Anda di halaman 1dari 8

Psychological Inquiry, 26: 130–137, 2015

Copyright Ó Taylor & Francis Group, LLC


ISSN: 1047-840X print / 1532-7965 online
DOI: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.989751

The Extended Process Model of Emotion Regulation:


Elaborations, Applications, and Future Directions
James J. Gross
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California

Three observations motivated the target article pulling in the same direction. At other times, valua-
(Gross, this issue). The first is that emotion regulation tion systems pull in different directions, and their
processes play a crucial role in health and illness, and divergent outputs compete with one another.
thus warrant deeper understanding. The second is that Emotion regulation refers to a particular type of
a growing appreciation of the importance of emotion interaction between valuation systems, one that
regulation processes has led to an extraordinary occurs when one valuation system (which I refer to as
increase in research in this area. The third is that there a “second-level” valuation system) takes another val-
is an urgent need for a process-oriented framework to uation system (which I refer to as a “first-level” valu-
integrate and guide this research, one that would ation system, i.e., one that is generating emotion) as
extend the process model of emotion regulation, first its input, evaluates it either negatively or positively,
proposed nearly two decades ago (Gross, 1998b). and activates action impulses that are intended to
I was therefore delighted that the dozen-plus dis- modify the activity of the first-level valuation system.
tinguished scholars who provided commentaries on Emotion regulation thus may be defined by the acti-
the target article not only endorsed these three moti- vation of a goal to modify an unfolding emotional
vating observations but also saw merit in the response (Gross, Sheppes, & Urry, 2011a).
extended process model (EPM) as a framework for According to the EPM, the second-level valuation
integrating existing research and for suggesting direc- system can modify the first-level valuation system in
tions for future research. In light of this agreeable five ways. These include (a) taking steps to influence
consensus, I use this response to (a) offer a precis of which situation one will be exposed to (situation
the EPM, (b) elaborate upon five core features of the selection), (b) changing one or more relevant aspects
EPM, (c) highlight some of the many applications of of the situation (situation modification), (c) influenc-
the EPM suggested by the commentators, and (d) dis- ing which portions of the situation are perceived
cuss theoretical and empirical challenges and future (attentional deployment), (d) altering the way the
directions. situation is cognitively represented (cognitive
change), and (e) directly modifying emotion-related
actions (response modulation). These represent the
Precis of the EPM five families of emotion regulation processes that
were described by the original process model of emo-
The EPM holds that emotions—like other types of tion regulation (Gross, 1998b).
affect—result from the operations of valuation sys- The EPM extends the original process model by
tems. Drawing upon cybernetic/control systems the- distinguishing three stages of the emotion regulation
ory, a valuation system may be represented by cycle (for other related approaches, see Bonanno &
distinguishing among states of the world (“W”), per- Burton, 2013; Webb, Gallo, Miles, Gollwitzer, &
ceptions of those states (“P”), negative or positive Sheeran, 2012). These stages are (a) identification
valuations of these perceptions in light of a relevant (concerned with whether to regulate emotion), (b)
goal or target state (“V”), and actions taken to realize selection (concerned with what strategy to use to reg-
the goal or target state (“A”). Some of these actions ulate emotion), and (c) implementation (concerned
are “mental” (e.g., attending to a stimulus); others are with implementing a particular tactic suited to the
“physical” (e.g., reaching for an object). present situation). In addition, the EPM describes the
As we go about our lives, many different valuation nature of the processing dynamics that emerge as
systems are typically active simultaneously. Each is the second-level valuation system iterates over time.
sensitive to different aspects of any given situation,
and each activates action impulses relevant to its own
evaluation of that situation. The concurrent activation Elaborations of Key Features of the EPM
of multiple valuation systems frequently leads the
valuation systems to interact with one another. Some- Commentators drew attention to a number of fea-
times, valuation systems are mutually supportive, tures of the EPM. In so doing, they helpfully
130
AUTHOR’S REPLY

