Anda di halaman 1dari 15

The impact of environmental education

on sense of place among urban youth


ALEX KUDRYAVTSEV,  MARIANNE E. KRASNY, AND RICHARD C. STEDMAN

Department of Natural Resources, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14853 USA

Citation: Kudryavtsev, A., M. E. Krasny, and R. C. Stedman. 2012. The impact of environmental education on sense of
place among urban youth. Ecosphere 3(4):29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1890/ES11-00318.1

Abstract. Research suggests that an ecologically informed sense of place, including strong place
attachment and ecological place meaning, contributes to pro-environmental behaviors. Yet it is unclear
whether an intervention such as environmental education can intentionally influence sense of place,
especially in cities. To investigate the impact of urban environmental education programs on sense of place,
we used pre/post surveys of youth in 5-week environmental and non-environmental summer youth
programs in the Bronx, New York City, in 2010. Results show that urban environmental education
programs—which engaged urban high school students in environmental stewardship, recreation,
environmental skills development, and environmental monitoring in the Bronx—were successful in
nurturing ecological place meaning, but did not strengthen students’ place attachment. No significant
changes in place attachment or place meaning were observed after non-environmental, control programs.

Key words: ecological place meaning; place attachment; sense of place; survey; urban environmental education.

Received 9 November 2011; revised 21 February 2012; accepted 24 February 2012; published 18 April 2012.
Corresponding Editor: C. D’Avanzo.
Copyright: Ó 2012 Kudryavtsev et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License, which permits restricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and sources are credited.
  E-mail: ak383@cornell.edu

INTRODUCTION tions such as education programs, especially in


the urban context.
In a recent article about the Ecological Society Given a call for environmental stewardship
of America’s Earth Stewardship initiative to including in urban areas, and the relationship
promote ‘‘the long-term integrity of the biosphere between sense of place and pro-environmental
and human well-being,’’ Chapin et al. (2011) behavior, we wanted to explore whether sense of
suggest that sense of place fosters an individual’s place can be nurtured by urban environmental
willingness to engage in environmental steward- education. This question is especially important
ship, including in cities. In fact, research shows given that half of the world’s population lives in
that different aspects of sense of place contribute cities (UN-HABITAT 2008), characterized as ‘‘the
to pro-environmental behaviors or behavioral dominant global human habitat’’ (Grove 2009)
intentions (Stedman 2002, Walker and Chapman and ‘‘the defining ecological phenomenon of the
2003, Ryan 2005, Halpenny 2010, Scannell and twenty-first century’’ (Newman and Jennings
Gifford 2010). Keeping in mind this relationship, 2008). We hypothesized that sense of place can
scholars propose that sense of place could be be influenced by urban environmental education.
purposely influenced in order to promote pro- Since natural elements are less dominant in cities
environmental behavior (Walker and Chapman relative to more rural settings (Barlett 2005a,
2003). However, little is known about whether Johnson and Catley 2009), a particularly provoc-
sense of place can be modified through interven- ative question is whether education programs

v www.esajournals.org 1 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

can help people view living organisms, biological meanings. Firey (1945) recognized that people
processes and ecosystems as integral parts of the ascribe symbols to places based on cultural
urban environment, i.e., as part of their sense of values and historical associations, and these
place. symbols may influence land use. Lynch (1960)
Urban environmental education programs, in was one of the first to use the term ‘‘sense of
which inner-city students explore local natural place,’’ referring to symbolic and memorable
phenomena or participate in stewardship, have aspects of the urban environment. In the 1970s,
existed in the Bronx for many years. In the early Tuan (1974, 1975, 1977) developed an experien-
1950s, students from kindergarten to the upper tial perspective on sense of place, which in his
grades were using urban natural trails in the view is created through personal experiences
Bronx to learn about natural science (Polley et al. with physical settings, and which can be under-
1953), and over the last four decades school stood through holistic studies of lived experienc-
groups have taken part in environmental mon- es. At the same time, Relph (1976) distinguished
itoring and wetlands restoration near the Bronx such aspects of sense of place as place attachment
River (Tanner et al. 1992). More recently, students and place meaning. In his view, place attachment
in public schools (de Kadt 2006), and in represents ties between people and places, and
education programs in community-based orga- place meaning is the essence of places or
nizations such as Rocking the Boat and Youth symbolic associations of places that define
Ministries for Peace and Justice (Young 2008, de people’s individual and cultural identity. These
Kadt 2011), have engaged in learning about the earlier works inspired sense of place scholarship
Bronx environment through water testing, field in different fields such as environmental psy-
trips, collaborating with researchers, restoration chology, human geography, cultural anthropolo-
projects and related activities. Although urban gy, architecture, sociology, and leisure studies
environmental education programs have a long (see a review by Farnum et al. 2005), as well as
history, we are not aware of research showing urban environmental restoration, stewardship,
that urban environmental education programs and conservation (Ryan 2000, Andersson et al.
foster an ecologically informed sense of place. 2007, Spartz and Shaw 2011).
In 2008, the first author conducted exploratory While there are multiple conceptualizations of
interviews of urban environmental educators in sense of place, in this paper we define sense of
six community-based organizations in the Bronx, place as a combination of place attachment and
New York City. The educators claimed that their place meaning (Stedman 2000a, 2002, Stokowski
programs were reconnecting urban communities, 2002, Stedman 2003b, Farnum et al. 2005,
including youth, with their urban natural envi- Smaldone et al. 2005, Van Patten and Williams
ronment in order to improve their pro-environ- 2008, Trentelman 2009, Semken and Brandt 2010)
mental behavior. Upon being introduced to the (Fig. 1). ‘‘Place attachment’’ is the bond between
notion of sense of place, educators agreed that people and places (Low and Altman 1992,
this ‘‘reconnecting’’ with the urban environment Jorgensen and Stedman 2001, Stedman 2003a,
could be conceptualized as nurturing an ecolog- Davenport and Anderson 2005). Conceptually,
ically based sense of place. Thus, the purpose of place attachment includes place dependence, i.e.,
this study was to determine the impact of urban the potential of a place to support preferred
environmental education programs on youths’ activities (Stokols and Shumaker 1981, Vaske and
ecologically informed sense of place in the Bronx. Kobrin 2001, Farnum et al. 2005, Halpenny 2006),
and place identity, i.e., the extent to which a place
SENSE OF PLACE LITERATURE reflects personal identity (Proshansky et al. 1983,
Korpela 1989, Trentelman 2009). ‘‘Place mean-
The idea of sense of place has evolved during ing’’ refers to the symbolic meanings that people
several decades and has been used in different ascribe to places (Stedman 2000b, 2002, 2008,
fields. Leopold’s (1949) suggestion that land- Smaldone et al. 2008), which may reflect the
scapes include multiple aspects such as ethical, physical, natural, social, cultural, familial, polit-
esthetic, economic, and ecological resembles the ical, economic or other aspects of places (Ardoin
current idea of multiple dimensions of place 2006, Semken and Butler Freeman 2008). In sum,

v www.esajournals.org 2 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

Fig. 1. Components of sense of place (adapted from Kudryavtsev et al. 2012).

