Anda di halaman 1dari 14

776118

research-article2018
PRQXXX10.1177/1065912918776118Political Research QuarterlyAvdan and Webb

International Relations
Political Research Quarterly

Not in My Back Yard: Public


1­–14
© 2018 University of Utah
Reprints and permissions:
Perceptions and Terrorism sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1065912918776118
https://doi.org/10.1177/1065912918776118
journals.sagepub.com/home/prq

Nazli Avdan1 and Clayton Webb1

Abstract
Why does the public care more about some terrorist attacks than others? In recent years, there has been a wave
of terrorist attacks carried out by similar terrorist organizations, but these attacks have produced disparate public
responses. Existing research shows that terrorist attacks are more traumatic for people who live near terrorist targets,
but this research cannot explain differences in public attitudes about attacks occurring in other countries. We argue
that threat perceptions are shaped by the physical and personal proximity of terrorist attacks. The identities of the
victims are rarely known. People impute the characteristics of victims based on the country where the attack occurred.
These perceived identities determine the empathy people feel toward victims and affect perceptions of terrorist threats.
People feel a greater sense of vulnerability when attacks occur near their borders. We test these arguments using a
series of online experiments. We find that the location of the attack and the race and nationality of the victims drive
threat perceptions.

Keywords
terrorism, public opinion, threat perceptions, race

Why does the public care about some terrorist attacks their profile pictures with the French flag (Gicobi 2015).
more than others? Consider three of the major terrorist Facebook also activated their safety check function during
attacks of 2015. On April 2, 2015, four gunmen belonging the Paris attack. The function allows users to “check in” as
to the Somalia-based terrorist organization Al-Shabaab safe for the benefit of friends and family. Until Paris, this
attacked Garissa University College in Garissa, Kenya feature had been used exclusively for natural disasters
(Levs and Yan 2015). Using explosives and small arms, (Shearlaw 2015). The attacks in Garissa and Ankara did not
they killed 147 people and wounded seventy-nine. On provoke similar waves of sympathy and did not receive the
October 10, 2015, two Islamic State in Syria (ISIS) sui- same kind of attention. Figure 1 shows the Weekly Google
cide bombers detonated explosive vests near a train termi- searches for the word “Terrorism” in the United States in
nal in Ankara, Turkey (Dearden 2015). The twin blasts 2015. Terrorism searches did not change after the attack in
killed ninety-seven people and wounded four hundred Garissa, actually fell after the attack in Ankara, but spiked
(Letsch and Khomami 2015; Melvin 2015). Finally, on after the attack in Paris. The attacks were similar, but they
November 13, 2015, seven terrorists operating in three were not viewed as such in the United States.
teams staged coordinated attacks at six locations in Paris, What explains this disparity? An article in the Atlantic
France. ISIS claimed responsibility for the attacks described the disparate responses from the global commu-
(Mullen, Melvin, and Armstrong 2015). One hundred nity as an “empathy gap.” There is a familiarity factor
thirty people were killed, and 367 were wounded where “people tend to perk up when they see themselves in
(Chandler, Calamur, and Ford 2015). The attacks had sim- the victims” (Graham 2015). Political scientists have ana-
ilar casualty rates and were carried out by similar organi- lyzed the effects of victim identity and target location on
zations, but the public responses were notably different. threat perceptions (Abrahms 2007, 2012) but overlooked
The attacks in Paris received more attention in the United other characteristics of victims. We argue that personal and
States than the attacks in Garissa and Ankara. Celebrities physical proximity shape public perceptions of terrorist
issued public statements expressing sadness and outrage fol-
lowing the attacks in Paris (Shearlaw 2015). There were 1
University of Kansas, Lawrence, USA
public vigils held all over the United States (Stockman
Corresponding Author:
2015). National monuments were lit up in the blue, white, Clayton Webb, University of Kansas, 1541 Lilac Lane, Room 504,
and red of the French flag (Mullen, Melvin, and Armstrong Lawrence, KS 66045-3177, USA.
2015). On social media, Facebook allowed users to drape Email: webb767@ku.edu
2 Political Research Quarterly 00(0)

Figure 1.  Weekly Google searches for “terrorism.”

attacks. Personal proximity is the affinity one feels with the requires the participation of the public. The public plays
victims of an attack. Physical proximity is the distance of an the role of victim and audience. Victims of terrorism suffer
attack from one’s home country. International terrorist physical harm and psychological trauma. The public is also
attacks occur overseas, but this does not mean the locations the primary target audience. The audience extends beyond
are unimportant. People feel a greater sense of vulnerability national boundaries to international audiences. Davis and
when attacks occur near their borders; people are less con- MacDonald (2004) show how the September 11, 2001 ter-
cerned with attacks that occur far away. We test these argu- rorist attacks affected the behavior and beliefs of people
ments using two experiments and evaluate the importance living in Canada. Both domestic and international audi-
of personal and physical proximity across a variety of con- ences underpin the coercive effectiveness of terrorism.
texts. We find that personal proximity heightens threat per- The distinction between the roles of the public-as-victim
ceptions but that different features of personal proximity are and public-as-audience has shaped the way different disci-
more important than others. We find mixed evidence sug- plines analyze the consequences of terrorism. Psychologists
gesting that physical proximity matters when attacks tran- have focused on the experiences of terrorism victims, find-
spire in neighboring states. ing that people living closer to attacks are more likely to
experience psychological trauma (Cohen et al. 2002;
Fischhoff et al. 2003; Galea et al. 2002; Schlenger et al.
Public Attitudes and Terrorism 2002). In the year following the 9/11 attacks, New York
Terrorism is “the premeditated use or threat to use vio- City residents displayed symptoms of posttraumatic stress
lence by individuals or subnational groups to obtain a disorder (PTSD) (Schlenger et al. 2002). There were similar
political or social objective through the intimidation of a findings following the attacks in Madrid and London
large audience beyond that of their immediate victims” (Allouche and Lind 2010). People outside the immediate
(Enders and Sandler 2006, 3). Terrorism is a form of proximity of attack sites may experience vicarious trauma
asymmetric conflict. Lacking the resources necessary to (Davis and MacDonald 2004; Schuster et al. 2001) through
coerce target governments with overwhelming force, ter- indirect experience, for example, via media exposure.
rorist organizations apply pressure on target governments Political science research has tended to focus on the
through target populations. “Terrorism seeks either to role of the public as an audience for terrorist violence.
inflict pain on noncombatants so they will pressure their The conventional wisdom is that terrorism is a costly sig-
government to accede to the terrorists’ political demands, nal of resolve and capabilities (Kydd and Walter 2006;
or to delegitimize a government as a means to replace or Pape 2003). Terrorist attacks elicit emotional responses
coerce it” (Arreguin-Toft 2001, 103). In this view, terror- among victims and people not present for the attacks
ism is performative, strategic communication (Jenkins (Lowenstein et al. 2001). Although the likelihood of
1980; Kydd and Walter 2006). Terrorists attack popula- being harmed by terrorism is negligible, the emotional
tions to communicate their capabilities and resolve to effects of terrorism magnify the sense of imagined danger
multiple audiences, foreign and domestic. (Kunreuther 2002). Therefore, as a costly signal, terror-
The communication strategy can capture public atten- ism is only effective insofar as it alters threat perceptions.
tion. Polls show that many American voters rate terrorism Kydd and Walter (2006, 59–60) argue that “terrorism
as a critical concern (Jacobson 2003). Terrorism is more often works” because “the greater the costs a terrorist
salient than other foreign policy issues because terrorism organization is able to inflict, the more credible its threat
Avdan and Webb 3