identified places the EPM might be further elabo- valuation, and (b) focuses attention on how emotions
rated. In the following sections, I briefly elaborate are regulated (by the higher level valuation system)
upon five key features of the EPM. rather than regulating (driving goal pursuit in the pri-
mary valuation system). There is thus a difference in
emphasis and focus between the two approaches, but
A Cybernetic/Control Systems Perspective
the basic valuation machinery postulated by the
In their penetrating commentary, Webb, Totter- EPM—namely, interacting hierarchically organized
dell, and Ibar (this issue) highlight several of the control loops—is drawn directly from classic cyber-
many sources of inspiration for the EPM. In particu- netic/control systems perspectives.
lar, Webb and colleagues note the continuity between
the EPM and cybernetic/control systems perspectives,
The Notion of a Valuation “System”
which posit that self-regulation is made possible by
negative feedback loops that decrease the discrepancy At the heart of the EPM is the notion of a valuation
between a current state and a reference value. I “system.” Two points should be made about valuation
completely agree with their assessment, and in the systems. First, although valuation systems share com-
target article I pointed to a number of cybernetic/con- mon features (four conceptually separable elements,
trol system precursors to the EPM (Carver & Scheier, denoted by W, P, V, and A), they also differ from one
1982, 2013; Magen & Gross, 2010; Miller, Galanter, another in important ways. Second, valuation systems
& Pribram, 1960; Powers, Clark, & McFarland, can be functionally coupled in such a way that several
1960a, 1960b; Rangel, Camerer, & Montague, 2008; interlocking valuation subsystems constitute a single
Wiener, 1948). I see this intellectual continuity as a “valuation system.”
core strength of the EPM, building as it does on the In the target article, the second-level valuation sys-
powerful notion that motivated behavior in many tem was described as consisting of three functionally
domains (including emotion regulation) relies upon coupled valuation subsystems (one focused on identi-
successive approximations of desired target states. fication, one on selection, and another on implemen-
The link between cybernetic/control systems per- tation), each of which is in turn composed of
spectives and emotion regulation has not always been common W-PVA elements. What I did not emphasize
evident to me. In fact, I actually went so far as to say in the target article is that the first-level valuation sys-
that “Carver and Scheier’s (1990) conception of emo- tem—which generates emotion—can also be con-
tion provides relatively inhospitable ground for the ceived of as involving functionally coupled valuation
study of emotion regulation” (Gross, 1998b, p. 272). subsystems. It is beyond the scope of this reply to
This judgment flowed from the fact that Carver and fully develop the implications of this idea, but in
Scheier’s analysis features two interacting feedback broad strokes, this conception makes clear contact
loops, the first of which is concerned with achieving with appraisal theories (e.g., Lazarus, 1991). In each
a (nonemotional) goal and the second of which is case, the first valuation subsystem “calls” the next
concerned with evaluating the rate of progress toward subsystem in line when its action output is a goal
that goal. Faster than expected progress leads to posi- representation that activates that valuation system,
tive emotion, whereas slower than expected progress which in turn may activate the next subsystem in line,
leads to negative emotion. Emotion is thus a by-prod- and so on.
uct of goal pursuit, and emotion regulation is nowhere The EPM emphasizes commonalities across valua-
considered. tion systems. However, as Parkinson (this issue) notes
From the vantage point of the EPM, however, in his excellent commentary, there are also important
cybernetic/control systems perspectives are fertile differences among valuation systems. In particular,
rather than inhospitable ground for work on emotion different valuation systems “care” about different
regulation. The EPM begins its analysis with a valua- types of inputs, lead to different kinds of outputs, and
tion system that is giving rise to emotion, and the operate on different time scales. Ochsner and Gross
EPM is indifferent as to what the source of the emo- (2014) suggested that three overlapping classes of
tion is (e.g., better than expected or worse than valuation systems may be distinguished. Core valua-
expected progress toward some goal, a threat to one’s tion systems respond to a specific set of highly delin-
safety, an exciting opportunity, etc.). The EPM is eated inputs (e.g., a sudden drop in blood pressure;
centrally concerned with the interaction between this the smell of attractive food). These are highly con-
first-order valuation system and one or more second- served systems the valuations of which show modest
order valuation systems that take this first-order valu- malleability due to learning and the action outputs of
ation system as input, and then seek to modify the which are relatively stereotyped, context indepen-
operation of the first-order valuation system. In this dent, and inflexible (e.g., fainting when a sudden
sense, from the vantage point of Carver and Scheier’s blood pressure drop is detected; salivating when
analysis, the EPM (a) adds an additional level of attractive food is detected). Contextual valuation
131
GROSS

systems, by contrast, take contextual information “response-focused” emotion regulation (Gross,


