Anda di halaman 1dari 4

Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 1981 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1981, Vol. 28, No. 5, 451-454 0022-0167/81/280S-0451$00.75

Brief Reports
Cognitive-Behavioral Prevention
of Adolescent Pregnancy
Steven Paul Schinke, Betty J. Blythe,
and Lewayne Dorman Gilchrist
University of Washington

Cognitive and behavioral methods were applied to assist adolescents with


avoidance of unplanned pregnancy. Small group training gave high-school
sophomores contraceptive information, steps for solving problems, and prac-
tice in communicating decisions about sexual behavior. Compared with un-
trained, control-condition teenagers, trained teenagers had more positive
posttest scores on measures of sexual knowledge, interpersonal problem solv-
ing, and in vivo performance. At 6-month follow-up, young women and young
men who participated in training groups had better attitudes toward family
planning and were practicing more effective contraception than were young
people in control conditions. Research findings have implications for cogni-
tive and behavioral counseling aimed at the primary prevention of personal
and social difficulties faced by young people.

Each year in this country, 1.3 million adoles- reduce early childbearing (Schinke, 1978,1979),
cents become pregnant (Tietze, 1978). With the mandate innovative approaches to help adoles-
annual number of births to teenagers growing at cents avoid unplanned pregnancy.
16%, members of this age cohort are responsible One primary prevention approach, which is
for one of every five deliveries (National Center advanced by the present team, includes cognitive
for Health Statistics, 1979). Abortions to young and behavioral factors that influence knowledge
people are also on the rise. The over 500,000 acquisition and use (Schinke & Gilchrist, 1977;
teenagers opting to prematurely terminate their Schinke, Gilchrist, & Small, 1979). In relation to
pregnancies represent a disproportionate one- preventing pregnancy, this means adolescents
third of legal and reported abortions in the United must possess factual information about human
States (National Center for Health Statistics, reproduction and contraception. Also, they need
1980). Although most young people neither in- to solve problems when relating birth-control
tend nor want to get pregnant (National Center information to themselves. Responsible sexual
for Health Statistics, 1978), fewer than 7% relin- behavior involves interpersonal communication
quish their babies for adoption or substitute care so young people can obtain contraception, regu-
(Zelnik & Kantner, 1978). Such statistics, com- late sexual encounters, and negotiate the use of
bined with a disappointing legacy of attempts to birth control. Indeed, survey and exploratory
data from Campbell and Barnlund (1977), Cvet-
kovich, Grote, Lieberman, and Miller (1978), and
Funding was provided by Grants HD 11095 and HD Dembo and Lundell (1979) correlate contracep-
02274 from the National Institute of Child Health and
Human Development (National Institutes of Health)
tive failure among young people with deficient
and by Maternal and Child Health Training Project 913 cognitive and behavioral skills. This conceptual
from the Bureau of Community Health Services (Health framework was the basis of a prospective test of
Services Administration), all awarded to the University cognitive-behavioral training aimed at preventing
of Washington Child Development and Mental Retar- unplanned adolescent pregnancy.
dation Center and administered through the United
States Public Health Service, Department of Health
and Human Services. Method
Gratefully thanked are Anna Bolstad, Gloria Burt,
Leona Eggert, Joan Hiltner, Charles Gelso, Lois Holt, Participants and Design
Andrew Renggli, Edith Watson, and our anonymous
reviewers. The study took place in a large public high
Requests for reprints should be sent to Steven Paul school. A sample of 36 students (19 women, 17
Schinke, School of Social Work, University of Wash- men; mean age = 15.89 years) was drawn from the
ington, Seattle, Washington 98195. sophomore class. After they and their parents

