverable by any- perspective on their educational processes, thus
one interested in subjecting the research to critical facilitating systematic reflection and exploration
scrutiny” (p. 18). Schön and Rein (1994) argued and contributing to professional identity develop-
that generalizability in action research is similar to ment and improvement of educational practice
people’s application of past experiences to new situ- (Zeichner, 2003; cf. Flum & Kaplan, 2006; Turner &
ations. They termed this generalizability reflective Patrick, 2009).
transfer: “the process by which patterns detected in
one situation are carried over as projective models
CONCLUSION
to other situations to generate new causal infer-
ences” (p. 204; in Friedman & Rogers, 2009). Fried- There is a growing perception that current motiva-
man and Rogers (2009) noted that reflective transfer tional theories—indeed, educational psychology
“is not simply seeing the new situation in terms of theories more generally—are failing to effect a
the old one, but using the comparison to become meaningful change in educational settings (Berliner,
more sharply aware of the key differences that need 2006; McCaslin & Lavigne, 2010; Turner, 2010;
to be addressed” (p. 45). Urdan & Turner, 2005). In this chapter, we pro-
However, some action researchers have acknowl- posed that the currently prevalent epistemological
edged the need to develop more explicit procedures assumptions that guide the theoretical conceptual-
for theoretical development (e.g., Dick et al., 2009; ization of motivational phenomena, the procedures
Friedman & Rogers, 2009) and methodological to investigate them, and the recommendations for
rigor (e.g., Baskerville & Wood-Harper, 1996). The applying theoretical understandings to educational
tensions of the dual focus on the transformation of practice may be incompatible with theoretical devel-
practice and maintaining reflective distance for theo- opments, methodological approaches, and practical
retical development and on the need to proceed and moral considerations that are relevant for the
quickly with practical changes versus the systematic complex, nonlinear, and unpredictable nature of
application of research methods stand at the founda- human phenomena. Similar challenges have been
tion of this research approach and need to be explic- posed in other psychological domains (e.g., Cron-
itly acknowledged and negotiated (Rapoport, 1970). bach, 1982; Fox, Prilleltensky, & Austin, 2009; Hill,
2006; Tuffin, 2005). These challenges call for the
Summary adoption of different assumptions and correspond-
The theoretical, methodological, practical, and ethi- ing guidelines for research and development of
cal challenges facing contemporary motivational motivational theory that are more compatible with
theory and research that aim to inform practice call characteristics of motivational phenomena in
for a change in researchers’ practice. We propose natural educational settings. We proposed three
that to develop practice-relevant motivation theory, such guidelines: asking practice-relevant research
researchers should pursue practice-relevant research questions, adopting a practice-relevant research
questions that address practitioners’ concerns, adopt orientation, and using practice-relevant research
a practice-relevant research orientation that values methodology. We also proposed the design-based
practitioners’ perspectives and constraints, and use and the participatory action research approaches as