(e.g., situational factors) into account when valuing 1998a) maps onto the EPM. This is an excellent
inputs and potential responses. A wider range of question. Although this was not emphasized in the
inputs is relevant to these systems, which can take as target article, I believe this distinction applies to
inputs core valuation systems. Contextual valuation the EPM in the sense that within any given cycle
systems also have a greater degree of context sensi- of the first-order valuation system that is generating
tivity, flexibility, and openness to learning than the emotion, a person may seek to alter relatively early
core valuation systems, as well as a wider range of steps in the valuation cycle (antecedent-focused
action outputs. Conceptual valuation systems are regulation) or may seek to alter relatively later
even more flexible than the other two classes of sys- steps in the valuation cycle (response-focused regu-
tems. Conceptual valuation systems take both propo- lation). This difference in which step in the valua-
sitional and nonpropositional input; this can include tion cycle is being targeted for regulation has very
other conceptual, core, and contextual valuation sys- real consequences (Gross, this issue), and it is my
tems. The valuations of these inputs are highly influ- hope that the more differentiated framework pro-
enced by prior experience and contextual variables. vided by the EPM will allow researchers to move
As might be expected, responses are highly flexible beyond the relatively coarse distinction between
and situationally determined. antecedent- and response-focused regulation.

Temporal Dynamics Context Sensitivity


The EPM highlights the temporally extended According to the EPM, emotion regulation is
nature of emotion regulation. In particular, it specifies defined by interactions among valuation systems over
how an initial perception of an emotion can activate time. These interacting valuation systems are highly
processes that identify whether regulation is required. sensitive to the evolving context within which they
If regulation is required, other processes are launched are operating. More specifically, the EPM conceives
to select a regulation strategy. Then, still other pro- of emotion regulation in terms of interactions that
cesses are activated to implement a particular emo- people have with each other and with other aspects of
tion regulation tactic. If this tactic appears to be a dynamically unfolding environment. In this sense,
successfully changing relevant features of the first- the EPM offers a situated cognition perspective on
level valuation system, the tactic will continue to be emotion regulation: What is emphasized is the way
used (emotion regulation maintenance) until the that actions taken in particular contexts serve contex-
goal is no longer active, either because the goal has tually defined goals. One implication is that context
been achieved or because another goal has displaced matters and that it is simply not possible to specify in
it. If the tactic is not successful, a new strategy/tactic a context-independent way whether a particular emo-
may be selected (emotion regulation switching), or tion regulation strategy is helpful or harmful.
the entire emotion regulation process may be aban- Given the EPM’s emphasis on rapidly evolving
doned (emotion regulation stopping). responses to dynamic situations, I was delighted that
In their valuable commentary, Kuppens and Ver- Koole and Veenstra (this issue) speak in strong sup-
duyn (this issue) bring to bear an emotion dynamics port of a situated cognition perspective, one that high-
perspective, which considers how emotions change lights the important role played by local affordances
over time (Kuppens, Stouten, & Mesquita, 2009). As in the generation of both emotion and emotion regula-
they made clear, emotion dynamics can be studied by tion. I was mystified, however, to see that Koole and
examining either global trajectories (the ebb and flow Veenstra not only fail to appreciate that the EPM
of emotion across situations) or emotional episodes takes such a perspective but instead mischaracterize
(patterns of response within a given situation). One the EPM as being concerned with static representa-
important insight that this perspective offers is that it tions located solely inside people’s heads. I’m not
may be possible to make robust inferences regarding sure what else I could have done in the target article
underlying emotion regulation processes based on to highlight the importance of the context within
observed emotion dynamics in both global trajecto- which regulation takes place (the W in each of the fig-
ries (by examining variability, covariation, inertia, ures), as well as the dynamic nature of emotion regu-
and cross-lags) and in emotional episodes (by exam- lation (highlighted via the spiral figures throughout
ining both the duration and the shape of the emotional the target article), but I’m glad for this opportunity to
response). reaffirm the crucial role played in the EPM by contex-
Kross (this issue) makes a number of important tual factors (such as other people).
observations about emotion regulation and, among Other commentators more fully appreciated the
other things, asks how the distinction drawn between EPM’s emphasis on contextual factors. For example,
“antecedent-focused” emotion regulation and one of the main themes in the incisive commentary
132
AUTHOR’S REPLY