to inflict future costs, and the more likely the target is to about out-groups with respect to our in-groups, the com-
grant concessions.” People observe the carnage and are munities to which we belong (Tajfel 1981). Individuals
meant to perceive it as dangerous (Crenshaw 1986, 400). identify with an in-group by accentuating the similarities
Terrorists try to exploit these perceptions to achieve their they have with other people (Burke 1991). The out-group
political goals (Becker and Rubinstein 2011). People are is defined in contrast to the self (Hogg and Abrams 1988).
meant to weigh the costs and put pressure on their gov- SIT stipulates that people have negative expectations
ernments. This pressure has a range of policy repercus- about members of out-groups (Linville, Fischer, and
sions whereby public attitudes shape terrorism and Salovey 1989). In contrast, people are more forgiving of,
counterterrorism output (Davis and Silver 2004; and sanguine toward, members of their in-groups. Out-
Hetherington and Suhay 2011; Huddy et al. 2005). The group animosity and in-group love cause people to make
salience of terrorism as a “very serious problem” reso- systematic attribution errors about strangers (Tajfel 1982;
nates with public support for “draconian policies” Tajfel and Turner 1979, 1986). For example, people attri-
(Jenkins 1981). bute heinous acts to inherent out-group characteristics
There is an important connection between these paral- and ignore differences among out-group members. People
lel lines of research that has not, to our knowledge, been dismiss similar behaviors by members of their own com-
considered. Psychologists analyze the importance of munities by attributing these behaviors to situational fac-
physical proximity for psychological trauma, but this tors and labeling them atypical.
research focuses on the micro-geography of the areas If out-group animosity toward assailants influences
around attacks. It does not consider the political impor- threat perceptions, in-group love toward victims should
tance of macro-geographic boundaries, national borders. also matter in how we view violence and what strategies
Political scientists analyze the importance of target selec- we support to cope with violence. Psychological distance
tion for target audiences, and the attendant implications to the out-group insulates people against the pain inflicted
for public opinion and strategic communication, but fail on strangers (Skitka, Bauman, and Mullen 2004). On the
to consider a number of politically salient characteristics flip side, commonality is a conduit for pain, allowing
of terrorism victims. In the following section, we develop people to feel more empathy with strangers who share
a theory to explain the roles that personal characteristics similar traits. The hostility one feels toward a terrorist
and physical boundaries play in shaping public percep- assailant and the empathy one feels toward a terrorism
tions of terrorist threats. victim depends on one’s personal proximity to the assail-
ant and personal proximity to the victim. Personal prox-
imity is the perceived emotional and psychological
Proximity and Attitudes distance between the self and the other. Members of the
in-group are personally proximate to us. Members of the
Personal Proximity out-group are personally remote from us.
Recent terrorism research shows that perpetrators’ demo- We approximate this distance based on rough group
graphic characteristics mold our perceptions of terrorist categorizations and designations. In the event of a terrorist
violence. D’Orazio and Salehyan (2016) show that attack, people make snap judgments about the identities of
Americans are more likely to classify violent acts by the victims. While news reporting in the immediate after-
Arab Americans as terrorism than similar acts by white math of an attack may reveal “facts on the ground” about
Americans. Similarly, Huff and Kertzer (2018) demon- the number of people injured and killed (Huff and Kertzer
strate Americans are predisposed to designate violence 2018), the ethnic characteristics of the victims are not
perpetrated by Muslims as terrorism while they define the immediately evident and may remain ambiguous.
same incidents as mass murder if the perpetrators are Nationalities of victims and assailants offer rough cues
non-Muslims. Underpinning these arguments is a mecha- built on naïve beliefs about national-or-state identities.
nism whereby individuals superimpose expectations and People think of foreigners living in different countries as
beliefs about perpetrators based on their demographic having shared languages and ethnicities, despite objective
traits. Muslim and Arab perpetrators are deemed to be heterogeneity (Smith 1991; Triandafyllidou 1998).1
more predisposed to commit terrorism. These expecta- Gartzke and Gleditsch (2006) refer to differences within-
tions are based on the broader categories in which we and-among countries based on race, religion, and lan-
place perpetrators. These expectations condition policy guage as demographic markers. Because people lack a
preferences. Piazza (2015) finds that people are more tol- sophisticated understanding of demographic heterogene-
erant of tougher punitive measures against ethnically dis- ity, they draw on demographic markers to impute national
similar assailants. identities.2 For example, the average person in the United
This recent literature implicitly draws on social iden- States would regard victims of a terrorist attack in Turkey
tity theory (SIT). SIT argues that we formulate opinions as nonwhite Muslims. This identity is attributed to the
4 Political Research Quarterly 00(0)