451
452 BRIEF REPORTS

gave written consent, students were assigned scribed for scoring. The performance test, pre-
randomly to one condition of a Solomon four- pared in pilot work (Blythe, Gilchrist, & Schinke,
group research design: pretest, training, and in press; Schinke, Gilchrist, & Blythe, 1980), en-
posttest; training and posttest; pretest and gaged individual young women and young men in
posttest; and posttest only. videorecorded interactions with an opposite-sex,
agemate confederate. A series of vignettes re-
Procedure quired youths to instigate and respond in stressful
dating situations (e.g., being asked to spend the
Trainers for the two contact conditions were a night with a date even though neither person uses
female and a male graduate student in second- birth control, having to broach the topic of con-
year internships of a M.S.W. program. Their traception with a reluctant partner). Tapes
ages were 29 and 28; they had 3 and 4 years were indexed with elapsed time and code de-
counseling experience and had not previously scriptors and randomly arranged for blind scoring
counseled teenagers on sexual matters. (Schinke & Smith, 1979). Follow-up 6 months
The pair carried out cognitive and behavioral after training measured adolescents' attitudes
training in 14 50-minute group sessions. Re- about family planning and birth control prac-
productive biology and contraceptive methods tices.
were covered by guest speakers, audiovisual aids, Measures were scored by two assistants, naive
and Socratic discussions. Problem solving fol- to study conditions and hypotheses. Computed
lowed Goldfried and Goldfried's (1980) schema with the Pearson product-moment correlation
and encompassed specifying problems, coming up coefficient, r, scoring reliability for problem-
with ways to deal with problems, judging the solving instruments ranged from .926 to .988 (M
worth and payoffs of these options, and planning = .973, SD = .026). Performance test scoring
how to apply selected options in demand situa- reliability went from .874 to 1.000 (M = .931, SD
tions. The young people were guided through the = .022). Analyses of resultant scores revealed no
sequence as they confronted decisions about pretest or within-condition differences. Ac-
dating, sexuality, birth control, pregnancy, cordingly, between-conditions analyses were done
abortion, childbearing, and parenthood. on posttest and follow-up data summed sepa-
Leaders taught components of verbal and rately across training and control conditions.
nonverbal communication via modeling, role play, Stochastic differences were examined by the t
and rehearsal (Schinke, 1981; Schinke, Gilchrist, statistic.
Smith, & Wong, 1979). Young people acted as
one another's antagonist, coach, and feedback Results
source as they practiced communicating contra-
ceptive decisions. Training groups concluded Knowledge test scores differentiated young
with teenagers' contracting to exercise acquired people in cognitive-behavioral prevention con-
knowledge and actions during the intervening ditions from those in control conditions. Young
week (Schinke & Rose, 1976). Outside applica- people given pregnancy prevention training an-
tions were monitored at the next group swered correctly more questions on human re-
meeting. production, £(34) = 3.40, p < .002, and birth
control, £(34) = 2.63, p < .02, than did control-
Measures and Analyses condition adolescents. Problem-solving mea-
sures characterized cognitive-behavioral condi-
By design, half of the young people completed tion youths as more skilled than those in control
pretest measures; all of them took identical conditions when they specified interpersonal
measures at the end of prevention training. problems, £(34) = 2.48, p < .02, identified obsta-
Measures encompassed reproductive and birth- cles to solving them, £(34) = 2.28, p < .05, and
control knowledge, problem solving, and inter- generated potential solutions to them, £(34) =
personal performance. 2.31, p<.05.
Schedules from Miller and Lief (1979) and from Teenagers' performance in videotaped inter-
Schinke (in press) quantified young people's actions distinguished between cognitive-behav-
awareness of sexual facts, myths, probabilities, ioral and control conditions. Retrospective rat-
and sequelae. Problem-solving instruments were ings of videotapes favored youths in the former
variants of the Perspective Taking Test (Shure condition on use of eye contact with the oppo-
& Spivack, Note 1), Means-End Thinking Test site-sex interpersonal partner, £(34) = 4.43, p <
(Shure & Spivack, 1972), and Anticipation of .001, the declarative "No" in response to social
Consequences Test (Spivack, Platt, & Shure, pressure, £(34) = 3.04, p < .005, statements ex-
1976). The three instruments were given orally, pressing refusal to risk pregnancy, £(34) = 2.19,
and youths' answers were recorded and tran- p < .05, and requests that partners share re-
BRIEF REPORTS 453