place attachment reflects how strongly people life’’ contributes to supporting environmental
gravitate towards places, while place meaning protection policies. Similarly, Scannell and Gif-
describes the reason for place attachment (Sted- ford (2010) found that place attachment based on
man 2008). the natural rather than the civic aspects of a place
In recent years researchers accumulated em- predicted pro-environmental behavior, and Hen-
pirical evidence that sense of place—including wood and Pidgeon (2001) showed that people
place attachment and the ecological dimension of express concerns about potential urbanization if
place meanings—may contribute to place-specific their place meanings include such symbols as
pro-environmental behaviors, behavioral inten- trees and forest. Supporting this idea, Andersson
tions, and attitudes. For example, correlational at al. (2007) revealed that strong place attach-
studies showed that people with strong place ment along with place meanings related to
attachment are likely to contribute to solutions of ecological knowledge and practice were drivers
local environmental problems (Kaltenborn 1998), for stewardship in allotment gardens. Finally,
support bans on motorized recreation in natural researchers contend that people tend to protect
areas (Warzecha and Lime 2001), hold negative places (Manzo and Perkins 2006) or aspects of
attitudes towards hydropower development (Vor- places (Stedman 2003b) that are meaningful to
kinn and Riese 2001), express an intention to them, which is consistent with the idea that
maintain valued natural resources such as water emphasized ecological place meanings may
quality in lakes (Stedman 2002), volunteer in contribute to pro-environmental behavior.
parks (Walker and Chapman 2003), and be Factors influencing sense of place have been
concerned about conserving nature in cities (Ryan reasonably well explored and can be organized
2005). Similarly, several studies using structural in two groups: direct experience of settings, and
equation modeling showed that place attachment learning about places from other people or
predicts place-specific pro-environmental behav- interpretive materials. A number of empirical
ior such as volunteering to protect parks (Hal- studies demonstrate that place attachment is
penny 2010), civic actions such as donation of time strengthened by frequent visits and use of places
and effort in nature refuges (Payton et al. 2005), (Ryan 2005), commitment to outdoor recreation
general pro-environmental behavior not related to activities that happen in a particular place
a specific place such as supporting environmental (Moore and Scott 2003), long-term residence
organizations and carpooling (Lee 2011), and (Lewicka 2005), and active engagement with
other types of behavior such as participating in a places such as participating in hands-on envi-
community cleanup (Vaske and Kobrin 2001). ronmental stewardship activities (Ryan et al.
Other studies suggest that pro-environmental 2001). Place attachment can also be strengthened
attitudes and behavior are fostered by strong through social interactions in places and oppor-
place attachment in combination with emphasized tunities to be a functional community member
ecological place meaning. Advancing our under- (Chawla 1992, Eisenhauer et al. 2000, Barlett
standing of the interaction between attachment 2005b, Ryan and Grese 2005). Place meanings are
and meanings, Brehm et al. (2006) found that somewhat more difficult to trace causally (Sted-
attachment that is based on such place meanings man 2002) but can be informed by direct
as ‘‘natural landscapes’’ and ‘‘presence of wild- experiences with places, including by character-

v www.esajournals.org 3 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

Box 1
Place attachment and ecological place meaning Likert scales (5-points: ‘‘Strongly disagree,’’
‘‘Somewhat disagree,’’ ‘‘Neutral,’’ ‘‘Somewhat agree,’’ and ‘‘Strongly agree’’).

Place Attachment Scale


1. The Bronx is the best place for what I like to do.
2. I feel like the Bronx is part of me.
3. Everything about the Bronx reflects who I am.
4. I am more satisfied in the Bronx than in other places.
5. I identify myself strongly with the Bronx.
6. The Bronx is not a good place for what I enjoy doing. (reverse coded)
7. There are better places to be than the Bronx. (reverse coded)
8. The Bronx reflects the type of person I am.

Ecological Place Meaning Scale


1. The Bronx is a place to connect with nature.
2. The Bronx is a place to watch animals and birds.
3. The Bronx is a place where people can find nature.
4. The Bronx is a place where trees are an important part of community.
5. The Bronx is a place where people have access to rivers.
6. The Bronx is a place where people come to community gardens.
7. The Bronx is a place where people have access to parks.
8. The Bronx is a place to canoe and boat.
9. The Bronx is a place to have fun in nature.
10. The Bronx is a place to learn about nature.
11. The Bronx is a place to enjoy nature’s beauty.
12. The Bronx is a place to grow food.

istics of the biophysical environment (Stedman goal to nurture sense of place in urban youth, this
2003a, Manzo 2005), as well as by information research explores the impact of urban environ-
about a place from other sources (Johnson and mental education on two components of sense of
Zipperer 2007). Traveling outside of a place may place. Specifically, we ask: (1) What is the effect of
help people accentuate its meanings, which may urban environmental education on youth’s place
be taken for granted (Davenport and Anderson attachment? (2) What is the effect of urban
2005, Smaldone et al. 2008), and sometimes environmental education on youth’s ecological
people may realize that they were attached to a place meaning?
place after it has been changed (Ryan 2000). To answer these questions, we conducted a
Scholars also suggest that place meanings can be survey study in environmental and non-environ-
conveyed, nurtured or created through interpre- mental summer youth programs in the Bronx,
tative materials, mass media, literature, films, New York City. First, we developed and pilot
photography, legends, customs, discussions, sto- tested a sense of place survey with youth in
rytelling, and other social interactions (Stewart et urban settings in the Bronx. Then we used a
al. 1998, Stokowski 2002, Vanclay 2008, Malpas quasi-experimental research design to implement
2010). pre/post-program sense of place surveys with
Bronx youth in an experimental group (urban
RESEARCH QUESTION AND METHODS environmental education programs) and a con-
trol group (non-environmental summer youth
Given the link between sense of place and pro- employment programs). Pre/post-program sur-
environmental behavior, and given educators’ vey results from both groups were compared by