by Aldao and Christensen (this issue) is the role of original (and current) position, namely that emotion
context in emotion regulation. One of the key points regulation should be viewed as including both intrin-
in this commentary is that the adaptiveness of any sic and extrinsic forms (Gross & Thompson, 2007;
motivated behavior—including emotion regulation— Gross, 2013b). However, more work needs to be
is dependent on context (Aldao, 2013). Similarly, in done—both theoretically and empirically—to figure
Schmader and Mendes’s (this issue) helpful commen- out how to best apply the EPM to extrinsic emotion
tary, they note with approval the EPM’s strong regulation, and to determine similarities and differen-
emphasis on how people are embedded in social ces between intrinsic and extrinsic regulation.
context.

Applications of the Extended Process Model


Intrinsic Versus Extrinsic Regulation
The EPM distinguishes two broad types of emo- In the target article, I noted that emotion regulation
tion regulation: intrinsic (which targets one or more is an active topic of investigation in all major subar-
aspects of emotion in the person who is doing the reg- eas within psychology (biological, cognitive, devel-
ulating) and extrinsic (which targets one or more opmental, social, industrial-organizational,
aspects of emotion in someone other than the person personality, clinical, health), as well as in other
who is doing the regulating). Whether people are related fields (including anthropology, business, eco-
engaged in intrinsic emotion regulation, extrinsic nomics, education, law, medicine, political science,
emotion regulation, or both, they seek to alter the and sociology). One compelling feature of a number
intensity, duration, and/or quality of the emotion. of the commentaries was that they explored applica-
Often, people down-regulate negative emotions and tions of the EPM in diverse areas within psychology.
up-regulate positive emotions (Gross, Richards, & In the following sections, I highlight some of these
John, 2006). On occasion, however, people try to up- applications.
regulate negative emotions and down-regulate posi-
tive emotions (Tamir, 2009). As with other goal-
Cognitive Applications
directed behavior, emotion regulation goals may be
either explicit or implicit (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee- In the cognitive domain, Saunders, Milyavskaya,
Chai, Barndollar, & Troetschel, 2001). and Inzlicht (this issue) argue that the implementa-
One intriguing aspect of Webb et al.’s (this issue) tion of cognitive control can be seen as a type of
commentary is their emphasis on the potential for emotion regulation. This is because goal conflict
complexity in extrinsic emotion regulation. In partic- produces negative affect, and cognitive control can
ular, they highlight the fact that extrinsic emotion be viewed as a means of reducing this negative
regulation involves interactions among valuation sys- affect. This view is predicated on the assumption
tems in each of two (or more) people, and they sug- that people seek “cognitive comfort” defined as “a
gest the value of computer simulations in subjectively pleasant state free of the aversive
understanding these complex within- and between- experience of goal conflict” (p. 109). This commen-
person dynamics. In Parkinson’s (this issue) excellent tary suggests the interesting possibility that suc-
commentary, he also stresses the complexity of cessful emotion regulation may be associated with
extrinsic regulation, and directs attention to the fact lesser engagement of cognitive control (because the
that emotions themselves are valuation systems and negative affect that would usually prompt cognitive
therefore are associated with actions that can influ- control has been reduced or eliminated). More
ence others’ emotional responses. broadly, this analysis presents a thought-provoking
I completely concur with these thoughtful observa- challenge to conventional distinctions between
tions. Indeed, my concerns along these lines are in “cognitive” and “affective” domains.
part responsible for my evolving thoughts on whether
extrinsic emotion regulation really should be consid-
Developmental Applications
ered a form of emotion regulation. In early work, I
took the position I now do, namely, that intrinsic and Diaz and Eisenberg (this issue) focus part of their
extrinsic emotion regulation are both part of the interesting commentary on developmental applica-
larger category of emotion regulation (Gross & Lev- tions of the EPM. These authors agree that age is a
enson, 1993). Worrying about the heterogeneity of key moderator of emotion regulation and consider
motives, goals, and processes engaged in these two antecedents and consequences of regulation across
cases, I subsequently argued that emotion regulation the lifespan. In particular, Diaz and Eisenberg empha-
should be restricted to intrinsic regulation only size that one source of individual differences in emo-
(Gross, 1998b). Since then, I’ve become convinced tion regulation is parenting, which they argue affects
that the overlap in core mechanisms justifies my all three stages of the EPM (identification, selection,
133
GROSS