victims despite the fact that many people in Turkey are not threatening. The consequences of these attacks are not as
Muslim and that many people in Turkey view themselves psychologically traumatizing, but the effects are politi-
as white. This contrasts with the way the average person in cally potent.
the United States would regard victims of a terrorist attack Physical proximity to the state is not the same as phys-
in France. The victims would be viewed as white and ical proximity to the self. A simple example highlights the
Christian despite the fact that France is an extremely distinction. Imagine a terrorist attack in Montreal,
diverse country. The initial shock of a terrorist attack, and Quebec. Montreal is closer to Burlington, Vermont than
resulting uncertainty, magnify the importance of these Austin, Texas. Existing research suggests that the attack
heuristics (Tversky and Kahneman 1973). would be more traumatic for people living in Burlington
People ration their emotional responses to terrorist than people living in Austin because Montreal is physi-
events through the heuristic of personal proximity. cally closer to Burlington than Austin. This may be true.
Personally proximate attacks are viewed as more threat- We argue that the attack is politically significant for peo-
ening, and personally remote attacks are viewed as less ple living in both locations. Whether one lives in Texas or
threatening. This intuition informs the first hypothesis: Vermont, Canadians are still our “neighbors” to the
north.3 The political significance of Canada’s proximity
Hypothesis 1: The personal proximity of terrorist is different from the importance of physical proximity for
attacks is positively related to the perceived threat of understanding PTSD.
terrorist attacks. There are two mechanisms that make physical proxim-
ity to one’s country important for threat perceptions. First,
there is a greater sense of familiarity with physically prox-
Physical Proximity imate countries. People are more likely to have traveled to
Physical proximity also affects threat perceptions. As dis- physically proximate locations. Consider people living in
cussed above, physical proximity is an important topic in the United States. According to the National Travel and
existing psychological research. People living near attack Tourism Office, more than half of international travelers
sites are more likely to exhibit PTSD and experience psy- from the United States in 2016 (66,960,943 travelers) vis-
chological trauma. Lowenstein et al. (2001) find that ited either Mexico (25,181,630) or Canada (12,221,768).4
traumatic events are more vivid for people living in the Polling data show that people living in the United States
immediate proximity of those events. Fischhoff et al. feel affinity toward these countries. According to Gallup,
(2003) consider the psychological consequences of direct when asked about their “overall opinion” of these two
and indirect exposure to the 9/11 attacks in New York countries in a 2017 survey, an overwhelming majority of
City. The attacks were most traumatic for people who Americans rated Canada (92%) and Mexico (64%) “very
directly experienced or witnessed the attacks. Physical favorable” or “mostly favorable.”5 Second, international
proximity in this literature refers to physical distance, terrorist attacks occurring in physically proximate coun-
measured in miles, of individuals from specific attack tries stimulate a sense of vulnerability. Borders are diffi-
sites. This physical distance is psychologically important cult to secure. This fact has played an important role in the
but is not politically significant. framing and formulation of U.S. border security policy in
Existing psychological research focuses on individual, the post 9-11 era (Ackleson 2005). People are concerned
micro-geography. We depart from this research because about terrorist groups operating in nearby countries com-
we are interested in national, macro-geography. The con- ing into the United States. If it can happen there, it can
ceptual difference falls along the same lines as the tradi- happen here. These dynamics make attacks in physically
tional micro-macro distinction in economics. The “micro” proximate countries more salient than attacks occurring in
in economics refers to the behavior of individuals while physically remote countries. This intuition informs the
the “macro” refers to the aggregate consequences of this second hypothesis:
behavior (Dopfer, Foster, and Potts 2004, 264). The com-
mon, aggregate unit of analysis in international relations is Hypothesis 2: The physical proximity of terrorist
the state. While existing psychological research empha- attacks is positively related to perceived threat of ter-
sizes the importance of physical distance from the indi- rorist attacks.
vidual for understanding psychological trauma, we argue
that the physical distance from national boundaries is Testing Proximity and Threat
important for political preferences and beliefs. Just as
people feel more at risk when attacks occur near them,
Perceptions
people will feel that their home countries, and thus them- We use two experiments to test our hypotheses. In both
selves through their home countries, are more at risk when experiments, participants were given a brief description
attacks occur in countries near their home countries. of a hypothetical terrorist attack and asked a series of
Attacks in contiguous countries should be particularly questions about the attack, their feelings about the attack,
Avdan and Webb 5

Table 1.  Experiment 1—Personal and Physical Proximity.


[Country] Terrorism
Terrorists used a bomb to destroy a bridge outside an airport in [city, country]. The blast left fifty [country] citizens dead and 123
wounded. A radical group calling themselves [group name], or [group acronym], claimed responsibility for the attack. The [group
acronym] leaders released a video calling for the national government to cut off diplomatic ties with the United States, saying, “it
is time that the U.S. government be held accountable for its imperialist policies.” They vowed to continue attacking public sites if
their demands were not met.

Proximity Location Group

Physical Personal Country City Name Acronym


Proximate Proximate Canada Brantford The Fighters for the Freedom of Canada FFC
Proximate Remote Mexico Moroleon-Uriangato Los Luchadores por la Libertad de México LLM
Remote Proximate England Exeter The Fighters for the Freedom of England FFE
Remote Remote Argentina Santa Rosa Los Luchadores por la Libertad de Argentina LLA

and themselves. Our analysis proceeds in two stages. The attack that occurs in a different country. The experimental
first experiment tests our hypotheses about personal and conditions are summarized in Table 1. A terrorist organi-
physical proximity. Our second experiment unpacks zation attacks a bridge outside of an airport in Canada,
some of the elements of personal proximity. Mexico, Argentina, or England. Fifty people are killed,
We recruited participants for each experiment using and another 123 are wounded. Canada and England are
Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk). MTurk is an online considered personally proximate, whereas Mexico and
marketplace where “task creators” (requesters) pay work- Argentina are considered personally remote. The national
ers in exchange for the completion of “Human Intelligence identities evoked by Canada and England are closer to the
Tasks” (HITs).6 We limited recruitment to voting age U.S. national identity of the United States than to the national
citizens. Some have expressed reservations about the use identities evoked by Argentina and Mexico. The racial and
of MTurk, arguing that MTurk samples are not representa- linguistic demographics of the populations of Canada and
tive of the U.S. population. This issue has been addressed England more closely resemble the population of the
by a number of studies. U.S. MTurk samples are as, or are United States. Canada and Mexico are contiguous to the
more, representative of the U.S. population in terms of United States. England and Argentina are, relatively,
age, geographic area, socioeconomic status, ethnicity, and physically remote because they do not share borders with
education level when compared with alternative recruit- the United States. We altered the city, country, and the
ment techniques (Buhrmester, Kwang, and Gosling 2011; name of the terrorist organization for the different condi-
Casler, Bickel, and Hackett 2013; Horton, Rand, and tions. The common vignette, the locations, and the names
Zeckhauser 2011; Mason and Suri 2012; Paolacci, of the terrorist organizations are given in Table 1.
Chandler, and Ipeirotis 2010). Also, as long as participants Participants responded to a short survey after being ran-
are sampled from the same group, randomization ensures domized into conditions. The dependent variable in the
that the causal inferences drawn from comparisons among analysis is threat perception. Participants were asked to
the experimental conditions are a function of the treat- rate the threat posed by the terrorist organization to the
ments and not group characteristics.7 If there are addi- United States from 0 (not threatening) to 10 (very threaten-
tional concerns about the demographic characteristics of ing). Participants were also asked a series of demographic
the samples, these concerns can be addressed by including questions—race, sex, education, political party, and so
controls in regression models to partial-out confounding on—and a number of manipulation checks to ensure that
variables. For each experiment, we conduct an analysis of they perceived the important features of the attack.
variance (ANOVA) to test the differences among the The results from the first experiment are summarized
experimental conditions and present a series of regression in Figure 2. The top panel of Figure 2 displays the
models to demonstrate the robustness of our findings to responses to the threat perception question for each of the
sample characteristics and alternative specifications. four conditions as box-plots. The thick horizontal lines in
the box-plots are the median responses, the boxes repre-
sent the interquartile ranges, and lines at the top and bot-
Personal and Physical Proximity
tom of each plot are the response maxima and minima for
In our first experiment, we manipulate personal and physi- each condition. In each case, the responses range from 0
cal proximity by randomizing participants into one of four to 10. The second panel shows summary statistics for the
conditions where they read about a hypothetical terrorist conditions and the experimental results. The first three
6 Political Research Quarterly 00(0)