186
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

examples of methodologies that simultaneously pro- Patrick, 2008; Urdan & Turner, 2005). We hope
mote practice-relevant motivational theory building that the growing number of critical voices will sup-
and its application to practice. port change in corresponding publishing patterns
One challenge to these recommendations is the and promotion criteria. This challenge notwith-
need to let go of a common conception about the standing, if the mission of educational psychology
role of motivational theory in affecting educational research is to develop knowledge beyond what is
practice. Specifically, the argument in this chapter known and to contribute to the betterment of
implies the need to set more modest goals for affect- human society, it is worthwhile to reflect on Kurt
ing educational practice than those promised by the- Lewin’s (1949) contention that
ories denoting deterministic cause–effect laws. It
to proceed beyond the limitations of a
seems, however, that such modest goals may be
given level of knowledge the researcher,
a more realistic approach to adopt. More than 3
as a rule, has to break down methodolog-
decades ago, McClelland (1978) argued that the fail-
ical taboos which condemn as “unscien-
ure of psychology to effect meaningful social changes
tific” or “illogical” the very methods or
and reform social institutions, including education,
concepts which later on prove to be basic
was because of the orientation of those leading the
for the next major progress. (p. 275)
reform efforts: “power-oriented people setting
impossible goals that they have attempted to reach That being said, the proposed practice-relevant
by powerful though inappropriate means” (p. 203). approach to motivational theory and research is only
Instead, McClelland argued, “Somehow . . . we have a beginning. Many issues central to this approach
got to learn to be patient and to proceed step by were touched on very briefly or not mentioned at all.
modest step to develop a behavioral technology that Issues that need to be addressed seriously include
will contribute significantly to human betterment” epistemological issues such as distinctions between
(p. 209). We believe that this advice is as relevant different types of motivational knowledge and their
today as it was then. status, construction, and importance (cf. Fenster-
A second challenge to the recommendations we macher, 1994); methodological issues concerning the
make for developing practice-relevant motivational great diversity of design-based and action research
theory and research is that they go against some methods and their contribution to different aspects of
strong traditions in academic politics. Design-based the scientific motivational field; and political issues
and action research studies may be harder to publish such as the status of the researchers who use these
because the criteria for their quality are different research methodologies and of the theoretical knowl-
from those commonly guiding the manuscript edge that is produced by this research. This chapter
review process. Moreover, studies with multiple provides an invitation for continuous dialogue about
cycles of design–enactment–analysis–redesign are these multifaceted theoretical, methodological, and
much more laborious and time consuming than the practical questions with the purpose of promoting
short-term experiment or the easily administered practice-relevant motivational theory and research.
survey. Scholars—particularly those in the early An initial step in this dialogue, perhaps, may be
stages of their career—may be reluctant to engage in the acknowledgment that the argument in this chap-
such intensive research, the academic fruits of ter calls on motivational researchers to engage in par-
which could only be published years later. To ticipatory action research on their own practice, from
encourage this type of research, a significant change the metatheoretical epistemological beliefs about the
needs to take place within the educational psychol- subject matter to the practical enactment of the pro-
ogy research community (cf. Berliner, 2006). This cedures of our trade. Arguably, the scientific project
chapter joins increasing criticism of the contempo- itself is a kind of communal action research—an
rary motivational research practices and their result- interdisciplinary global community that, for genera-
ing knowledge claims (Blumenfeld, 1992; Brophy, tions, has been addressing meaningful questions con-
2005; McCaslin, 2009; Turner, 2010; Turner & cerning the actual practice of scientific research as

187
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

well as the professional identity of its own practitio- integral inclusion of the participants’ voices and per-
ners through collaborative efforts between practitio- spectives in a flexible open-ended inquiry process.
ners (i.e., researchers) and theorists (i.e., These features allow situational and contextual char-
philosophers of science). acteristics to affect both the content and the process
of inquiry. Even more significant, these features of
the research approach call on the participants—re-
A FINAL POINT ON THE APPLICATION
searchers, educators, and potential participating stu-
OF MOTIVATION THEORY TO
dents alike—to engage in constructive exploration of
EDUCATIONAL SETTINGS
their own beliefs, values, roles, and practices. We
For this chapter, we were charged with the mission believe that developing such exploratory orientation
of reviewing the ways in which motivational theory and skills is becoming imperative for success in
can be successfully applied to educational settings. school, work, and society at large (Flum & Kaplan,
The conclusion of our review is that recommenda- 2006; Kaplan & Flum, 2009).
tions for the application of current motivational the-
ories to practice have theoretical, methodological,
practical, and ethical problems. We concluded fur- References
ther that to promote successful application, motiva- Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and stu-
tional research and theory building will need to dent motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology,
adopt a practice-relevant research approach. 84, 261–271. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.261
It might seem to the reader that this conclusion Argyris, C., Putnam, R., & Smith, D. (1985). Action sci-
ence: Concepts, methods, and skills for research and
amounts to an overall dismissal of contemporary intervention. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
motivational theories or that we consider it impossi-
Axelrod, R., & Cohen, I. D. (2000). Harnessing complex-
ble to apply these theories and recommendations to ity: Organizational implications of a scientific frontier.
educational settings. This is not the case. The fact is New York, NY: Basic Books.
that the approach to motivational theory building and Bandura, A. (1982). The psychology of chance encoun-
research proposed here—asking practice-relevant ters and life paths. American Psychologist, 37,
research questions, adopting practice-relevant 747–755. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.37.7.747
research orientations, and using practice-relevant Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control.
research methodology—constitutes, itself, an apt New York, NY: Freeman.
application of contemporary motivational theory to Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement
educational settings because it actually incorporates goals and optimal motivation: Testing multiple goal
models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
the recommendations summarized so succinctly by 80, 706–722. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.706
Urdan and Turner (2005) and listed earlier in this Baskerville, R. L., & Wood-Harper, A. T. (1996). A criti-
chapter. This research approach involves guided cal perspective on action research as a method for
engagement in collaborative learning of meaningful information systems research. Journal of Information
issues that promotes the relevance of the topic to the Technology, 11, 235–246. doi:10.1080/0268396
96345289
participants, supports their autonomy and control,
focuses them on the purpose of learning from mis- Bell, P. (2004). On the theoretical breadth of design-
based research in education. Educational Psychologist,
takes, and provides methodical feedback that empha- 39, 243–253. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3904_6
sizes attributions to effort and strategies about Berliner, D. C. (2006). Educational psychology: Searching
progress toward proximal (and distal) goals of learn- for essence throughout a century of influence. In
ing and growth. Despite the limitations of contempo- P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of
rary theoretical recommendations for the application educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 3–27). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
of motivational theory to practice, they do have
merit. More important, however, the research Blumenfeld, P. C. (1992). Classroom learning and
motivation: Clarifying and expanding goal theory.
approach that we propose goes beyond the recom- Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 272–281.
mendations to address their limitations through the doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.272