and implementation) via parents’ communications best conceptualized as “emotion regulation in the
about when and how to regulate emotions, as well as workplace” (p. 54). When a cashier or airline stew-
via the parents’ own emotion regulation practices. ardess is told to smile at customers and to avoid nega-
Much more remains to be learned, however, about tive emotion displays however unreasonable the
the match between parenting styles and children’s customer’s requests, these instructions seem to be a
temperament, as well as the most effective ways to clear call for emotion regulation. Grandey acknowl-
intervene to help enhance parents’ emotion regulation edges that there are differences in focal constructs,
skills. methods, and outcomes in the emotional labor and
In their thought-provoking commentary, Riediger emotion regulation literatures. Like Grandey, how-
and Luong (this issue) also consider developmental ever, I believe that the benefits of the conceptual
applications but focus on adolescence through later move she is advocating are overwhelming, and seem
adulthood. They argue for the importance of examin- likely to flow in both directions (Gross, 2013a). This
ing age differences both in which regulation strate- is because researchers interested in emotional labor
gies are used and in how effective these strategies stand to benefit from developments in the larger
are. They note just how variable emotions are across emotion regulation literature. At the same time,
the lifespan (e.g., more negative emotions during ado- researchers interested in general emotional regulation
lescence, more positive emotions during later adult- processes can see how their models (and interven-
hood) and suggest that emotion regulation skills and tions) apply to the real-world context of the workplace
motives may be one important contributor to these with its very real impact on health and well-being.
differences in emotional functioning across the
lifespan.
Clinical Applications
The target article describes how the EPM can be
Social Applications
used to analyze both normal- and pathological-range
Schmader and Mendes’s (this issue) interesting variation in emotion regulation. A more complete
commentary uses a case-study approach to apply the treatment of how the EPM might be used to analyze
EPM to social psychology. The first case study con- links between emotion regulation and psychopathol-
siders how interactions with outgroup members can ogy may be found in Sheppes, Suri, and Gross (in
lead to subjective feelings of anxiety as well as physi- press). Clinical applications were also nicely elabo-
ological threat responses that motivate emotion regu- rated in several of the commentaries.
lation attempts from both majority and minority Mennin and Fresco (this issue) consider clinical
group members. Unfortunately, these regulatory applications of the EPM, with a particular focus on
efforts can actually backfire, and this observation distress disorders. These are psychiatric disorders
highlights the need for more research on how to suc- that involve extended processing of negative affect.
cessfully manage the anxiety occasioned by cross- They include generalized anxiety disorder, major
group interactions. The second case study examines depressive disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder.
stereotype threat, which occurs when people fear con- Mennin and Fresco argue that the EPM provides
firming a negative group-based stereotype. As insights into these disorders by drawing attention to
Schmader and Mendes point out, one important the role of motivation (i.e., the interactions between
mechanism for the performance decrements observed reward and security valuation systems), self-referen-
in stereotype threat may be the use of relatively mal- tial processing (i.e., negative self-referential process-
adaptive forms of emotion regulation such as emo- ing), and contextual learning (i.e., how various types
tional suppression. This leads to the important of learning help “tune” relevant valuation systems).
question of how such feelings of threat might be suc- They also made the important broader point that an
cessfully managed. The third case study concerns the emotion regulation perspective may be particularly
emotion of shame, which often is a target of down- well suited to a research domain criteria perspective
regulation. However, the authors argue that shame (Insel et al., 2010) that showcases common risk fac-
may have adaptive qualities and note that a more tors and mechanisms that cut across traditional dis-
widespread recognition of these adaptive qualities tinctions among mental disorders.
might have the salutary effect of decreasing maladap- In their insightful commentary, Giuliani and Berk-
tive emotion regulation efforts. man (this issue) argue that craving is an affective
state and that the EPM is a useful framework for
examining the self-regulation of craving (e.g., for
Industrial/Organizational Applications
drugs or food). One way this framework is useful is
Grandey (this issue) provides a compelling argu- that it highlights mechanisms that are associated with
ment that emotional labor—which for decades has regulating craving. These include situation selection
been seen as its own separate literature—is actually (e.g., decreasing exposure to cues that will provoke
134
AUTHOR’S REPLY