Figure 2.  Personal and physical proximity.

columns of the second panel show the number of obser- Table 2.  Personal and Physical Proximity.
vations in each condition (N), the median responses
(Medians), and the mean responses (Means). The next Model 1 Model 2
five columns of the second panel give the ANOVA results. Intercept 4.830** (0.303) 4.398** (0.583)
The contributions of the two experimental factors to the Personal Prox 1.587** (0.355) 1.476** (0.333)
sum and mean squared deviations are given along with Physical Prox 0.671** (0.355) 0.722* (0.341)
the F statistics for each experimental factor. Democrat −0.146 (0.434)
The observed differences in threat perceptions are con- Independent −0.710 (0.470)
sistent with theoretical expectations. Terrorist attacks in Other −0.250 (0.806)
personally proximate countries are viewed as more threat- Female 1.712** (0.337)
ening than attacks in personally remote countries, and ter- White −0.293 (0.610)
rorist attacks in physically proximate countries are viewed Millennial 0.070 (0.353)
as more threatening than attacks in physically remote Income 0.892* (0.372)
countries, but only the F statistic for the personal proxim- College −1.630** (0.355)
ity experimental factor reaches the α = .05 level of statisti- News 0.312 (0.380)
Adj. R2 .072 .212
cal significance. The ordinary least squares regression
N 306 306
(OLS) results presented in Table 2 show the main effects
presented in Table 1 are not driven by the demographic Standard errors in parentheses. Personal Prox: Canada and England =
features of the sample. Model 1 shows the results from the 1. Physical Prox: Canada and Mexico = 1.
regression of the threat perception variable on the experi- *p < .05. **p < .01.
mental factors, personal and physical proximity. Model 2
includes a number of demographic controls. Democrat, otherwise. Finally, News is coded 1 if the participant
Independent, and Other are variables included to capture reports watching or reading news for seven or more hours
the partisanship of the respondent. Each is coded 1 for par- a week.8 This final variable is used to differentiate between
ticipants who identified with each political group and 0 high and low information participants.
otherwise. Republicans are the omitted category. Female The results presented in Table 2 are consistent with the
is coded 1 if the participant identifies as a woman and zero results presented in Figure 2. The coefficient for personal
otherwise. White is coded 1 if the participant identifies proximity is positive and statistically significant in both
their race as white and 0 otherwise. Millennial is coded 1 models; the coefficient for physical proximity is positive
if the participant falls into an age group of either eighteen in both models but only reaches the α = .05 level of statis-
to twenty-five or twenty-six to thirty-five, and 0 other- tical significance in model 2. The demographic controls
wise. Income is coded 1 if the participant reports an presented in Table 2 are consistent with previous research.
income above $35,000 and 0 otherwise. College is coded Republicans in our sample, on average, gave higher eval-
1 if the participant has at least a bachelor’s degree and 0 uations of the threat posed by the different terrorist groups
Avdan and Webb 7

Figure 3.  Country comparisons and physical proximity.

compared with Democrats, Independents, or people who second panel compares the distributions of the physically
identified their political affiliations as “Other.” Women proximate and physically remote countries, and the third
and people with higher incomes, on average, perceived panel shows t tests for the comparisons among the experi-
the threats posed by the terrorist organizations as higher mental conditions and the experimental factors. The black
compared with men and people with lower incomes.9 The lines in the top two panels are the physically proximate
coefficient on College is negative and statistically signifi- countries. The gray lines are the physically remote coun-
cant. This is consistent with research showing that people tries. The solid lines in the top panel are the personally
with college degrees tend to feel less anxiety toward out- proximate countries, and the dashed lines are the person-
groups (Mansfield and Mutz 2009). Race and Age were ally remote countries. The vertical lines in panel 1 are the
not significant determinants of threat perceptions in this means associated with the experimental conditions. The
sample. vertical lines in panel 2 are the means associated with the
The results for physical proximity appear mixed. The physically proximate and physically remote countries.
mean and median responses are higher for the physically The patterns in Figure 3 illustrate why the results in the
proximate conditions compared with the physically models presented above appear ambiguous. Comparing
remote conditions, and the coefficients are in the expected the physically proximate countries to the physically
direction in both regression models. However, the effect remote countries, the differences between the means are
of physical proximity is only significant at the α = .05 relatively small. The difference in means between Canada
level in the second regression model. Although this could and England is less than one, and the difference in means
be considered as evidence against the physical proximity between Mexico and Argentina is less than one. This
hypothesis, a careful examination of the results reveals explains why the t tests for these comparisons shown in
that the F test for physical proximity and the coefficient panel 3 are small. However, there are clear differences in
for physical proximity in model 1 are significant at α = .1. the four distributions. The highest density region of the
This is sufficient evidence to suggest the physical prox- Canada distribution is greater than the highest density
imity hypothesis should not be dismissed entirely. region of the England distribution, and the highest density
Figure 3 unpacks the effects of physical proximity. The region of the Mexico distribution is greater than the high-
top panel of Figure 3 shows the distributions of the est density region of the Argentina distribution.10 The
responses across the four experimental conditions, the effects of physical proximity are clearer in the second
8 Political Research Quarterly 00(0)

Table 3.  Experiment 2—Racial and Religious Proximity.


Terrorism in [Eastern Europe/Sub-Saharan Africa]
Terrorists detonated twin bombs in the city of [city], [country] yesterday. One in a large [religious building], another in a nearby
market. The blasts left fifty [country] citizens dead and 123 wounded. A radical group calling themselves the [group name] ([group
acronym]) claimed responsibility for the attack. The FFR leaders released a video calling for the national government to cut off
diplomatic ties with the United States, saying, “it is time that the U.S. government be held accountable for its imperialist policies.”
They vowed to continue attacking public sites if their demands were not met.