188
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

Brophy, J. E. (2004). Motivating students to learn Deci, E. L. (2009). Large-scale school reform as viewed
(2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. from the self-determination theory perspective.
Brophy, J. E. (2005). Goal theorists should move on from Theory and Research in Education, 7, 244–252.
performance goals. Educational Psychologist, 40, doi:10.1177/1477878509104329
167–176. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4003_3 Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999a). A
Brown, A. L. (1992). Design experiments: Theoretical meta-analytic review of experiments examin-
and methodological challenges in creating com- ing the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic
plex interventions in classroom settings. Journal motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.
of the Learning Sciences, 2, 141–178. doi:10.1207/ doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627
s15327809jls0202_2 Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999b). The
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: undermining effect is a reality after all—Extrinsic
Harvard University Press. rewards, task interest, and self-determination:
Reply to Eisenberger, Pierce, and Cameron
Brydon-Miller, M., Greenwood, D., & Maguire, P. (2003). (1999) and Lepper, Henderlong, and Gingras
Why action research? Action Research, 1, 9–28. (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125, 692–700.
doi:10.1177/14767503030011002 doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.692
Cameron, J. (2001). Negative effects of reward on intrin- Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Extrinsic
sic motivation—A limited phenomenon: Comment rewards and intrinsic motivation in education:
on Deci, Koestner, and Ryan (2001). Review of Reconsidered once again. Review of Educational
Educational Research, 71, 29–42. doi:10.3102/ Research, 71, 1–27. doi:10.3102/00346543071001001
00346543071001029
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
Cassell, C., & Johnson, P. (2006). Action research: self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY:
Explaining the diversity. Human Relations, 59, Plenum Press.
783–814. doi:10.1177/0018726706067080
Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., & Koestner, R. (2001). The per-
Chandler, D., & Torbert, B. (2003). Transforming vasive negative effects of reward on intrinsic motiva-
inquiry and action: Interweaving 27 flavors of action tion: Response to Cameron. Review of Educational
research. Action Research, 1, 133–152. doi:10.1177/ Research, 71, 43–51. doi:10.3102/
14767503030012002 00346543071001043
Checkland, P., & Holwell, S. (1998). Action research: Delpit, L. (1988). The silenced dialogue: Power and peda-
Its nature and validity. Systemic Practice and Action gogy in educating other people’s children. Harvard
Research, 11, 9–21. doi:10.1023/A:1022908820784 Educational Review, 58, 280–298.
Cobb, P., Confrey, J., diSessa, A., Lehrer, R., & Delpit, L. (1995). Other people’s children: Cultural conflict
Schauble, L. (2003). Design experiments in educa- in the classroom. New York, NY: New Press.
tional research. Educational Researcher, 32, 9–13.
doi:10.3102/0013189X032001009 Design-Based Research Collective. (2003). Design-
based research: An emerging paradigm for edu-
Codding, R. S., & Poncy, B. C. (Eds.). (2010). cational inquiry. Educational Researcher, 32, 5–8.
Generalization of academic skills [Special issue]. doi:10.3102/0013189X032001005
Journal of Behavioral Education, 19(1).
Dick, B., Stringer, E., & Huxham, C. (2009). Theory
Collins, A. (1992). Toward a design science of education. in action research. Action Research, 7, 5–12.
In E. Scanlon & T. O’Shea (Eds.), New directions in doi:10.1177/1476750308099594
educational technology (pp. 15–22). Berlin, Germany:
Springer-Verlag. diSessa, A., & Cobb, P. (2004). Ontological innovation
and the role of theory in design experiments. Journal
Colsant, L. C. (1995). “Hey, man, why do we gotta
of the Learning Sciences, 13, 77–103. doi:10.1207/
take this. . .?” Learning to listen to students. In
s15327809jls1301_4
J. G. Nicholls & T. A. Thorkildsen (Eds.), Reasons
for learning: Expanding the conversation on student- Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2003). Achievement
teacher collaboration (pp. 62–89). New York, NY: goals and intrinsic motivation: Coherence, con-
Teachers College Press. cordance, and achievement orientation. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 378–385.
Corbett, H. D., & Wilson, R. L. (1995). Make a difference
doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00013-1
with, not for, students: A plea for researchers and
reformers. Educational Researcher, 24, 12–17. Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motiva-
tion, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA:
Cronbach, L. (1982). Prudent aspirations for social
Psychology Press.
inquiry. In L. Kruskal (Ed.), The future of the social
sciences (pp. 61–81). Chicago, IL: University of Eccles-Parsons, J., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B.,
Chicago Press. Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., & Midgley, C. (1983).