craving), situation modification (e.g., moving away deployment might have in common (p. 70). I agree,
from a desirable food), attentional deployment (e.g., and would take this argument one step further, sug-
directing attention away from food), cognitive change gesting that the EPM is a framework that will help
(e.g., thinking about benefits of eating a healthy researchers expose mechanistic similarities and dif-
meal), and response modulation (e.g., inhibiting the ferences not only among different emotion regulation
urge to eat something unhealthy). A second way this strategies but also among other types of self- and
framework is useful is that it suggests novel interven- other-regulation processes.
tions to increase self-regulation of craving at each of A third commentary drew attention both to cases
the stages in the EPM (identification, selection, where the EPM seemed to exclude from consider-
implementation). ation things it should include, and cases where the
EPM seemed to include things it might better
exclude. Parkinson (this issue) gives as an example of
Challenges and Future Directions the former case classic work by Lazarus and col-
leagues in which participants were instructed to view
In the target article, I outlined five promising a movie as staged rather than real (Lazarus, Opton,
directions for future research on emotion regulation. Nomikos, & Rankin, 1965), arguing that these partici-
Here, I consider some of the broader theoretical and pants were not engaging in emotion regulation as I’ve
empirical challenges (and opportunities) identified by defined it because no goal to regulate the emotion
the commentators. was activated. I am sympathetic to Parkinson’s con-
cerns about boundary drawing but believe that a goal
can be activated “from the outside” (by the experi-
Theoretical Challenges
menter, in this case) and that this goal may be opera-
One theoretical challenge is one of the most basic, tive even when the person who has been led to
namely, defining how emotion regulation is related to activate an emotion regulation goal may not be aware
other constructs (e.g., emotion, self-regulation), as well that this has happened. This is why I have previously
as how different emotion regulation strategies (e.g., cited this and related work as examples of reappraisal
cognitive change, response modulation) are related. (Gross, 1998b). Parkinson (this issue) also worries
This challenge is noted in several commentaries. about cases such as expressive suppression (in which
One commentary advocates dramatically trimming emotion-expressive behavior is inhibited), saying that
the definition of emotion regulation so that it refers he sees the processes associated with suppression as
only to “the set of processes that determines the offset very different from those associated with other forms
of an activated emotional response” (Koole & Veen- of emotion regulation, such as reappraisal. Here I
stra, this issue, p. 62). However, one disadvantage of agree with Parkinson that there seem to be important
this truncated conception of emotion regulation is differences in the antecedents, mechanisms, and out-
that it fails to address efforts to modify emotion onset, comes associated with suppression and reappraisal
emotion quality (i.e. which emotion is activated), or (Gross, 2013b). However, as I’ve argued in the target
emotion magnitude. These constitute much of what article, I see both as instances of the broader class of
people do in everyday life to regulate emotions emotion regulation, united by the goal to modify the
(Gross et al., 2006). emotion trajectory.
A second commentary argues for both splitting These definitional debates—which mirror those
and lumping (Kross, this issue). One motivation for that have played out in the field—highlight the need
splitting is that emotion regulation strategies do not to clarify the psychological and neurobiological
seem homogeneous. For example, Kross (this issue) mechanisms that underlie emotion, emotion regula-
indicates that cognitive change may not be a “pure tion, and other forms of affect and affect regulation.
form” because “people can change the way they think This is a major goal of the EPM. From this perspec-
to change the way they feel in potentially infinite tive, whether one lumps or splits depends upon one’s
ways” and because it seems likely that “certain reap- particular research goals, and I’ve argued that the
praisals are more helpful in certain situations com- field would benefit from moving beyond debates
pared to others” (p. 69). I agree, and this is why in the about what distinctions should be made to demonstra-
EPM I’ve emphasized the distinction between broad tions of what is gained (in terms of new understand-
emotion regulation strategies (such as cognitive ing of the underlying processes) with one or another
change) and the specific tactics that are used in a par- approach (Gross, Sheppes, & Urry, 2011b).
ticular situation. Elsewhere, Kross argues in favor of
lumping, saying that it may be time “to begin inte-
Empirical Challenges
grating across closely related areas of inquiry to iden-
tify common threads,” and gives as an example an However defined, one of the key empirical chal-
analysis of what cognitive change and attentional lenges is providing a more detailed account of the
135
GROSS