Proximity Location Group

Racial Religious Country City Building Name Acronym


Proximate Proximate Romania Buzau Church The Fighters for the Freedom of Romania FFR
Proximate Remote Albania Elbasan Mosque The Fighters for the Freedom of Albania FFA
Remote Proximate Zambia Kasama Church The Fighters for the Freedom of Zambia FFZ
Remote Remote Guinea Kankan Mosque The Fighters for the Freedom of Guinea FFG

panel. There is a larger difference in the means, and the The countries were chosen based on their major national
contrast is apparent in the trends. The black line (physi- religions and the major racial ethnic groups. The experi-
cally proximate countries) increases from left to right, and mental conditions are summarized in Table 3.
the gray line (physically remote countries) decreases from The level of personal proximity to the United States
left to right. The t-statistic for the difference between the varies across the four countries. A country is considered
physically proximate mean and the physically remote racially proximate to the United States if the major racial
mean is 1.83. This is significant at the α = .1 level. In total, ethnic group is white. Romania and Albania are majority
we view the evidence presented here as consistent with white countries. Zambia and Guinea are majority non-
Hypothesis 1, but the effects of physical proximity are far white countries. A country is considered religiously prox-
less cogent than the effects of personal proximity. imate to the United States if the major religious ethnic
The results from the first experiment provide support for group is Christian. Romania and Zambia are majority
our arguments about personal and physical proximity, but Christian countries. Albania and Guinea are majority
there are additional issues that can be considered. Our Muslim countries. We highlight the difference between
experimental factors vary the location, language, race, and Christian and Muslim countries in the vignettes by allow-
colonial heritage of the victims. Existing research has ing one of the attack locations to be a church or a mosque.
emphasized another important element of identity that Race and religion are two of the major demographic
affects threat perceptions, religion. Huff and Kertzer (2018) markers that distinguish people living in France from peo-
show that violent acts by Muslims are more likely to be ple living in Kenya and Turkey. Did people care less about
labeled terrorism. A related finding suggests that white these attacks in Kenya because the victims were not white?
Americans are more likely to label Arab Americans as ter- Did people care less about the attacks in Turkey because
rorists in the wake of violence compared with other ethnic Turkey is a majority Muslim country? Are both dynamics
minorities (D’Orazio and Salehyan 2016). Do race and reli- at work? Our personal proximity argument suggests that
gion also affect the empathy people feel toward victims? people impute the characteristics of the victims based on
Our next experiment extends our argument about the per- the location of the attack, process their affinity for the vic-
sonal proximity of terrorist attacks along these dimensions, tims, and then evaluate the seriousness of the attack.
the imputed race and religion of terrorism victims. The results from the second experiment are presented
in Figure 4. The top panel of Figure 4 summarizes the
responses to the threat perception question across the four
Racial and Religious Proximity conditions. The second panel of Figure 4 shows the sum-
In our second experiment, we considered the importance mary statistics and ANOVA results. Racial proximity has
of two cultural markers for the relationship between per- a statistically significant effect on threat perceptions. The
sonal proximity and threat perceptions, religious proxim- attacks in Romania and Albania were perceived as more
ity and racial proximity. Again, participants were threatening than the attacks in Zambia and Guinea. This
randomized into one of four conditions where they read is consistent with the results from experiment 1. Race
about hypothetical terrorist attacks that occurred in dif- matters. Surprisingly, the religion of the victims did not
ferent countries. Terrorists detonated two bombs in dif- have a statistically significant influence on threat percep-
ferent locations. One bomb is detonated in a market. The tions. In fact, the attacks in Albania were perceived as the
other bomb is detonated in either a church or a mosque. most threatening among all the conditions. These results
Avdan and Webb 9

Figure 4.  Racial and religious proximity.

Table 4.  Racial and Religious Proximity.

Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6


Intercept 4.943** (0.348) 4.713** (0.639) 5.338** (1.325) 4.001** (1.234)
Religious Prox −0.292 (0.361) −0.336 (0.353) 1.173 (0.780) 1.027 (0.709)
Racial Prox 0.842* (0.365) 0.829* (0.358) 0.883 (0.726) 2.222** (0.667)
Democrat −0.501 (0.471) −0.013 (1.100) −1.443 (0.936)
Independent −0.811 (0.501) −0.922 (1.131) −2.462* (1.030)
Other −1.832* (0.908) −3.211 (3.487)  
Female 1.351** (0.352) 1.396* (0.701) 2.309** (0.691)
White −0.754 (0.776) −2.923* (1.287) 0.782 (1.368)
Millennial 0.298 (0.362) −0.329 (0.753) 0.404 (0.708)
Income 0.531 (0.377) 0.503 (0.783) 0.631 (0.685)
College −0.590 (0.371) −1.581** (0.788) −0.467 (0.705)
News −0.099 (0.394) −1.306* (0.774) −0.554 (0.713)
Adj. R2 .021 .102 .194 .371
N 312 312 94 78

Standard errors in parentheses. Religious Prox: Romania and Zambia = 1. Racial Prox: Romania and Albania = 1.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

suggest race matters more than religion. The regression of the victims (model 5) and the race of the victims
results presented in Table 4 corroborate these findings. (model 6). We wanted to see whether the observed effects
Table 4 presents estimates from four OLS regression were influenced by the inability of the participants to per-
models. The first two models show the main effects and ceive the differences among the countries. Participants
the main effects controlling for demographic factors. The might know that Guinea and Zambia are African coun-
effect of racial proximity is statistically significant, even tries while Albania and Romania were European, but they
after controlling for the political affiliation, sex, race, age, might not know the religious profiles of the countries
income, education, and news consumption of the partici- despite our attempt to frame the news about the victims.
pants. The control variables are consistent with the previ- The attrition in the samples is consistent with this expec-
ous regression results. The signs of the coefficients do not tation. Only 30 percent of participants were able to cor-
change, but only the effect of sex (Female) on threat per- rectly identify the religion of the victims (model 5), and
ceptions meets conventional levels of significance. only 25 percent of the participants were able to correctly
The next two models select observations based on identify the race of the victims (model 6). Limiting the
whether the participant accurately identified the religion sample to participants who were able to correctly
10 Political Research Quarterly 00(0)