v www.esajournals.org 4 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

two-tailed t-tests, using Stata 10 software. We scale described below. We administered the scale
also used Pearson’s correlation to explore wheth- to ten high school students (approximately 15
er place attachment becomes more based on years old) in the Bronx participating in summer
ecological place meanings after urban environ- youth employment programs that were not
mental education programs. related to the environment. After completing
the paper-based survey the students discussed
SURVEY DEVELOPMENT how their understanding of the questions, which
led to minor revisions of items to make them
To explore the impact of urban environmental more understandable.
education on Bronx students’ sense of place, we
adapted an existing place attachment scale Ecological place meaning scale
(Jorgensen and Stedman 2001) and created a We are not the first to employ a quantitative
new ecological place meaning scale appropriate approach to explore place meaning. For example,
for the urban context (Box 1). in relation to a national park, Young (1999a, b)
used a five-point scale to rate how well a place
Place attachment scale can be described by 26 place meaning items such
To assess place attachment, scholars often use as ‘‘ecologically important,’’ ‘‘scenic,’’ and ‘‘spir-
Likert scale surveys with items such as ‘‘This is itually valuable.’’ In addition, Stedman (2002,
the best place for what I like to do’’ and ‘‘I feel 2003b) and Stedman et al. (2007) used Likert-
like this place is part of me’’ (Williams and scale surveys in a rural county to assess
Roggenbuck 1989, Stedman 2000a, Jorgensen and meanings related to environmental quality
Stedman 2001, Warzecha and Lime 2001, Kyle et (‘‘My lake is a place of high environmental
al. 2004). In all of the studies that we are aware quality’’), meanings related to social aspects of
of, place attachment scales are reliable (Cron- places (‘‘My lake is a place to escape from
bach’s alpha . 0.7), whether place attachment is civilization’’), and ecological place meanings
measured as one scale (Moore and Scott 2003, appropriate for rural areas (‘‘My lake is a pristine
Stedman et al. 2007) or two separate scales for wilderness’’). Although researchers have called
place dependence and place identity (Vaske and for the development of a scale to measure an
Kobrin 2001, Williams and Vaske 2003, Burduk et ecological dimension of the relationship between
al. 2009). people and places (Davenport and Anderson
To measure place attachment, we used a five- 2005), we are not aware of ecological place
point Likert scale with items representing two meaning scales per se, especially those applicable
sub-constructs: place identity and place depen- to the urban environment.
dence. We adapted these items from Jorgensen To measure ecological place meaning in the
and Stedman’s (2001) scale, a reliable scale used Bronx, we constructed a five-point Likert scale
in previous research projects (e.g., Stedman et al. with 12 items. These items share a common
2007, Halpenny 2010). Although other place underlying construct: viewing nature-related
attachment scales have been adapted for youth phenomena, including ecosystems and associat-
(Vaske and Kobrin 2001, Rioux 2011), we decided ed activities, as symbols of the Bronx. To create
not to use them because some of their items are scale items, we asked environmental educators in
not completely consistent with our underlying six community-based organizations in the Bronx
theoretical constructs. For example, Vaske and to list phenomena (e.g., birds and parks) and
Kobrin’s (2001) items ‘‘I am very attached to this activities (e.g., gardening and canoeing) that may
place’’ and ‘‘I think often about coming here’’ are serve as ecological place meanings in the Bronx.
supposed to reflect place identity; yet we contend Based on conversations with the educators we
that these items probably reflect place attachment created an ecological place meaning scale that
overall, not specifically place identity. To ensure initially included 17 items. This scale was refined
that our place attachment scale could be under- through pilot testing with the ten above-men-
stood and used with urban high school students, tioned high school students to make the items
in January 2010 we conducted a pilot test of this understandable for this age group. The final
scale, along with the ecological place meaning ecological place meaning scale was reduced to 12

v www.esajournals.org 5 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

items to avoid redundancy. We decided to word SURVEY IMPLEMENTATION


all items positively in the ecological place
meaning scale because we were focusing on To explore the impact of urban environmental
meanings that have a positive valence. Further, education programs on sense of place in youth
we did not want to unnecessarily burden or we used a quasi-experimental research design,
confuse our youthful respondents by including which involves experimental and control groups
negative non-ecological place meanings or re- where true randomization is not possible (An-
wording some items as negative (e.g., ‘‘The Bronx derson 1990, Shadish and Cook 2002, Wiersma
is not a place to get close to nature’’) (DeVellis and Jurs 2005). We administered pre/post-pro-
2003). gram surveys to youths in urban environmental
Content validity of the ecological place mean- education (experimental group) and non-envi-
ing scale, i.e., how appropriate the items are to ronmental summer youth employment programs
measure a construct (Haynes et al. 1995, Litwin (control group) located along the Bronx River
1995, DeVellis 2003), is based primarily on the watershed in New York City (Table 1). In July-
expertise of the Bronx environmental educators August 2010, experimental and control students
who helped us create scale items reflecting participated in 5–6-week summer youth pro-
nature-related settings and activities appropriate grams in community-based organizations and at
for the local context. Content validity can be a high school, usually Monday through Friday,
compromised by exclusion or under-representa- about 24 hours per week. Students in both
tion of items reflecting different dimensions of a groups earned minimum wage, except for 16
construct (Haynes et al. 1995); thus we decided to students within the experimental group who
include a relatively large number of scale items earned high school credit toward graduation.
that are representative of and relevant to the Programs in the experimental group can be
Bronx natural environment. Construct validity, broadly categorized as urban environmental
which is ‘‘the extent to which a particular education. This is the term preferred by educa-
measure relates to other measures consistent tors in these programs. Although a number of
with theoretically derived hypotheses concerning studies have focused on environmental educa-
the concept’’ (Carmines and Zeller 1979) or ‘‘how tion in cities (Krasny and Tidball 2009, Tidball
meaningful the scale or survey instrument is and Krasny 2010), a comprehensive literature
when in practical use’’ (Litwin 1995), can be and theoretical framework for urban environ-
tested through experimentation in which two mental education is lacking. Urban environmen-
groups are expected to differ on the test tal education programs involve urban youth in
(Cronbach and Meehl 1955). In our case, as we restoration, stewardship, monitoring, recreation
describe in the results section below, construct and activism with the ultimate goal to improve
validity is confirmed by the fact that ecological ecological and social aspects of the urban
place meaning became more emphasized among environment. A shared goal in most urban
students in urban environmental education, and environmental education in the Bronx is recon-
not among students in the non-environmental necting urban communities with the urban
programs. environment. In addition, these programs pursue

Table 1. High school students in experimental and control groups.