189
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In An agenda for change (pp. 7–8). New York, NY: New
J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement moti- Press.
vation (pp. 75–146). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures: Selected
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental essays. New York, NY: Basic Books.
effects of reward: Myth or reality? American
Psychologist, 51, 1153–1166. doi:10.1037/0003- Giroux, H. A. (1985). Critical pedagogy, cultural politics
066X.51.11.1153 and the discourse of experience. Journal of Education,
167, 22–41.
Eisenberger, R., Pierce, W. D., & Cameron, J. (1999).
Effects of reward on intrinsic motivation: Negative, Graham, S., & Weiner, B. (1996). Theories and prin-
neutral, and positive. Psychological Bulletin, 125, ciples of motivation. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee
677–691. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.677 (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology
(pp. 63–84). New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achieve-
ment goal construct. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck Guttman, L. (1968). A general nonmetric technique
(Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation for finding the smallest coordinate space for a con-
(pp. 52–72). New York, NY: Guilford Press. figuration of points. Psychometrika, 33, 469–506.
doi:10.1007/BF02290164
Elliot, A. J., & Moller, A. (2003). Performance-approach
goals: Good or bad forms of regulation? International Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot,
Journal of Educational Research, 39, 339–356. A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achieve-
doi:10.1016/j.ijer.2004.06.003 ment goal theory: Necessary and illuminating.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 638–645.
Ertmer, P. A., & Newby, T. J. (1993). Behaviorism, doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.3.638
cognitivism, constructivism: Comparing critical
features from an instructional design perspec- Heron, J., & Reason, P. (2001). The practice of co-oper-
tive. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 6, 50–72. ative inquiry: Research “with” rather than “on” peo-
doi:10.1111/j.1937-8327.1993.tb00605.x ple. In P. Reason & H. Bradbury (Eds.), Handbook
of action research: Participative inquiry and practice
Fantuzzo, J., & Atkins, M. (1992). Applied behavior (pp. 179–188). London, England: Sage.
analysis for educators: Teacher centered and class-
room based. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, Hickey, D. T. (2003). Engaged participation versus mar-
37–42. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-37 ginal non-participation: A stridently sociocultural
approach to achievement motivation. The Elementary
Fenstermacher, G. D. (1994). The knower and the
School Journal, 103, 401–429. doi:10.1086/499733
known: The nature of knowledge in research on
teaching. Review of Research in Education, 20, 3–56. Hickey, D. T., & McCaslin, M. (2001). A comparative
Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (2006). Dynamic devel- and sociocultural analysis of context and motivation.
opment of action, thought, and emotion. In R. M. In S. Volet & S. Järvelä (Eds.), Motivation in learn-
Lerner (Ed.) & W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of ing contexts: Theoretical and methodological implica-
Child Psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human tions (pp. 33–56). Amsterdam, the Netherlands:
development (6th ed., pp. 313–399). New York, NY: Pergamon.
Wiley. Hickey, D. T., & Zuiker, S. J. (2005). Engaged par-
Flum, H., & Kaplan, A. (2006). Exploratory orientation ticipation: A sociocultural model of motivation
as an educational goal. Educational Psychologist, 41, with implications for educational assessment.
99–110. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4102_3 Educational Assessment, 10, 277–305. doi:10.1207/
s15326977ea1003_7
Fox, D., Prilleltensky, I., & Austin, S. (2009). Critical
psychology: An introduction (2nd ed.). London, Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model
England: Sage. of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41,
111–127. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4102_4
Friedman, V. J. (2001). Action science: Creating communi-
ties of inquiry in communities of practice. In P. Reason Hill, D. B. (2006). Theory in applied social psychology:
& H. Bradbury (Eds.), Handbook of action research: Past mistakes and future hopes. Theory & Psychology,
Participative inquiry and practice (pp. 159–170). 16, 613–640. doi:10.1177/0959354306067440
London, England: Sage. Human-Vogel, S. (2008). Complexity in education: Does
Friedman, V. J., & Rogers, T. (2009). There is nothing so chaos and complexity require a different way of
theoretical as good action research. Action Research, learning and teaching? Educational Research Review,
7, 31–47. doi:10.1177/1476750308099596 3, 93–100.
Gates, H. L., Jr. (1995). Multiculturalism: A conversa- Jörg, T., Davis, B., & Nickmans, G. (2007). Towards a
tion among different voices. In D. Levine, R. Lowe, new, complexity science of learning and education.
B. Peterson, & R. Tenorio (Eds.), Rethinking schools: Educational Research Review, 2, 145–156.