sponsibility for birth control and cognate sexual to equip youths for stresses of independent living,
decisions, t(34) = 2.46, p < .05. money management, marriage, and parenting
Six-month follow-up data signified the carry- (Hawkins, Bradley, & White, 1977; Little &
over of pregnancy prevention training. Be- Kendall, 1979; Robin, in press).
tween-conditions analyses evidenced better at- Future investigations might bridge gaps left by
titudes toward family planning, t(32) = 2.08, p < the present research. The contribution of each
.05, more habitual contraception, t(32) = 2.38, p cognitive and behavioral component—informa-
< .05, greater protection at last intercourse, t (32) tion, problem solving, interpersonal communi-
= 3.26, p < .005, and less reliance on inadequate cation—could be studied in factorial designs.
birth control, i(32) = 4.35, p < .001, for trained Longitudinal follow-up data with larger samples
than for untrained, control-condition teen- would test the maintenance of posttraining
agers. changes and would quantify additional benefits
Subjective clinical reports lend warmth to ob- of generalized learning. Since first intercourse
jective outcomes (Kazdin, 1977; Wolf, 1978). is happening early (Zabin, Kantner, & Zelnik,
Obtained from anonymous end-of-session ques- 1979), pregnancy prevention could profit pre-
tionnaires, feedback from training condition teenagers. Sorely needed are formats sensitive
participants praised group process and content. to young members of ethnic and racial minorities
Illuminating were "This is neat stuff!" "Hearing (Fry, Kropf, & Coe, 1980; Sue & Sue, 1977).
other guys having a hard time talking about sex Cognitive-behavioral pregnancy prevention
is great." "I like role playing." "Let's keep doing counseling seems a promising approach to help
like you have to do on dates when you get has- young people deal with sexual issues and related
sled." "I really use what we talk about. I even dilemmas.
tell my sister about it." In the same vein,
teachers and parents expressed the value of cog-
nitive-behavioral training. Just a single com- Reference Note
plaint was voiced, due to an apparent inconsis- 1. Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. Cognitive problem
tency between a parent's prior understanding of solving skills, adjustment, and social class (Re-
the study and a young person's description of one search and Evaluation Report No. 22). Philadel-
group session. phia, Pa.: Department of Mental Health Services,
Hahnemann Community Mental Health/Mental
Retardation Center, 1970.
Discussion
Results of this study are promising. Group References
cognitive and behavioral training let young
women and young men acquire skills to prevent Blythe, B. J., Gilchrist, L. D., & Schinke, S. P. Preg-
unplanned pregnancy. High-school sophomores nancy-prevention groups for adolescents. Social
Work, in press.
who participated in cognitive-behavioral training, Campbell, B. K., & Barnlund, D. C. Communication
compared to classmates assigned to assessment- patterns and problems of pregnancy. American
only conditions, gained sexual knowledge, prob- Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1977,47,134-139.
lem-solving abilities, and persuasive patterns of Cvetkovich, G., Grote, D., Lieberman, E. J., & Miller,
interpersonal communication. Six months after W. Sex role development and teenage fertility-re-
group training, cognitive-behavioral condition lated behavior. Adolescence, 1978,13, 231-236.
adolescents were more favorably disposed toward Dembo, M. H., & Lundell, B. Factors affecting ado-
family planning and were practicing more effec- lescent contraception practices: Implications for sex
tive contraception than were control-condition education. Adolescence, 1979,14, 657-64.
Fry, P. S., Kropf, G., & Coe, K. J. Effects of counselor
adolescents. and client racial similarity on the counselor's response
The described approach to counseling adoles- patterns and skills. Journal of Counseling Psy-
cents has several advantages. Doubtless, the chology, 1980, 27,130-137.
untoward consequences of early pregnancy are Gilchrist, L. D., Schinke, S. P., & Blythe, B. J. Primary
best avoided through primary prevention (Gil- prevention services for children and youth. Children
christ, Schinke, & Blythe, 1979). Group contexts and Youth Services Review, 1979,1, 379-391.
enable human services workers to reach greater Goldfried, M. R., & Goldfried, A. P. Cognitive change
numbers of young people. Cognitive and be- methods. In F. H. Kanfer & A. P. Goldstein (Eds.),
havioral methods tap synergetic skills for com- Helping people change (2nd ed.). New York:
Pergamon Press, 1980.
petently handling adolescent sexuality (Dembo Hawkins, J. G., Bradley, R. W., & White, G. W. Anxi-
& Lundell, 1979). The cognitive-behavioral ety and the process of deciding about a major and
approach seems equally suited to alleviate teen- vocation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1977,
age struggles with alcohol and drug abuse, delin- 24,398-403.
quency, family conflicts, and vocation choices and Kazdin, A. E. Assessing the clinical or applied impor-
454 BRIEF REPORTS