Group Group description Age Sex


Experimental Students in 5–6-week summer urban environmental education Mean age ¼ 16.2 ? ¼ 36 (57%)
(n ¼ 63) programs in three community-based organizations and one high Min age ¼ 14 / ¼ 27 (43%)
school: Satellite Academy High School in the Bronx (16 students), Max age ¼ 21
Rocking the Boat (12), Mosholu Preservation Corporation (20), and SD ¼ 0.23
Youth Ministries for Peace and Justice (15).
Control Students of similar characteristics in 5–6-week non-environmental Mean age ¼ 16.3 ? ¼ 13 (54%)
(n ¼ 24) summer youth employment programs in two community-based Min age ¼ 14 / ¼ 11 (46%)
organizations: Phipps Community Development Corporation (14 Max age ¼ 20
students) and the Point Community Development Corporation (10). SD ¼ 0.39

v www.esajournals.org 6 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

other goals such as youth development, which overlapping educational approaches justify cate-
we did not evaluate. gorizing these environmental programs as one
Organizations in our study regularly conduct experimental group, despite some differences in
several-week urban environmental education actual activities. In contrast, students in the
programs in spring, summer and fall semesters. control group participated in office work and
We administered surveys in summer when a mentoring younger students in summer pro-
relatively large number of new students join grams, while engaging in activities related to
these programs, which allows conducting a mixed media, arts, dance, and sports, which took
quasi-experimental study with a reasonable place mostly indoors. Most students in both the
sample size. Our sample included youth partic- experimental and control groups also participat-
ipants at all available urban environmental ed in team building activities and college visits.
education programs whose curriculum focused On the first day of the programs, students
on the environment along the Bronx River completed the pre-program, paper-based survey
watershed in the Bronx, New York City in at home because parental permission was re-
summer 2010. The content of urban environmen- quired for students under 18 years old. Students
tal education programs in our study varied. who took the pre-program survey also partici-
Instead of using or adapting existing curricula pated in the post-program survey on the last day
such as Project Learning Tree or Project WET, of their programs at the sites where their
educators designed their own activities. Four programs were held, and received $5 in com-
activities dominated each program: (1) environ- pensation. In the experimental group, 63 students
mental stewardship, (2) recreation, (3) environ- completed both pre/post-program surveys (80%
mental monitoring, and (4) trainings and return rate); and in the control group, 24 students
workshops (Fig. 2). completed both surveys (60% return rate).
Environmental stewardship activities in the Differences between experimental and control
experimental group were embedded in civic groups (Table 1) in terms of participants’ mean
ecology practices (Tidball and Krasny 2010; age (t(85) ¼ 0.25, p ¼ 0.80) and sex ratio (Chi2(1, N
Krasny and Tidball, in press), e.g., working ¼ 87) ¼ 0.06, p ¼ 0.80) are not significant. Most
alongside environmental leaders or community students in the experimental group (86%) and
members to steward street trees, restore oyster control group (92%) live in the Bronx, while a few
reefs, water plants in community gardens, students live in other boroughs of New York City.
remove invasive plants in an urban forest, According to educators, except for 2–3 returning
reintroduce fish in the Bronx River, or maintain students in the experimental and control groups,
a green roof. Recreation activities included students were participating in these programs for
canoeing, kayaking, or rowing on the Bronx the first time and had limited prior knowledge
River or other waterways. Environmental mon- about the programs. Participants’ ethnicity was
itoring activities took place in parks, botanical not recorded, but we observed that both exper-
and community gardens, or along waterways, imental and control groups were comprised of
and included creel surveys, bird surveys, or approximately equal numbers of African Amer-
water quality testing. Trainings and workshops icans and Latinos.
led by invited community leaders, professional
ecologists, and staff from local colleges included RESULTS
indoor and outdoor sessions focused on learning
about environmental science and developing Place attachment mean scores in pre/post-
environmental skills such as tree pruning and program surveys in the experimental and control
plant identification. In addition, each program in groups ranged from 2.77 to 3.02, which is about
the experimental group included several unique the midpoint on the 5-point scale (Table 2).
activities such as a food survey at farmers Ecological place meaning scores in the same
markets and stores; a trip to a farm, island or surveys were slightly above the midpoint (Table
historic area outside the Bronx; or watching a 3), with the exception of the post-program
movie related to environmental justice. The experimental group, which scored higher (3.57).
shared focus on the urban environment and The pre-program unpaired t-test demonstrated

v www.esajournals.org 7 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

Fig. 2. Examples of urban environmental education activities in the Bronx, New York City, summer 2010: (A)
Environmental stewardship on a green roof, Youth Ministries for Peace and Justice, (B) Recreation on the Bronx
River, Rocking the Boat, (C) Biodiversity monitoring by students from Satellite Academy High School, (D) Tree
pruning workshop conducted by Trees New York for students at Mosholu Preservation Corporation. Photos:
Alex Kudryavtsev.

v www.esajournals.org 8 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

Table 2. Place attachment survey results.

Pre-program Post-program Paired t-test


Group n Mean SD Alpha Mean SD Alpha t df p
Experimental 63 2.90 0.88 0.85 3.02 0.83 0.84 1.378 62 0.173
Control 24 2.85 0.94 0.90 2.77 0.91 0.86 0.532 23 0.600

Table 3. Ecological place meaning survey results.

Pre-program Post-program Paired t-test


Group n Mean SD Alpha Mean SD Alpha t df p
Experimental 63 3.16 0.91 0.93 3.57 0.85 0.92 4.777 62 0.000
Control 24 3.04 0.88 0.91 3.05 0.93 0.94 0.048 23 0.962

no significant difference between the experimen- that, to a certain extent, interventions such as
tal and control groups in terms of their initial urban environmental education may nurture
place attachment (t(85) ¼ 0.239, p ¼ 0.812) or their sense of place, which others have found might
initial ecological place meaning (t(85) ¼ 0.557, p ¼ foster place-specific pro-environmental behav-
0.579), which suggests that the likelihood of iors.
initial selection biases is small. The survey results in the experimental group
Using paired t-tests to compare pre/post- suggest that relatively short yet intensive sum-
program mean scores, we found that place mer urban environmental education programs
attachment showed no significant change in may significantly increase students’ ecological
either group (Table 2). At the same time, we place meaning, i.e., their perceptions of the
found that the mean score for ecological place presence and importance of nature in the local
meaning increased significantly in the experi- urban setting. Because improvement was not
mental group from 3.16 to 3.57, and did not observed in the control group engaged in non-
change in the control group (Table 3). For the environmental programs, strengthening ecologi-
experimental group, Pearson’s correlation be- cal place meaning in the experimental group may
tween place attachment and ecological place be attributed to these urban environmental
meaning was not significant in pre-program education programs that combine multiple teach-
(r(61) ¼ 0.177, p ¼ 0.166), but became significant ing approaches. Our finding is consistent with
post-program (r(61) ¼ 0.358, p ¼ 0.004). In the the idea that place meanings are not solely
control group, this correlation was significant in inherent (Greider and Garkovich 1994) and
pre-program (r(22) ¼ 0.416, p ¼ 0.043) and post- may be influenced through direct experiences
program (r(22) ¼ 0.728, p ¼ 0.000). and interpretations of places (Cuba and Hum-
mon 1993). Indeed, ecological processes in urban
DISCUSSION places can become part of sense of place through
participation in the environmental restoration
As the global population becomes increasingly activities (Newman and Jennings 2008) that were
urban (Bloom 2011), attention needs to be paid to a significant component of the urban environ-
how humans can foster sustainability and pro- mental education programs in this study. How-
vide for ecosystem services in cities (Andersson ever, based on our survey data, we are unable to
2006). In particular, scholars have called for determine the effect of specific aspects of urban
enhancing environmental stewardship and relat- environmental education programs on ecological
ed environmental education in cities (Tidball and place meaning. For example, recreation in natural
Krasny 2007, Krasny and Tidball 2009), and areas and environmental monitoring activities
suggest that sense of place may facilitate stew- may have a different effect on ecological place
ardship for ecosystem resilience and human well- meaning.
being (Chapin et al. 2011). Our research shows The pre-program mean scores of ecological