190
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

Jörg, T., Davis, B., & Nickmans, G. (2008). About the Ludema, J. D., Cooperrider, D. L., & Barrett, F. J. (2001).
outdated Newtonian paradigm in education and Appreciative inquiry: The power of the uncondi-
complexity and a complexity science of learning: tional positive question. In P. Reason & H. Bradbury
How far are we from a paradigm shift? Educational (Eds.), Handbook of action research: Participative
Research Review, 3, 77–100. inquiry and practice (pp. 189–199). London,
Joseph, D. (2004). The practice of design-based research: England: Sage.
Uncovering the interplay between design, research, Maehr, M. L. (1984). Meaning and motivation: Toward
and the real-world context. Educational Psychologist, a theory of personal investment. In C. Ames & R.
39, 235–242. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3904_5 Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behav- (Vol. 1, pp. 115–144). New York, NY: Academic
ior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Press.

Kaplan, A. (2008). Achievement motivation. In Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school
E. Anderman & L. H. Anderman (Eds.), Psychology cultures. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
of classroom learning: An encyclopedia (Vol. 1, Maehr, M. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1980). Culture and
pp. 13–17). Detroit, MI: Macmillan. achievement motivation: A second look. In N.
Kaplan, A., & Flum, H. (Eds.). (2009). Motivation and Warren (Ed.), Studies on cross-cultural psychology
identity [Special issue]. Educational Psychologist, 44(2). (Vol. 2, pp. 221–267). New York, NY: Academic
Press.
Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (2002). Adolescents’
achievement goals: Situating motivation in socio- Maroulis, S., Guimerà, R., Petry, H., Stringer, M. J.,
cultural contexts. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Gomez, L. M., Amaral, L. A. N., & Wilensky, U.
Adolescence and education: Vol. 2. Academic motiva- (2010). Complex systems view of educational policy
tion of adolescents (pp. 125–167). Greenwich, CT: research. Science, 330, 38–39. doi:10.1126/sci-
Information Age. ence.1195153
Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (2007). The contribution McCaslin, M. (2009). Co-regulation of student motiva-
and prospects of goal orientation theory. Educational tion and emergent identity. Educational Psychologist,
Psychology Review, 19, 141–184. doi:10.1007/ 44, 137–146. doi:10.1080/00461520902832384
s10648-006-9012-5 McCaslin, M., & Lavigne, A. L. (2010). Social policy,
Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. J. (2002). Should childhood educational opportunity, and classroom practice: A
be a journey or a race? A response to Harackiewicz co-regulation approach to research on student moti-
et al. (2002). Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, vation and achievement. In T. Urdan & S. Karabenick
646–648. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.3.646 (Eds.), Advances in Motivation and Achievement:
Vol. 16B. The decade ahead: Applications and contexts
Kelly, A. E. (2003). Research as design. Educational of motivation and achievement (pp. 211–249). Bingley,
Researcher, 32, 3–4. doi:10.3102/0013189X032001003 England: Emerald Group. doi:10.1108/S0749-7423
Latour, B. (1987). Science in action. Cambridge, MA: (2010)000016B01
Harvard University Press. McClelland, D. C. (1978). Managing motivation to
Lee, E. (1995). Taking multicultural, anti-racist educa- expand human freedom. American Psychologist, 33,
tion seriously: An interview with Enid Lee. In 201–210. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.33.3.201
D. Levine, R. Lowe, B. Peterson, & R. Tenorio (Eds.), McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., &
Rethinking schools: An agenda for change (pp. 9–16). Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive.
New York, NY: New Press. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Lepper, M. R., Henderlong, J., & Gingras, I. (1999). doi:10.1037/11144-000
Understanding the effects of extrinsic rewards on McIntyre, A. (2008). Participatory action research.
intrinsic motivation—Uses and abuses of meta- Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
analysis: Comment on Deci, Koestner, and Ryan
(1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125, 669–676. McMurtry, A. (2008). Complexity theory 101 for edu-
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.669 cators: A fictional account of a graduate seminar.
McGill Journal of Education, 43, 265–282.
Lewin, K. (1949). Cassirer’s philosophy of science and
the social sciences. In P. A. Schilpp (Ed.), The phi- McMurtry, A., Bowsfield, S., Breckenbridge, A., Davis,
losophy of Ernst Cassirer (pp. 269–288). Evanston, B., Ibrahim-Didi, K., Kin, M., . . . Wells, K. (2004).
IL: Library of Living Philosophers. Complexity in education. Retrieved from http://www.
complexityandeducation.ualberta.ca/glossary.htm
Lewis, M. D. (2000). The promise of dynamic systems
approaches for an integrated account of human Merton, R. K., & Barber, E. (2004). The travels and
development. Child Development, 71, 36–43. adventures of serendipity. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00116 University Press.