mechanisms that enable different forms of emotion challenge is to identify with greater specificity which
regulation in different contexts. This is important neural populations are engaged at each point in the
because, as Parkinson (this issue) correctly notes, the emotion regulation process for any given emotion
EPM currently doesn’t provide implementation-level regulation strategy or tactic. As noted in the target
details. article, one promising approach is manipulating
One domain in which there has been particularly (enhancing or degrading) these brain systems A sec-
exciting progress is the domain of regulating ond approach is examining emotion regulation suc-
responses to food cues. In one seminal study in this cesses and failures in individuals with emotion-
domain, self-reported dieters were recruited for a relevant disorders.
functional magnetic resonance imaging study in
which they made decisions about which foods they
wished to eat (Hare, Camerer, & Rangel, 2009). Concluding Comment
Some participants were classified as self-control-
lers—they made decisions based both on the health It is now widely appreciated that emotion regula-
value of the food item and its taste. Other participants tion plays a crucial role in healthy adaptation and that
were classified as non-self-controllers—they made difficulties with emotion regulation are associated
decisions based on taste alone. Findings suggested with psychological and physical health problems.
that (a) activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex This realization has led to a surge of interest in emo-
(vmPFC) was associated with the value participants tion regulation processes. My goal in the target article
placed on food cues; (b) vmPFC responses were mod- was to extend the original process model of emotion
ulated by both health and taste for self-controllers, regulation, which I introduced nearly two decades
and only by taste for non-self-controllers; (c) success- ago now, when the field of emotion regulation was
ful self-control (i.e., choosing the healthier option) just emerging.
was associated with relatively greater recruitment of I am very grateful to the dozen-plus scholars who
left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC); and (d) took the time to carefully read the target article and
self-controllers showed greater recruitment of dlPFC provide such thoughtful commentaries. In this reply,
than non-self-controllers. I have endeavored to elaborate upon core features of
Subsequent studies have begun to more directly the EPM, to draw out some of the applications of the
examine the mechanisms that underlie self-regulation EPM suggested by the commentators, and to point to
by manipulating these processes. For example, in one crucial theoretical and empirical challenges that lie
study, participants were asked to either up- or down- ahead. Whatever disagreements may exist, I remain
regulate their food cravings while making decisions unremittingly optimistic about the immediate and
about what to eat (Hutcherson, Plassman, Gross, & longer-term benefits of a deeper understanding of
Rangel, 2012). Findings indicated that down-regula- emotion and emotion regulation.
tion of craving led to decreased responses in the
value-sensitive region of the dlPFC but not in the
vmPFC (selective value modulation) and that the rel- Acknowledgments
ative contribution of dlPFC signal to behavior
increased (behavior control modulation). By contrast, I thank Charles Carver, Amit Goldenberg, Gal
up-regulation of craving led to increased responses in Sheppes, and Gaurav Suri for their helpful insights
the vmPFC but not dlPFC, and the relative contribu- and comments.
tion of the vmPFC signal to behavior increased.
These and related findings are beginning to clarify
how valuation systems interact with one another to Note
produce behavior.
How might these developments be leveraged to Address correspondence to James Gross, Depart-
sharpen our understanding of the mechanisms that ment of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford,
support emotion regulation? Efforts to describe the CA 94305-2130. E-mail: gross@stanford.edu
brain systems that support emotion regulation are
best developed for cognitive change. These efforts
suggest that reappraisal engages dorsomedial, dorso- References
lateral, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, as well as
temporal and parietal cortex. Depending on the con- Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: Cap-
text, this network either up- or down-regulates the turing context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8,
155–172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459518
activity of emotion-generative systems including the Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., &
amygdala and ventral striatum. Yet these brain Troetschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious acti-
regions are each highly differentiated, and one crucial vation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality

136
AUTHOR’S REPLY

& Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027. http://dx.doi.org/ Hutcherson, C. A., Plassmann, H., Gross, J. J., & Rangel, A.
10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1014 (2012). Cognitive regulation during decision-making shifts
Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An behavioral control between ventromedial and dorsolateral
individual differences perspective on coping and emotion reg- prefrontal value systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 32,
ulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 591–612. 13543–13554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6387-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691613504116 11.2012
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful Insel, T. R., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S.,
conceptual framework for personality, social, clinical and Quinn, K., . . ., & Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria
health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111–135. (FDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.92.1.111 on mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 167,
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2013). Goals and emotion. In E. R. 748–751.
W. M. D. Robinson & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of Kuppens, P., Stouten, J., & Mesquita, B. (2009). Individual differ-
cognition and emotion (pp. 176–194). New York, NY: ences in emotion components and dynamics: Introduction to
Guilford. the special issue. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1249–1258.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930902985605
positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psycho- Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford, UK:
logical Review, 97, 19–35. Oxford University Press.
Gross, J. J. (1998a). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion Lazarus, R. S., Opton, E. M., Nomikos, M. S., & Rankin, N. O.
regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expres- (1965). The principle of short-circuiting of threat: Further evi-
sion, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- dence. Journal of Personality, 33, 622–635. http://dx.doi.org/
chology, 74, 224–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1965.tb01408.x
3514.74.1.224 Magen, E., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Getting our act together: Toward a
Gross, J. J. (1998b). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An general model of self-control. In R. Hassin, K. N. Ochsner, &
integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271– Y. Trope (Eds.), Self control in society, mind, and brain (pp.
299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 335–353). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Gross, J. J. (2013a). Conceptualizing emotional labor: An emotion Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the
regulation perspective. In A. A. Grandey, J. M. Diefendorff, & structure of behavior. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and
D. E. Rupp (Eds.), Emotional labor in the 21st century: Winston.
Diverse perspectives on the psychology of emotion regulation Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2014). The neural bases of emotion
at work (pp. 288–294). New York, NY: Psychology Press/ and emotion regulation: A valuation perspective. In J. J. Gross
Routledge. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 23–42).
Gross, J. J. (2013b). Emotion regulation: Taking stock and moving for- New York, NY: Guilford.
ward. Emotion, 13, 359–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032135 Powers, W. T., Clark, R. K., & McFarland, R. L. (1960a). A general
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: feedback theory of human behavior: Part I. Perceptual and
Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Motor Skills, 11, 71–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/
Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970–986. http://dx. pms.1960.11.1.71
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.970 Powers, W. T., Clark, R. K., & McFarland, R. L. (1960b). A gen-
Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regula- eral feedback theory of human behavior: Part II. Perceptual
tion in everyday life. In D. K. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, & J. N. and Motor Skills, 11, 309–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/
Hughes (Eds.), Emotion regulation in couples and families: pms.1960.11.3.309
Pathways to dysfunction and health (pp. 13–35). Washington, Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework
DC: American Psychological Association. for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making.
Gross, J. J., Sheppes, G., & Urry, H. L. (2011a). Emotion genera- Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 545–556. http://dx.doi.org/
tion and emotion regulation: A distinction we should make 10.1038/nrn2357
(carefully). Cognition and Emotion, 25, 765–781. http://dx. Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (in press). Emotion regulation
doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.555753 and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology.
Gross, J. J., Sheppes, G., & Urry, H. L. (2011b). Taking one’s lumps Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why?: Pleasure
while doing the splits: A big tent perspective on emotion gener- and utility in emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psy-
ation and emotion regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 789– chological Science, 18, 101–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
793. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.586590 j.1467-8721.2009.01617.x
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Con- Webb, T. L., Gallo, I. S., Miles, E., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran,
ceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion P. (2012). Effective regulation of affect: An action control per-
regulation (pp. 3–24). New York, NY: Guilford. spective on emotion regulation. European Review of Social
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in Psychology, 23, 143–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation 10463283.2012.718134
system. Science, 324, 646–648. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/ Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Control and communication in the
science.1168450 animal and the machine. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

137

Anda mungkin juga menyukai