v www.esajournals.org 9 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

place meaning in the experimental and control Bronx students’ weak place attachment. It is also
groups were just above the midpoint on a 5-point possible that, similar to environmental steward-
scale, and thus cannot be considered particularly ship activities in other studies (Ryan et al. 2001,
high scores. Our explanation of these scores is Ryan 2005), urban environmental education with
based on ideas that place meanings are rooted in a focus on environmental stewardship is more
characteristics of the physical environment (Sted- likely to foster general place attachment to
man 2003a), which is far from pristine in cities, certain types of ecosystems, such as rivers and
and social and interpretive mechanisms through parks, than attachment to a particular place.
which place meanings are developed, negotiated Whereas we measured place attachment only
and shared (Stewart et al. 1998, Stokowski 2002). to the Bronx, one could hypothesize that urban
Urban environmental educators suggested sev- environmental education may be more successful
eral reasons for moderate pre-program ecological in strengthening place attachment towards spe-
place meaning scores, including students’ gener- cific places where education activities are con-
ally limited experience of natural aspects of the ducted, such as a certain park, section of a river,
Bronx before urban environmental education or particular community garden. Further, our
programs (C. Kennedy, personal communication). findings contrast with another study in the non-
Some of these students rarely experienced the urban context (Semken and Butler Freeman
natural environment in the inner city because of 2008), in which undergraduate students’ place
highways, industrial facilities, or other infrastruc- attachment towards Arizona significantly
ture blocking access to waterfronts or other green strengthened as the result of taking an introduc-
areas in their communities (J. Terrell, personal tory geology course. We may hypothesize that
communication). In addition, some parents in the pedagogical approaches, curriculum, audience
Bronx discourage their children from involve- demographics, location and length of residence,
ment with the urban natural environment in- the scale and characteristics of places, and other
cluding community gardens because of its factors determine the effect of different types of
perceived lack of safety (J. Plewka, personal education programs on place attachment and
communication). Finally, ecological place meaning sense of place in general. Factors influencing
in the Bronx is perhaps sometimes underempha- sense of place in the urban stewardship context
sized due to stigmatization of this area as could be addressed in future quantitative studies
ecologically degraded, akin to other types of with a larger sample size or in-depth qualitative
stigmatization of inner-city places (Wacquant studies. In fact, currently we are conducting
2007). Similar to what researchers have reported narrative research with educators and youth in
in relation to inner-city, high density neighbor- these same Bronx organizations to explore the
hoods in general (Permentier et al. 2011), mechanisms of nurturing sense of place among
residents of the Bronx may think that the Bronx urban students.
has a poor reputation compared to ‘‘low-density The mean pre/post-program place attachment
garden-city neighborhoods,’’ and thus assign scores in the Bronx in both experimental and
little ecological meaning to this place. control groups are around the midpoint or lower
Contrary to place meaning, urban environ- on the 5-point scale. In contrast, place attachment
mental education programs in the Bronx did not in other studies conducted in more natural areas
significantly strengthen place attachment. This such as trails, parks and lakes was considerably
result may be explained by research that suggests above the midpoint (Moore and Scott 2003,
that place attachment develops over long or Stedman et al. 2007). Based solely on our
frequent experiences of places (Tuan 1977, Hay research we cannot claim that attachment to a
1998). The environmental education programs in city is in general lower than attachment to more
this study were only 5–6 weeks long, which is rural or natural places. Yet relatively low place
perhaps not enough time to increase attachment attachment in the Bronx could be explained by
to a place where most participants already reside. the fact that many students in the Bronx hold
Sometimes people do not bond with a place even both positive as well as strong negative place
if they grew up there (Johnson and Zipperer meanings underpinning their place attachment.
2007), which may be another explanation of For example, in informal conversations with the

v www.esajournals.org 10 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

first author, some students mentioned such munity-based initiatives to create more urban
positive descriptors of the Bronx as family, farms, roof gardens, community gardens and
friends, and home, as well as negative descrip- greenways, or to further restore aquatic ecosys-
tors such as crime, poverty, underserved schools, tems and urban forests. Similar to other feedback
industrial facilities, highways, empty lots, dirty loops in social-ecological systems (Tidball and
streets, air pollution and lack of parks. Krasny 2011), it is possible that ecological place
While place attachment may be based on meanings and community-based environmental
different place meanings (Stedman 2003b), in- stewardship may be reinforcing each other,
cluding social and natural (Brehm et al. 2004, especially if education programs are embedded
Brehm 2007), a notable result of this study is that in environmental stewardship.
the correlation between place attachment and Finally, developing ecological place meaning
ecological place meaning in the experimental could redefine self-identity of urban residents,
group became significant after treatment. This which, given the link between self-identity and
suggests that, although place attachment in the pro-environmental behavior (Devine-Wright and
experimental group did not increase, it became Clayton 2010, Whitmarsh and O’Neill 2010), may
more based on an ecological set of place influence how people interact with their envi-
meanings. This also corresponds to Barlett’s ronment. Sense of place in general ‘‘is understood
(2005a) idea that attachment to urban places can as closely linked to identity’’ (McClaren 2009)
be based on meanings of place related to such and our place meanings are related to our sense
natural components as trees, grass and birds. of self and may tell who we are (Korpela 1989,
However, an unexpected result was that in the Hull et al. 1994). In addition, meanings that
control group this correlation was significant in people attribute to their environment are viewed
both pre-program and post-program survey, as ‘‘symbolic reflections of how people define
which may suggest that there were some themselves’’ (Greider and Garkovich 1994).
unobserved differences between control and Hence it is possible to assume that place
experimental groups that are not easily interpret- meanings like ‘‘The Bronx is a place to connect
able. with nature’’ may foster such self-conceptions as
Applying the concept of ecological place ‘‘I am a person who connects with nature in the
meaning to urban settings is quite provocative Bronx,’’ thus contributing to nature conservation
because usually the built environment rather attitudes and environmental stewardship in the
than the natural environment dominates our urban context.
attention in cities (Barlett 2005a, Budruk et al.
2009), and because natural aspects are sometimes CONCLUSION
perceived as occurring only outside the city
(Johnson and Catley 2009). One of the motiva- Resonating with Chapin and colleagues’ (2011)
tions for this research was that acknowledging call for Earth Stewardship, previous research has
the presence of green areas in cities and of the demonstrated that place-based stewardship be-
ecological worthiness of urban places—which haviors may be facilitated by sense of place. Our
reflects positive place meanings—might inspire research further shows that sense of place in
commitment to urban environmental steward- cities can be nurtured by urban environmental
ship (Light 2003, Ryan 2005). In contrast, education. Urban environmental education pro-
exclusively negative environmental information, grams in the Bronx help young people see
which is sometimes emphasized in environmen- ecological aspects of the urban landscape as
tal education, media, and other descriptions of legitimate and worthwhile. These programs
cities, may lead to the denial of environmental teach students to view cities as places to interact
problems (Dickinson 2009) or the feeling that one with nature, grow food, and engage in outdoor
cannot contribute to environmental solutions recreation and learning. Such sense of place may
(Ewing and Gold 2011). Thus urban environ- ultimately enhance environmental stewardship
mental education programs that emphasize in urban communities. The concept of ecological
ecological place meaning or worthiness of the place meaning—combined with other constructs
natural environment in cities may inspire com- such as place attachment, self-identity, pro-