191
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. J. (2001). Rapoport, R. N. (1970). Three dilemmas in action
Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for research. Human Relations, 23, 499–513.
whom, under what circumstances, and at what doi:10.1177/001872677002300601
cost? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77–86. Rasch, G. (1980). Probabilistic models for some intelligence
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.77 and attainment tests. Copenhagen, Denmark: Nielsen
Morowitz, H. J. (2002). The emergence of everything. & Lydiche. (Original work published 1960)
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Reason, P., & Bradbury, H. (2008). Introduction. In
Newman, B. M., & Newman, P. R. (2007). Dynamic P. Reason & H. Bradbury (Eds.), The handbook of
systems theory. In B. M. Newman & P. R. Newman action research: Participative inquiry and practice
(Eds.), Theories of human development (pp. 270–296). (2nd ed., pp. 1–10). London, England: Sage.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Reason, P., & Goodwin, B. (1999). Toward a science
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and demo- of quality in organizations: Lessons from com-
cratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University plexity theory and postmodern biology. Concepts
Press. and Transformation, 4, 281–317. doi:10.1075/
cat.4.3.04rea
Nicholls, J. G., & Hazzard, S. P. (1995). Students as col-
laborators in curriculum construction: Science and Reeve, J. (2004). Self-determination theory applied to
tolerance. In J. Nicholls & T. Thorkildsen (Eds.), educational settings. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan
Reasons for learning: Expanding the conversation on (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research
student-teacher collaboration (pp.114–136). New (pp. 183–203). Rochester, NY: University of
York, NY: Teachers College Press. Rochester Press.
O’Donnell, A. M. (2004). A commentary on design Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling
research. Educational Psychologist, 39, 255–260. motivating style toward students and how they can
doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3904_7 become more autonomy supportive. Educational
Psychologist, 44, 159–175. doi:10.1080/
Osberg, D. (2005). Redescribing “education” in com- 00461520903028990
plex terms. Complicity: An International Journal of
Complexity and Education, 2, 81–83. Rowland, G. (2007). The challenge of new science.
Performance Improvement Quarterly, 20, 9–20.
Osberg, D. (Ed.). (2009). “Enlarging the space of the
possible” around what it means to educate and be Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination the-
educated [Special issue]. Complicity: An International ory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social
Journal of Complexity and Education, 6(1). development, and well-being. American Psychologist,
55, 68–78. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68
Patrick, H., Anderman, L. H., Ryan, A. M., Edelin, K.
C., & Midgley, C. (2001). Teachers’ communica- Salomon, G. (1984). Television is “easy” and print is
tion of goal orientations in four fifth-grade class- “tough”: The differential investment of mental effort
rooms. The Elementary School Journal, 102, 35–58. in learning as a function of perceptions and attribu-
doi:10.1086/499692 tions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 647–658.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.76.4.647
Patrick, H., & Middleton, M. (2002). Turning the kalei-
doscope: What we see when self-regulated learn- Sandoval, W. A. (2004). Developing learning theory
ing is viewed with a qualitative lens. Educational by refining conjectures embodied in educational
Psychologist, 37, 27–39. designs. Educational Psychologist, 39, 213–223.
doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3904_3
Perry, R. P., Hechter, F. J., Menec, V. H., & Weinberg, L.
E. (1993). Enhancing achievement motivation and Sandoval, W. A., & Bell, P. (Eds.). (2004). Design-based
performance in college students: An attributional research methods for studying learning in context
retraining perspective. Research in Higher Education, [Special issue]. Educational Psychologist, 39(4).
34, 687–723. doi:10.1007/BF00992156 doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3904_1
Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspec- Sarason, S. B. (1990). The predictable failure of educational
tive on the role of student motivation in learning and reform. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.
teaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, Schoenfeld, A. H. (1999). Looking toward the twenty-first
95, 667–686. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.667 century: Challenges of educational theory and prac-
Prigogine, I., & Stengers, I. (1984). Order out of chaos: tice. Educational Researcher, 28, 4–14.
Man’s new dialogue with nature. Toronto, Ontario, Schön, D. A., & Rein, M. (1994). Frame reflection: Toward
Canada: Bantam Books. the resolution of intractable policy controversies. New
Radford, M. (2008). Prediction, control and the chal- York, NY: Basic Books.
lenge to complexity. Oxford Review of Education, 34, Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2005). An implicit
505–520. doi:10.1080/03054980701772636 motive perspective on competence. In A. Elliot & C.