perceive the differences among victims did not change white. This pattern manifests in our experimental data as
the inferences from the previous regressions, but there well. In addition to the regression analyses presented in
were interesting changes in the sizes of the effects. Table 4, we conducted a supplementary analysis where
The coefficient for racial proximity increases by 170 we restricted the sample by race. We found that the effect
percent when the sample is limited to people who cor- of racial proximity was most acute in the sample of white
rectly identified the race of the victims. The effect of participants (245 of 312) who correctly identified the race
racial proximity is 0.829 in model 4 and increases to of the victims (sixty of 245). Racial proximity had no
2.222 in model 6. This change buttresses the support for effect for the nonwhite participants.11 Given the predilec-
the inference that racial proximity affects threat percep- tion of the racial majority, it is not surprising to see news
tions. When people were able to differentiate the victims reporting and national conversations skewed toward cov-
based on race, race mattered more! A similar change hap- erage of white victims. There appears to be a “rally
pens in the case of religious proximity. The sign of the around our race” effect. This fixation on white tragedy
coefficient changes from negative to positive, consistent has important political implications.
with the theoretical expectation, but the coefficient does Not all terrorist attacks move the public opinion nee-
not reach conventional levels of significance. The reli- dle. Informing the public is costly. Politicians are willing
gion of the victims does not appear to have an indepen- to bear these costs for attacks on white people but are not
dent effect on affinity for victims. We explore the for attacks on nonwhite people. This implies varying lev-
implications of this finding in more detail in the follow- els of political utility during campaigns and elections. In
ing section. 2016, the terrorist attacks in Brussels, Belgium and Nice,
France received considerable attention in both U.S. presi-
dential primaries (Lee 2016; Tatum 2016). Similar attacks
Discussion and Conclusion in Istanbul, Turkey; Maiduguri, Nigeria; Ziltan, Libya;
We began with a discussion of the 2015 attacks in Garissa, and Lahore, Pakistan were ignored. These differences
Kenya, Ankara, Turkey, and Paris France. The attacks also matter during nonelection years. Future research
used similar methods, were carried out by similar groups, should consider how these differences influence the will-
and had similar death tolls but garnered disparate public ingness of people to accept potentially expensive and
responses. People cared more about the attacks in France invasive counterterrorism policies.
than the attacks in Turkey and Kenya. We believe the per- These findings also have normative implications. Race
sonal and physical proximities of the attacks explain this should not govern our empathy. Terrorist violence occurs
“empathy gap.” We tested these expectations using two every day, but we only change our Facebook profiles and
online survey experiments. hold public vigils when it happens in predominately white
Personal proximity is determined by the affinity peo- countries to predominately white people. These signs of
ple feel toward victims of attacks. We tested this proposi- solidarity may be cathartic, but focusing on the suffering
tion in multiple contexts. In experiment 1, we tested this of predominately white populations is costly. Depicting
hypothesis by varying the location of the attack. Attacks terrorist attacks in Brussels and Paris as unique normal-
against countries that had more overlapping demographic izes terrorist attacks that occur in other countries. If these
markers with the United States (Canada and England) depictions fall along racial or cultural lines, then these
were perceived as more threatening than attacks against depictions normalize violence against racial and cultural
other states. In experiment 2, we unpack some of these groups. This generates an indifference to suffering that has
demographic markers, race and religion. The United important social and political consequences. In domestic
States is a majority white and majority Christian country. settings, this manifests as indifference to the suffering of
Attacks against white countries (Romania and Albania) minority groups. Inequality and violence against minority
were perceived as more threatening than attacks against populations become more acceptable to dominant cultural
nonwhite countries (Zambia and Guinea). There are a groups. In international settings, this manifests as callous
number of theoretically relevant findings that can be dis- foreign policies. Western countries refuse to accept refu-
tilled from these results. gees from war-torn societies because of the small risk that
First, race matters. In the aftermath of an attack, peo- refugees will commit acts of terrorism. This process of
ple do not know the racial characteristics of the victims, normalization creates zones of indifference—domesti-
but people impute these characteristics and assess the cally and internationally—that perpetuate poverty, vio-
severity of threats. People do not care as much about peo- lence, and suffering.
ple who do not look like them. Of course, not all American Second, religion does not seem to matter. Religious
are white. The empathy gap reflects the sentiments of the proximity is not statistically significant in the ANOVA or
cultural majority. Americans, on average, cared more the regression models. Limiting the sample to people who
about the attacks in Paris because most Americans are correctly identified the religion of the victims did not
Avdan and Webb 11

Table 5.  Identifying Racial and Religious Proximity.

Full Religion Race

Country Religion Race X N X N ∆ X N ∆


Romania Christian White 4.63 81 5.68 16 +1.05 6.44 18 +1.81
Albania Muslim White 4.67 97 3.82 41 0.85 4.80 21 +0.13
Zambia Christian Nonwhite 3.51 80 3.92 13 +0.41 3.35 28 0.16
Guinea Muslim Nonwhite 4.15 54 4.00 24 0.15 3.54 11 0.61

alter the inferences from the models. Although this find- suggest that an effect might be observed in a larger sample
ing was not consistent with our expectations, the null or in a sample where there is less attrition. It might be
result does not mean that religion is irrelevant. easier for participants to identity Iraq and Iran as “Muslim
Table 5 shows the attrition rates in the sample for the countries” compared with Albania and Guinea. Second,
second experiment when the sample is limited to partici- these patterns highlight valuable avenues for future
pants who correctly identified the religion and race of the research. Which countries have national identities that are
victims. The countries and each country’s dominant race easier, or more difficult, to identify? Which characteristics
and religion are listed along the vertical dimension of the of those identities are most salient? When, if ever, do the
table. The horizontal dimension of the table displays the religious characteristics of the victims matter? How, if at
average responses ( X ) and sample sizes (N) associated all, are racial and religious identities of victims related in
with the full sample (Full) and the samples that are these processes?
restricted to people who correctly identified the religions Physical proximity is determined by location. People
of the victims (Religion) and the races of the victims care more about international attacks that occur in neigh-
(Race). Table 5 also shows the changes in the means asso- boring countries. We tested this proposition in experi-
ciated with the restrictions (Δ). ment 1 by manipulating the country where the attack
Restricting the sample to individuals who correctly occurred, countries that border the United States, and
identified the religions and races of the victims causes countries that do not. Physical proximity was a signifi-
substantial sample attrition. The number of participants cant predictor of threat perceptions. There are different
in each category falls by, roughly, 60 to 80 percent in ways to view the substantive significance of the physical
many of the conditions. The results do not change in sub- proximity result. On one hand, this result is not surpris-
stantively meaningful ways, so there is no concern that ing. Existing research shows that attacks are more trau-
this attrition affects the inferences from the models, but matic for people in the immediate vicinities of attacks.
the patterns in the changes are illuminating. On the other hand, this result has important implications
The changes in the means caused by the attrition are for thinking about the geography of terrorist attacks. We
consistent with our personal proximity hypothesis. When differ from existing research by focusing on the role of
the sample is limited only to people who correctly iden- physical distance in perceptions of terrorist events in for-
tify the religions of the victims, the mean threat percep- eign countries. Our results qualify the effects of distance
tions increase for Christian victims and decrease for in two important ways. First, our results show that conti-
Muslim victims. When the sample is limited only to peo- guity heightens threat perceptions. Existing research
ple who correctly identify the races of the victims, the treats distance as linear because it focuses on the micro-
mean threat perceptions increase for white victims and geography of terrorist events. People in Manhattan were
decrease for nonwhite victims. The changes in the means more traumatized by 9/11 than people in Minneapolis.
are larger for the subsample of participants able to iden- Our result suggests a more politically important macro-
tify the race, but the differences still exist. geographical perspective. Distance matters for trauma
These patterns have two important implications. First, and threat perceptions, but borders matter too. Attacks
religion might matter in other circumstances. We deliber- occurring near one’s borders create a greater sense of vul-
ately chose countries that have not played an important nerability. Second, our results also suggest a “rally around
role in U.S. politics. We wanted to avoid the possibility of the neighborhood” effect. Other things equal, people
pre-conceived attitudes about the countries, unrelated to cared more about attacks against the immediate neigh-
national identities, driving the results. Although we avoid bors of the United States—Canada and Mexico—than
this risk, the rate of attrition illustrates that it is harder for attacks in more distant countries. Although these results
people to impute the racial and religious characteristics of are meaningful, we should qualify these results by saying
the victims when they know less about the countries. that our study only suggests that physical proximity mat-
This makes the test conservative. The patterns in the data ters for contiguous countries. More research is necessary
12 Political Research Quarterly 00(0)