v www.esajournals.org 11 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

environmental behavior, and community-based dimensions of attachment. American Behavioral


restoration—may open new avenues for thinking Scientist 50:142–165.
about how people interact with urban natural Budruk, M., H. Thomas, and T. Tyrrell. 2009. Urban
resources and what motivates them to engage in green spaces: a study of place attachment and
environmental attitudes in India. Society and
environmental stewardship in cities.
Natural Resources 22:824–839.
Burduk, M., H. Thomas, and T. Tyrrell. 2009. Urban
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS green spaces: a study of place attachment and
environmental attitudes in India. Society and
We thank urban environmental educators and Natural Resources 22:824–839.
organizers in New York City—including Anthony Carmines, E. G., and R. A. Zeller. 1979. Reliability and
Archino, Jennifer Beaugrand, Sharon De La Cruz, validity assessment. Sage, London, UK.
Gretchen Ferenz, Adam Green, Damian Griffin, Addy Chapin, F. S., M. E. Power, S. T. A. Pickett, A. Freitag,
Guance, Dawn Henning, Carol Kennedy, Adam J. A. Reynolds, R. B. Jackson, D. M. Lodge, C.
Liebowitz, Stephen Oliveira, Danny Peralta, Jennifer Duke, S. L. Collins, A. G. Power, and A. Bartuska.
Plewka, Anne-Marie Runfola, Julien Terrell, and 2011. Earth Stewardship: science for action to
Chrissy Word—for facilitating many aspects of this sustain the human-earth system. Ecosphere 2:89.
project. We thank anonymous reviewers for their Chawla, L. 1992. Childhood place attachments. Pages
comments. The Community Forestry and Environ- 63–86 in I. Altman and S. M. Low, editors. Place
mental Research Partnership at University of Califor- attachment. Plenum Press, New York, New York,
nia, Berkeley provided funding for this study. USA.
Cronbach, L. J., and P. E. Meehl. 1955. Construct
LITERATURE CITED validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bul-
letin 52:281–302.
Anderson, G. 1990. Fundamentals of educational Cuba, L., and D. M. Hummon. 1993. A place to call
research. Falmer, London, UK. home: identification with dwelling, community,
Andersson, E. 2006. Urban landscapes and sustainable and region. Sociological Quarterly 34:111–131.
cities. Ecology and Society 11:34. Davenport, M. A., and D. H. Anderson. 2005. Getting
Andersson, E., S. Barthel, and K. Ahrné. 2007. from sense of place to place-based management: an
Measuring social-ecological dynamics behind the interpretive investigation of place meaning and
generation of ecosystem services. Ecological Appli- perceptions of landscape change. Society and
cations 17:1267–1278. Natural Resources 18:625–641.
Ardoin, N. 2006. Toward an interdisciplinary under- de Kadt, M. 2006. The Bronx River: a classroom for
standing of place: lessons for environmental environmental, political and historical studies.
education. Canadian Journal of Environmental Capitalism Nature Socialism 17:99–110.
Education 11:112–126. de Kadt, M. 2011. The Bronx River: an environmental
Barlett, P. F. 2005a. Introduction. Pages 1–34 in P. F. and social history. History Press, Charleston, South
Bartlett, editor. Urban place: reconnecting with the Carolina, USA.
natural world. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachu- DeVellis, R. F. 2003. Scale development: theory and
setts, USA. applications. Second edition. Sage, Thousand Oaks,
Barlett, P. F. 2005b. Reconnecting with place: faculty California, USA.
and the Piedmont Project at Emory University. Devine-Wright, P., and S. Clayton. 2010. Introduction
Pages 39–60 in P. F. Barlett, editor. Urban place: to the special issue: place, identity and environ-
reconnecting with the natural world. MIT Press, mental behavior. Journal of Environmental Psy-
Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA. chology 30:267–270.
Bloom, D. E. 2011. 7 billion and counting. Science Dickinson, J. L. 2009. The people paradox: self-esteem
333:562–567. striving, immortality ideologies, and human re-
Brehm, J. M. 2007. Community attachment: the sponse to climate change. Ecology and Society
complexity and consequence of the natural envi- 14:34.
ronment facet. Human Ecology 35:477–488. Eisenhauer, B. W., R. S. Krannich, and D. J. Blahna.
Brehm, J. M., B. W. Eisenhauer, and R. S. Krannich. 2000. Attachment to special places on public lands:
2004. Dimensions of community attachment and an analysis of activities, reasons for attachments,
their relationship to well-being in the amenity-rich and community connections. Society and Natural
rural West. Rural Sociology 69:405–429. Resources 13:421–441.
Brehm, J. M., B. W. Eisenhauer, and R. S. Krannich. Ewing, K., and W. Gold. 2011. Realizing the educa-
2006. Community attachments as predictors of tional potential of ecological restoration. Pages
local environmental concern: the case for multiple 347–361 in D. Egan, E. E. Hjerpe, and J. Abrams,

v www.esajournals.org 12 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

editors. Human dimensions of ecological restora- environmental self-regulation. Journal of Environ-