192
Motivation Theory in Educational Practice

S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motiva- Thompson, T. Y., & Zeiler, M. D. (Eds.). (1986). Analysis
tion (pp. 31–51). New York, NY: Guilford Press. and integration of behavioral units. Hillsdale, NJ:
Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2004). Self-efficacy in edu- Erlbaum.
cation revisited: Empirical and applied evidence. In Thorkildsen, T. A., & Nicholls, J. G. (1991). Students’
D. M. McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Big theories critiques as motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26,
revisited (pp. 115–138). Greenwich, CT: Information 347–368. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2603&4_7
Age.
Torbert, W. R. (2001). The practice of action inquiry.
Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008). In P. Reason & H. Bradbury (Eds.), Handbook of
Motivation in education: Theory, research, and appli- action research: Participative inquiry and practice
cations (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson (pp. 250–260). London, England: Sage.
Education.
Trendler, G. (2009). Measurement theory, psychology
Shavelson, R. J., Phillips, D. C., Towne, L., & Feuer, and the revolution that cannot happen. Theory &
M. J. (2003). On the science of education design Psychology, 19, 579–599. doi:10.1177/0959354
studies. Educational Researcher, 32, 25–28. 309341926
doi:10.3102/0013189X032001025
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in
Skaalvik, E. M., & Skaalvik, S. (2002). Internal and exter- differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96,
nal frames of reference for academic self-concept. 506–520. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506
Educational Psychologist, 37, 233–244. doi:10.1207/
S15326985EP3704_3 Tuffin, K. (2005). Understanding critical social psychology.
London, England: Sage.
Skinner, B. F. (1935). The generic nature of the con-
cepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Turner, J. C. (2001). Using context to enrich and challenge
Psychology, 12, 40–65. doi:10.1080/00221309.1935. motivation theory. In S. Volet & S. Jarvela (Eds.),
9920087 Motivation in learning contexts: Theoretical advances and
methodological implications (pp. 85–104). Amsterdam,
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & the Netherlands: Pergamon.
Wellborn, J. G. (2009). Engagement as an organi-
zational construct in the dynamics of motivational Turner, J. C. (2010). Unfinished business: Putting moti-
development. In K. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), vation theory to the “classroom test.” In T. Urdan
Handbook of motivation in school (pp. 223–245). & S. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in Motivation
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. and Achievement: Vol. 16B. The decade ahead:
Applications and context of motivation and achievement
Stacey, R. D. (2001). Complex responsive processes in orga- (pp. 109–138). Bingley, England: Emerald Group.
nizations: Learning and knowledge creation. London, doi:10.1108/S0749-7423(2010)000016B007
England: Routledge.
Turner, J. C., & Meyer, D. K. (2000). Studying and
Stetsenko, A. (2008). From relational ontology to trans-
understanding the instructional contexts of
formative activist stance on development and learn-
classrooms: Using our past to forge our future.
ing: Expanding Vygotsky’s (CHAT) project. Cultural
Educational Psychologist, 35, 69–85. doi:10.1207/
Studies of Science Education, 3, 471–491. doi:10.1007/
S15326985EP3502_2
s11422-008-9111-3
Stipek, D. J. (1998). Motivation to learn: From theory to Turner, J. C., Midgley, C., Meyer, D. K., Gheen, M.,
practice. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Anderman, E. M., Kang, Y., & Patrick, H. (2002).
The classroom environment and students’ reports of
Stokes, D. E. (1997). Pasteur’s quadrant: Basic science and avoidance strategies in mathematics: A multimethod
technical innovation. Washington, DC: Brookings. study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 88–106.
Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1997). A cognitive theory of Turner, J. C., & Patrick, H. (2008). How does motivation
cultural meaning. Cambridge, England: Cambridge develop and how does it change? Reframing motiva-
University Press. tion research. Educational Psychologist, 43, 119–131.
Strogatz, S. (2003). Sync: The emerging science of sponta- doi:10.1080/00461520802178441
neous order. New York, NY: Hyperion. Turner, J. C., & Patrick, H. (2009). The cultural situat-
Tabak, I. (2004). Reconstructing context: Negotiating the edness of motivation. In A. Kaplan, S. Karabenick,
tension between exogenous and endogenous educa- & L. De Groot (Eds.), Culture, self and motivation:
tional design. Educational Psychologist, 39, 225–233. Essays in honor of Martin L. Maehr (pp. 243–265).
doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3904_4 Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Teo, T. (2009). Philosophical concerns in critical psy- Urdan, T. C., Kneisel, L., & Mason, V. (1999). The effect
chology. In D. Fox, I. Prilleltensky, & S. Austin of particular instructional practices on student moti-
(Eds.), Critical psychology: An introduction (2nd ed., vation: An exploration of teachers’ and students’ per-
pp. 36–53). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. ceptions. In T. Urdan (Ed.), Advances in motivation