tance of behavior change through social validation. tion. Social Work in Health Care, 1977, 3,
Behavior Modification, 1977,1, 427-452. 159-167.
Little, V. L., & Kendall, P. C. Cognitive-behavioral Schinke, S. P., Gilchrist, L. D., & Blythe, B. J. Role of
interventions with delinquents: Problem solving, communication in the prevention of teenage preg-
role-taking, and self-control. In P. C. Kendall & S. nancy. Health and Social Work, 1980, 5(3),
D. Hollon (Eds.), Cognitive-behavioral interven- 54-59.
tions. New York: Academic Press, 1979. Schinke, S. P., Gilchrist, L. D., & Small, R. W. Pre-
Miller, W. R., & Lief, H. I. The Sex Knowledge and venting unwanted adolescent pregnancy: A cogni-
Attitude Test (SKAT). Journal of Sex and Marital tive-behavioral approach. American Journal of
Therapy, 1979,5, 282-287. Orthopsychiatry, 1979, 49, 81-88.
National Center for Health Statistics, U.S. Public Schinke, S. P., Gilchrist, L. D., Smith, T. E., & Wong,
Health Services, Dept. of Health, Education, and S. E. Group interpersonal skills training in a natural
Welfare. (DHEW). Wanted and unwanted child- setting: An experimental study. Behaviour Re-
bearing in the United States, 1968, 1969, and 1972 search and Therapy, 1979, 17, 149-154.
National Natality Surveys. Vital and Health Sta- Schinke, S. P., & Rose, S. D. Interpersonal skill
tistics (Series 21, No. 32). Washington, D.C.: U.S. training in groups. Journal of Counseling Psy-
Government Printing Office, 1978. chology, 1976,23,442-448.
National Center for Health Statistics, U.S. Public Schinke, S. P., & Smith, T. E. A videotape character
Health Services, DHEW. Advance report: Final generator for training and research. Behavioral
natality statistics, 1977. Monthly Vital Statistics Engineering, 1979, 5, 101-104.
Report, 1979, 27(11), 1-27. Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. Means-end thinking, ad-
National Center for Health Statistics, United States justment, and social class among elementary-
Public Health Service, DHEW. Health, United school-aged children. Journal of Consulting and
States, 1979. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Clinical Psychology, 1972, 38, 348-353.
Printing Office, 1980. Spivack, G., Platt, J. J., & Shure, M. B. The problem-
Robin, A. L. The parent-adolescent problem-solving solving approach to adjustment. San Francisco:
communication training program. Adolescence, in Jossey-Bass, 1976.
press. Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. Barriers to effective cross-cul-
Schinke, S. P. Teenage pregnancy: The need for tural counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
multiple casework services. Social Casework, 1978, 1977, 24, 420-429.
59,406-410. Tietze, C. Teenage pregnancies: Looking ahead to
Schinke, S. P. Research on adolescent health: Social 1984. Family Planning Perspectives, 1978, 10,
work implications. In W. T. Hall & C. L. Young 205-207.
(Eds.), Health and social needs of the adolescent Wolf, M. M. Social Validity: The case for subjective
professional responsibilities. Pittsburgh, Pa.: measurement on how applied behavior analysis is
University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior
Health, 1979. Analysis, 1978,11, 203-214.
Schinke, S. P. Interpersonal-skills training with ado- Zabin, L. S., Kantner, J. F., & Zelnik, M. The risk of
lescents. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eisler, & P. M. Miller adolescent pregnancy in the first month of inter-
(Eds.), Progress in behavior modification (Vol. 11). course. Family Planning Perspectives, 1979, 11,
New York: Academic Press, 1981. 215-222.
Schinke, S. P. A school-based model for teenage Zelnik, M., & Kantner, J. F. First pregnancies to
pregnancy prevention, Social Work in Education, in women aged 15-19: 1976 and 1971. Family Plan-
press. ning Perspectives, 1978,10,11-20.
Schinke, S. P., & Gilchrist, L. D. Adolescent pregnancy:
An interpersonal skill training approach to preven- Received July 14,1980 •

Anda mungkin juga menyukai