tion: integrating science, nature, and culture. Island mental Psychology 9:241–256.
Press, Washington, D.C., USA. Krasny, M. E., and K. G. Tidball. 2009. Applying a
Farnum, J., T. Hall, and L. E. Kruger. 2005. Sense of resilience systems framework to urban environ-
place in natural resource recreation and tourism: an mental education. Environmental Education Re-
evaluation and assessment of research findings. search 15:465–482.
General technical report PNW-GTR-660. U.S. De- Krasny, M. E., and K. G. Tidball. In press. Civic ecology:
partment of Agriculture, Forest Service, Pacific a pathway for Earth Stewardship in cities. Frontiers
Northwest Research Station, Portland, Oregon, in Ecology and the Environment.
USA. Kudryavtsev, A., R. C. Stedman, and M. E. Krasny.
Firey, W. 1945. Sentiment and symbolism as ecological 2012. Sense of place in environmental education.
variables. American Sociological Review 10:140– Environmental Education Research 18:229–250.
148. Kyle, G. T., A. Graefe, R. Manning, and J. Bacon. 2004.
Greider, T., and L. Garkovich. 1994. Landscapes: the Effects of place attachment on users’ perception of
social construction of nature and the environment. social and environmental conditions in a natural
Rural Sociology 59:1–24. setting. Journal of Environmental Psychology
Grove, J. M. 2009. Cities: managing densely settled 24:213–225.
social-ecological systems. Pages 281–294 in F. S. Lee, T. H. 2011. How recreation invovlement, place
Chapin, G. P. Kofinas, and C. Folke, editors. attachment and conservation commitment affect
Principles of ecosystem stewardship: resilience- environmentally responsible behavior. Journal of
based natural resource management in a chaning Sustainable Tourism 19:895–915.
world. Springer, New York, New York, USA. Leopold, A. 1949. A Sand County almanac and
Halpenny, E. A. 2006. Environmental behaviour, place sketches here and there. Oxford University Press,
attachment and park visitation: a case study of New York, New York, USA.
visitors to Point Pelee National Park. Dissertation. Lewicka, M. 2005. Ways to make people active: the role
University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada. of place attachment, cultural capital, and neigh-
Halpenny, E. A. 2010. Pro-environmental behaviours borhood ties. Journal of Environmental Psychology
and park visitors: the effect of place attachment. 25:381–395.
Journal of Environmental Psychology 30:409–421. Light, A. 2003. Urban ecological citizenship. Journal of
Hay, R. 1998. Sense of place in developmental context. Social Philosophy 34:44–63.
Journal of Environmental Psychology 18:5–29. Litwin, M. S. 1995. Hot to measure survey reliability
Haynes, S. N., D. C. S. Richard, and E. S. Kubany. 1995. and validity. Sage, Thousand Oaks, California,
Content validity in psychological assessment: a USA.
functional approach to concepts and methods. Low, S. M., and I. Altman. 1992. Place attachment: a
Psychological Assessment 7:238–247. conceptual inquiry. Pages 1–12 in I. Altman and
Henwood, K., and N. Pidgeon. 2001. Talk about woods S. M. Low, editors. Place attachment. Plenum Press,
and trees: threat of urbanization, stability, and New York, New York, USA.
biodiversity. Journal of Environmental Psychology Lynch, K. 1960. The image of the city. MIT Press,
21:125–147. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA.
Hull, B. R., M. Lam, and G. Vigo. 1994. Place identity: Malpas, J. 2010. New media, cultural heritage and the
symbols of self in the urban fabric. Landscape and sense of place: mapping the conceptual ground.
Urban Planning 28:109–120. International Journal of Heritage Studies 14:197–
Johnson, C. Y., and W. C. Zipperer. 2007. Culture, place 209.
and urban growth in the U.S. South. Urban Manzo, L. C. 2005. For better or worse: exploring
Ecosystems 10:459–474. multiple dimensions of place meaning. Journal of
Johnson, E. A., and K. M. Catley. 2009. Urban soil Environmental Psychology 25:67–86.
ecology as a focal point for environmental educa- Manzo, L. C., and D. D. Perkins. 2006. Finding
tion. Urban Ecosystems 12:79–93. common grounds: the importance of place attach-
Jorgensen, B. S., and R. C. Stedman. 2001. Sense of ment to community participation and planning.
place as an attitude: lakeshore owners attitudes Journal of Planning Literature 20:335–350.
toward their properties. Journal of Environmental McClaren, M. 2009. The place of the city in environ-
Psychology 21:233–248. mental education. Pages 301–307 in M. McKenzie,
Kaltenborn, B. P. 1998. Effects of sense of place on P. Hart, H. Bai, and B. Jickling, editors. Fields of
responses to environmental impacts: a study green: restorying culture, environment, and edu-
among residents in Svalbard in the Norwegian cation. Hampton, Cresskill, New Jersey, USA.
high Arctic. Applied Geography 18:169–189. Moore, R. L., and D. Scott. 2003. Place attachment and
Korpela, K. M. 1989. Place-identity as a product of context: comparing a park and a trail within. Forest

v www.esajournals.org 13 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

Science 49:877–884. tion, and indigenous knowledge systems. Springer,


Newman, P., and I. Jennings. 2008. Cities as sustainable London, UK.
ecosystems: principles and practices. Island Press, Semken, S., and C. Butler Freeman 2008. Sense of place
Washington, D.C., USA. in the practice and assessment of place-based
Payton, M. A., D. C. Fulton, and D. H. Anderson. 2005. science teaching. Science Education 92:1042–1057.
Influence of place attachment and trust on civic Shadish, W. R., and T. D. Cook. 2002. Experimental
action: a study at Sherburne National Wildlife and quasi-experimental designs for generalized
Refuge. Society and Natural Resources 18:511–528. causal inference. Houghton Mifflin, Boston, Mas-
Permentier, M., G. Bolt, and M. van Ham. 2011. sachusetts, USA.
Determinants of neighborhood satisfaction and Smaldone, D., C. Harris, and N. Sanyal. 2005. An
perception of neighborhood reputation. Urban exploration of place as a process: the case of
Studies 48:977–996. Jackson Hole, WY. Journal of Environmental
Polley, J. W., J. O. Loretan, and C. F. Blitzer. 1953. Psychology 25:397–414.
Community action for education: the story of the Smaldone, D., C. Harris, and N. Sanyal. 2008. The role
Bronx Park community of New York City. Teachers of time in developing place meanings. Journal of
College, Columbia University, New York, New Leisure Research 40:479–504.
York, USA. Spartz, J., and B. R. Shaw. 2011. Place meanings
Proshansky, H. M., A. K. Fabian, and R. Kaminoff. surrounding an urban natural area: a qualitative
1983. Place-identity: physical world socialization of inquiry. Journal of Environmental Psychology
the self. Journal of Environmental Psychology 3:57– 31:344–352.
83. Stedman, R. C. 2000a. Attitude and identity in the
Relph, E. 1976. Place and placelessness. Pion, London, prediction of behavior: fight or flight from threat-
UK. ened places? In 63rd Annual Meeting of the Rural
Rioux, L. 2011. Promoting pro-environmental behav- Sociological Society, Washington, D.C.
iour: collection of used batteries by secondary Stedman, R. C. 2000b. Up north: a social psychology of
school pupil. Environmental Education Research place. Dissertation. University of Wisconsin, Mad-
17:353–373. ison, Wisconsin, USA.
Ryan, R. L. 2000. A people-centered approach to Stedman, R. C. 2002. Toward a social psychology of
designing and managing restoration projects: in- place: predicting behavior from place-based cogni-
sights from understanding attachment to urban tions, attitude, and identity. Environment and
natural areas. Pages 209–228 in P. H. Gobster and Behavior 34:561–581.
B. R. Hull, editors. Restoring nature: perspectives Stedman, R. C. 2003a. Is it really just a social
from the social sciences and humanities. Island construction? The contribution of the physical
Press, Washington, D.C., USA. environment to sense of place. Society and Natural
Ryan, R. L. 2005. Exploring the effects of environmen- Resources 16:671–685.
tal experience on attachment to urban natural Stedman, R. C. 2003b. Sense of place and forest science:
areas. Environment and Behavior 37:3–42. toward a program of quantitative research. Forest
Ryan, R. L., and R. E. Grese. 2005. Urban volunteers Science 49:822–829.
and the environment: forest and prairie restoration. Stedman, R. C. 2008. What do we ‘‘mean’’ by place
Pages 171–188 in P. F. Bartlett, editor. Urban place: meanings? Implications of place meanings for
reconnecting with the natural world. MIT Press, managers and practitioners. Pages 61–81 in L. E.
Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA. Kruger, T. E. Hall, and M. C. Stiefel, editors.
Ryan, R. L., R. Kaplan, and R. E. Grese. 2001. Understanding concepts of place in recreation
Predicting volunteer commitment in environmen- research and management. General technical report
tal stewardship programmes. Journal of Environ- PNW-GTR-744. U.S. Department of Agriculture,
mental Planning and Management 44:629–648. Forest Service, Pacific Northwest Research Station,
Scannell, L., and R. Gifford. 2010. The relations Portland, Oregon, USA.
between natural and civic place attachment and Stedman, R. C., R. C. Lathrop, B. Clark, J. Ejsmont-
pro-environmental behavior. Journal of Environ- Karabin, P. Kasprzak, K. Nielsen, D. Osgood, M.
mental Psychology 30:289–297. Powell, A.-M. Ventelä, K. E. Webster, and A.
Semken, S., and E. Brandt. 2010. Implications of sense Zhukova. 2007. Perceived environmental quality
of place and place-based education for ecological and place attachment in North American and
integrity and cultural sustainability in diverse European temperate lake districts. Lake and
places. Pages 287–302 in D. J. Tippins, M. P. Reservoir Management 23:330–344.
Mueller, M. van Eijck, and J. D. Adams, editors. Stewart, E. J., B. M. Hayward, and P. J. Devlin. 1998.
Cultural studies and environmentalism: the con- The ‘‘place’’ of interpretation: a new approach to
fluence of ecojustice, place-based (science) educa- the evaluation of interpretation. Tourism Manage-