193
Kaplan, Katz, and Flum

and achievement (Vol. 11, pp. 123–158). Stamford, Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (Eds.). (2007). Motivation
CT: JAI Press. at school: Interventions that work [Special issue].
Educational Psychologist, 42(4).
Urdan, T., & Turner, J. C. (2005). Competence motiva-
tion in the classroom. In A. Elliot & C. S. Dweck Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook
(Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation of motivation in school. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 297–317). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Whyte, W. F. (Ed.). (1991). Participatory action research.
Urry, J. (2005). The complexity turn. Theory, Culture & Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Society, 22, 1–14. doi:10.1177/0263276405057188 Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (1992). The development of
Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy achievement task values: A theoretical analysis.
in school: Critical review of the literature and future Developmental Review, 12, 265–310. doi:10.1016/
directions. Review of Educational Research, 78, 0273-2297(92)90011-P
751–796. doi:10.3102/0034654308321456 Wigfield, A., & Tonks, S. (2002). Adolescents’ expectan-
Vallacher, R. R., & Nowak, A. (1997). The emergence of cies for success and achievement task values during
dynamical social psychology. Psychological Inquiry, the middle and high school years. In F. Pajares &
8, 73–99. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0802_1 T. Urdan (Eds.), Academic motivation of adolescence
(pp. 53–82). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Vargas, E. A., & Vargas, J. S. (1991). Programmed
instruction: What it is and how to do it. Journal Wilson, T. D., Damiani, M., & Shelton, N. (2002).
of Behavioral Education, 1, 235–251. doi:10.1007/ Improving the academic performance of college
BF00957006 students with brief attributional interventions. In
J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement:
Waldrop, M. M. (1992). Complexity: The emerging sci- Impact of psychological factors on education (pp. 89–108).
ence at the edge of order and chaos. New York, NY: San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-
Touchstone. 012064455-1/50008-7
Weick, K. E. (2005). Managing the unexpected: Complexity Witherington, D. C. (2007). The dynamic systems
as distributed sensemaking. In R. R. McDaniel, approach as a metatheory for developmental
Jr., & D. J. Driebe (Eds.), Uncertainty and surprise psychology. Human Development, 50, 127–153.
in complex systems: Questions on working with the doi:10.1159/000100943
unexpected (pp. 51–65). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
doi:10.1007/10948637_5 Yin, R. K. (2008). Case study research: Design and meth-
ods. London, England: Sage.
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation
Zeichner, K. M. (2001). Education action research. In
and emotion. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
P. Reason & H. Bradbury (Eds.), Handbook of action
Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation: Metaphors, theory, research: Participative inquiry and practice (pp. 273–283).
and research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. London, England: Sage.
Weiner, B. (2010). The development of an attribution- Zeichner, K. M. (2003). Teacher research as profes-
based theory of motivation: A history of ideas. sional development for P–12 educators in the
Educational Psychologist, 45, 28–36. doi:10.1080/ USA. Educational Action Research, 11, 301–326.
00461520903433596 doi:10.1080/09650790300200211

194

View publication stats

Anda mungkin juga menyukai