to discern the salience of physical proximity across other  5. http://news.gallup.com/poll/1624/perceptions-foreign-


spatial demarcations, for example, regional proximity or countries.aspx
continental proximity. The results also suggest other   6. Participants were randomly assigned into conditions and
fruitful avenues for future research considering physical were compensated ($0.40) once the task was completed.
The three experiments were conducted between October
proximity and political salience in the domestic context.
2015 and February 2016, each experiment took approxi-
We know that people living in the vicinity of attacks are
mately ten minutes to complete.
more traumatized by attacks. Do the locations of domes-   7. Randomization for the experiments was automated through
tic attacks affect the perceived salience of attacks as well? Qualtrics, a web-based survey platform that includes tools
Are attacks against cities perceived as more threatening for designing experiments and managing data.
than attacks in remote areas? Are attacks against sym-   8. The use of covariates in the analysis of experimental data
bolic or economically important targets perceived as is somewhat controversial. The experimental factors are
more threatening than alternative soft targets? exogenous by design. Some argue that this makes con-
The defining feature of terrorism as a political strategy trol variables unnecessary in experimental analyses. We
extends beyond the physical damage caused by terrorist are agnostic about the use of control variables in experi-
attacks. Terrorism has a psychological dimension. It is mental analyses. Control variables are not used in models
1 and 3 or the analysis of variance (ANOVA) models.
meant to incite fear. The results from this study illustrate
The inferences from the models are consistent across
that psychological consequences of terrorism depend on
specifications.
more than tactics or organizational traits. The environ-  9. This is consistent with the work of Huddy, Khatib, and
mental context of the attack and the attack characteristics Capelos (2002).
affect threat perceptions. We gain a more vivid grasp of 10. The Argentina distribution appears bi-modal, but the line is
the consequences of transnational terrorism, and the higher around 0.
potential political implications, when we consider the 11. These results are presented in the online supplemental
locations of attacks and the identities of the victims. materials, Section 2.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests Supplemental Materials


The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with Supplemental materials for this article are available with the man-
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this uscript on the Political Research Quarterly (PRQ) website.
article.
References
Funding Abrahms, Max. 2007. “Why Democracies Make Superior
Counterterrorists.” Security Studies 16 (2): 223–53.
The authors received financial support for the research from the
Abrahms, Max. 2012. “The Political Efficacy of Terrorism
University of Kansas Department of Political Science.
Revisited.” Comparative Political Studies 45 (3): 366–93.
Ackleson, Jason. 2005. “Constructing Security on the U.S.–
Notes Mexico Border.” Political Geography 24 (2): 165–84.
 1. The terms nation, state, and nation-state have distinct Allouche, Jeremy, and Jeremy Lind. 2010. “Public Attitudes to
meanings. A nation is a group of people with a common Global Uncertainties: A Research Synthesis Exploring the
heritage, history, or culture. A state is a territory that is Trends and Gaps in Knowledge.” https://esrc.ukri.org/files/
recognized as an organized political community. A nation- public-engagement/public-dialogues/full-report-public-
state is a sovereign country whose citizens or participants attitudes-to-global-uncertainties/ (accessed May 14, 2018).
are relatively homogeneous. The latter definition is most Arreguin-Toft, Ivan. 2001. “How the Weak Win Wars: A
relevant for our argument. Although the populations of Theory of Asymmetric Conflict.” International Security 26
most states are not homogeneous, people impute state (1): 93–128.
identities based on the numerically or politically dominant Becker, Gary, and Yona Rubinstein. 2004. Fear and the
ethnic, religious, and/or religious grouping (Gartzke and Response to Terrorism: An Economic Analysis. University
Gleditsch 2006). of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL.
 2. Some may attach a higher weight to race or religion, Buhrmester, Michael, Tracy Kwang, and Samuel D. Gosling.
whereas others may weight these equally. We have no a 2011. “Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A New Source of
priori reason to expect people to attach differential weights Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality, Data?” Perspectives on
to these categories. Psychological Science 6 (1): 3–5.
  3. President Bill Clinton famously said that Canada and the Burke, Peter J. 1991. “Identity Processes and Social Stress.”
United States are “neighbors by the grace of nature” in a American Sociological Review 56:836–49.
speech he gave to the Canadian Parliament in February of Casler, Krista, Lydia Bickel, and Elizabeth Hackett. 2013.
1995. See http://www.presidency.ucsb.edu/ws/?pid=51017 “Separate but Equal? A Comparison of Participants and
for the full address. Data Gathered via Amazon’s MTurk, Social Media, and
 4. https://thepointsguy.com/2017/01/record-number-of- Face-to-Face Behavioral Testing.” Computers in Human
americans-traveled-abroad-2016/ Behavior 29 (6): 2156–60.
Avdan and Webb 13