v www.esajournals.org 14 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29


KUDRYAVTSEV ET AL.

ment 19:257–266. Making sense of place: exploring concepts and


Stokols, D., and S. A. Shumaker. 1981. People in places: expressions of place through differen sense and
a transactional view of settings. Pages 442–488 in lenses. National Museum of Australia Press,
J. H. Harvey, editor. Cognition, social behavior, and Canberra, ACT, Australia.
the environment. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, Vaske, J. J., and K. C. Kobrin. 2001. Place attachment
New Jersey, USA. and environmentally responsible behavior. Journal
Stokowski, P. A. 2002. Languages of place and of Environmental Education 32:16–21.
discourses of power: constructing new senses of Vorkinn, M., and H. Riese. 2001. Environmental
place. Journal of Leisure Research 34:368–382. concern in a local context: the significance of place
Tanner, M. J., D. Hernandez, J. A. Hernandez, and P. S. attachment. Environment and Behavior 33:249–
Mankiewicz. 1992. Restoration-based education on 263.
the Bronx River. Restoration and Management Wacquant, L. 2007. Territorial stigmatization in the age
Notes 10:14–17. of advanced marginality. Thesis Eleven 91:66–77.
Tidball, K. G., and M. E. Krasny. 2007. From risk to Walker, G. J., and R. Chapman. 2003. Thinking like a
resilience: What role for community greening and park: the effects of sense of place, perspective-
civic ecology in cities? Pages 149–164 in A. E. J. taking, and empathy on pro-environmental inten-
Wals, editor. Social learning towards a more tions. Journal of Park and Recreation Administra-
sustainable world. Wagengingen Academic Press, tion 21:71–86.
Wagengingen, The Netherlands. Warzecha, C. A., and D. W. Lime. 2001. Place
Tidball, K. G., and M. E. Krasny. 2010. Urban attachment in Canyonlands National Park: visitors’
environmental education from a social-ecological assessment of setting attributes on the Colorado
perspective: conceptual framework for civic ecolo- and Green Rivers. Journal of Park and Recreation
gy education. Cities and the Environment 3:11. Administration 19:59–78.
Tidball, K. G., and M. E. Krasny. 2011. Toward an Whitmarsh, L., and S. O’Neill. 2010. Green identity,
ecology of environmental education and learning. green living? The role of pro-environmental self-
Ecosphere 2:21. identity in determining consistency across diverse
Trentelman, C. K. 2009. Place attachment and commu- pro-environmental behaviours. Journal of Environ-
nity attachment: a primer grounded in the lived mental Psychology 30:305–314.
experience of a community sociologist. Society and Wiersma, W., and S. Jurs. 2005. Research methods in
Natural Resources 22:191–210. education: an introduction. Pearson, Boston, Mas-
Tuan, Y.-F. 1974. Topophilia: a study of environmental sachusetts, USA.
perception, attitudes, and values. Prentice-Hall, Williams, D. R., and J. W. Roggenbuck. 1989. Measur-
Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, USA. ing place attachment: some preliminary results.
Tuan, Y.-F. 1976. Geopiety: a theme in man’s attach- Paper presented at the 1989 Symposium on Leisure
ment to nature and to place. Pages 11–39 in D. Research. National Recreation and Park Associa-
Lowenthal and M. Bowden, editors. Geographies tion, Arlington, Virginia, USA.
of the mind. Oxford University Press, New York, Williams, D. R., and J. J. Vaske. 2003. The measurement
New York, USA. of place attachment: validity and generalizability of
Tuan, Y.-F. 1977. Space and place: the perspective of a psychometric approach. Forest Science 49:830–
experience. University of Minnesota Press, Minne- 840.
apolis, Minnesota, USA. Young, H. 2008. The Big Apple and beyond: challenges
UN-HABITAT. 2008. State of the world’s cities 2008/ and successes of habitat restoration in the Long
2009: harmonious cities. Earthscan, London, UK. Island Sound Watershed. Ecological Restoration
Van Patten, S. R., and D. R. Williams. 2008. Problems in 26:182–184.
place: using discursive social psychology to inves- Young, M. 1999a. The relationship between tourist
tigate the meanings of seasonal homes. Leisure motivations and the interpretation of place mean-
Sciences 30:448–464. ing. Tourism Geographies 1:387–405.
Vanclay, F. 2008. Place matters. Pages 3–11 in F. Young, M. 1999b. The social construction of tourist
Vanclay, M. Higgins, and A. Blackshaw, editors. places. Australian Geographer 30:373–389.

v www.esajournals.org 15 April 2012 v Volume 3(4) v Article 29

Anda mungkin juga menyukai