Chandler, Adam, Krishnadev Calamur, and Matt Ford. Hetherington, Marc J., and Elizabeth Suhay. 2011.
2015. “The Paris Attacks: The Latest.” The Atlantic, “Authoritarianism, Threat, and Americans’ Support for the
November 22. http://www.theatlantic.com/international/ War on Terror.” American Journal of Political Science 55
archive/2015/11/paris-attacks/415953/ (accessed: October (3): 546–60.
31, 2016). Hogg, Michael A., and Dominic Abrams. 1988. Social
Cohen, Silver R., E. Alison Holman, Daniel N. McIntosh, Identifications: A Social Psychology of Intergroup
Michael Poulin, and Virginia Gil-Rivas. 2002. “Nationwide Relations and Group Processes. London: Routledge.
Longitudinal Study of Psychological Responses to Horton, John J., David G. Rand, and Richard J. Zeckhauser.
September 11.” Journal American Medical Association 2011. “The Online Laboratory: Conducting Experiments
288:1235–44. in a Real Labor Market.” Experimental Economics 14 (3):
Crenshaw, Martha. 1986. “The Psychology of Political 399–425.
Terrorism.” In Political Psychology, edited by Margaret G. Huddy, Leonie, Standley Feldman, Charles Taber, and
Hermann, 379–413. New York: Jossey-Bass. Gallya Lahav. 2005. “Threat, Anxiety, and Support for
Davis, Christopher G., and Stephanie L. Macdonald. 2004. Antiterrorism Policies.” American Journal of Political
“Threat Appraisals, Distress and the Development of Science 49 (3): 593–608.
Positive Life Changes after September 11th in a Canadian Huddy, Leonie, Nadia Khatib, and Theresa Capelos. 2002.
Sample.” Cognitive Behavioral Therapy 33 (2): 68–78. “Trends: Reactions to the Terrorist Attacks of September
Davis, Darren W., and Brian D. Silver. 2004. “Civil Liberties 11, 2001.” Public Opinion Quarterly 66 (3): 418–50.
vs. Security: Public Opinion in the Context of the Terrorist Huff, Connor, and Joshua Kertzer. 2018. “How the Public
Attacks on America.” American Journal of Political Defines Terrorism.” American Journal of Political Science
Science 48 (1): 28–46. 62 (1): 55–71.
Dearden, Lizzie. 2015. “Ankara Explosions: Video Captures Jacobson, Gary C. 2003. “Terror, Terrain, and Turnout:
Moment Bomb Goes Off as Singing Protesters Call Explaining the 2002 Midterm Elections.” Political Science
for Peace.” The Independent, October 10. http://www. Quarterly 118 (1): 1–22.
independent.co.uk/news/world/europe/ankara-explo- Jenkins, Brian Michael. 1980. The Study of Terrorism:
sions-video-captures-moment-bomb-goes-off-as-sing- Definitional Problems (No. P-6563). Santa Monica: RAND
ing-protesters-call-for-peace-a6688811.html (accessed Corporation.
October 31, 2016). Jenkins, Brian. 1981. “The Psychological Implications of Media-
Dopfer, Kurt, John Foster, and Jason Potts. 2004. “Micro- Covered Terrorism.” Report for the Rand Corporation, Santa
Meso-Macro.” Journal of Evolutionary Economics 14 (3): Monica, CA. June.
263–79. Kunreuther, Howard. 2002. “Risk Analysis and Risk Management
D’Orazio, Vito, and Idean Salehyan. 2016. “Who Is a Terrorist? in an Uncertain World.” Risk Analysis 22:655–64.
Ethnicity, Group Affiliation, and Understandings of Kydd, Andrew H., and Barbara F. Walter. 2006. “The Strategies
Political Violence.” Paper presented at the Peace Science of Terrorism.” International Security 31 (1): 49–80.
Conference, South Bend, October, 2016. Lee, Esther. 2016. “Hillary Clinton, Donald Trump Respond
Enders, Walter, and Todd Sandler. 2006. The Political Economy to Brussels Terror Attacks.” US Weekly, March, 22.
of Terrorism. New York: Cambridge University Press. http://www.usmagazine.com/celebrity-news/news/hill-
Fischhoff, Baruch, Roxana M. Gonzalez, Deborah A. Small, ary-clinton-donald-trump-respond-to-brussels-terror-at-
and Jennifer S. Lerner. 2003. “Judged Terror Risk and tacks-w199800 (accessed November 1, 2016).
Proximity to the World Trade Center.” Journal of Risk and Letsch, Constanze, and Nadia Khomami. 2015. “Turkey Terror
Uncertainty 26 (2/3): 137–51. Attack: Mourning after Scores Killed in Ankara Blasts.”
Galea, Sandro, Jennifer Ahern, Heidi Resnick, Dean Kilpatrick, The Guardian, October 11. https://www.theguardian.com/
Michael Bucuvalus, Joel Gold, and David Vlahvov. 2002. world/2015/oct/10/turkey-suicide-bomb-killed-in-ankara
“Psychological Sequelae of the September 11 Terrorist (accessed October 31, 2016).
Attacks in New York City.” New England Journal of Levs, Joshua, and Holly Yan. 2015. “147 Dead, Islamist
Medicine 346 (13): 982–87. Gunmen Killed after Attack at Kenya College.” CNN, April
Gartzke, Erik, and Kristian S. Gleditsch. 2006. “Identity and 2. http://www.cnn.com/2015/04/02/africa/kenya-univer-
Conflict: Ties that Bind and Differences that Divide.” sity-attack/ (accessed October 31, 2016).
European Journal of International Relations 12 (1): 53–87. Linville, Patricia W., Gregory W. Fischer, and Peter Salovey.
Gicobi, Maryanne. 2015. “Paris Attack Revives Debate on Garissa 1989. “Perceived Distributions of the Characteristics of
University Terror Raid.” Daily Nation, November 18. http:// In-Group and Out-Group Members: Empirical Evidence
www.nation.co.ke/news/Garissa-vs-Paris-debate/1056- and a Computer Simulation.” Journal of Personality and
2961082-n9pc4y/index.html (accessed October 31, 2016). Social Psychology 57 (2): 165–88.
Graham, David A. 2015. “The Empathy Gap between Paris Lowenstein, George F., Elke U. Weber, Christopher K. Hsee,
and Beirut. Why Have Attacks in France Gotten So and Ned Welch. 2001. “Risk as Feelings.” Psychological
Much More Attention than Bombings in Lebanon?” The Bulletin 127 (2): 267–86.
Atlantic, November 16. https://www.theatlantic.com/inter- Mansfield, Edward, and Diana Mutz. 2009. “Support for Free
national/archive/2015/11/paris-beirut-terrorism-empathy- Trade: Self-Interest, Sociotropic Politics, and Out-Group
gap/416121/ (accessed October 18, 2016). Anxiety.” International Organization 63 (3): 425–457.
14 Political Research Quarterly 00(0)

Mason, Winter, and Siddharth Suri. 2012. “Conducting https://www.theguardian.com/world/2015/nov/16/paris-


Behavioral Research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk.” attacks-global-solidarity-nigeria-kenya (accessed October
Behavioral Research Methods 44 (1): 1–23. 31, 2016).
Melvin, Don. 2015. “At Least 95 Killed in Twin Bombings Skitka, Linda J., Christopher W. Bauman, and Elizabeth Mullen.
Near Train Station in Turkey’s Capital.” CNN, October 2004. “Political Tolerance and Coming to Psychological
10. http://www.cnn.com/2015/10/10/middleeast/turkey- Closure Following the September 11, 2001, Terrorist
ankara-bomb-blast/ (accessed October 31, 2016). Attacks: An Integrative Approach.” Personality and Social
Mullen, Jethro, Don Melvin, and Paul Armstrong. 2015. “Terror Psychology Bulletin 30 (6): 743–56.
in Paris: What We Know So Far.” CNN, November 15. Smith, Anthony D. 1991. National Identity. Reno: University
http://www.cnn.com/2015/11/13/europe/paris-attacks-at-a- of Nevada Press.
glance/ (accessed October 31, 2016). Stockman, Farah. 2015. “Why We Care More about Paris.”
Paolacci, Gabriele, Jesse Chandler, and Panagiotis Ipeirotis. The Boston Globe, November 18. https://www.boston-
2010. “Running Experiments on Amazon Mechanical globe.com/opinion/2015/11/18/why-care-more-about-
Turk.” Judgment and Decision Making 5 (5): 411–19. paris/kzjWdI6lw4SVtmBM0GgtxN/story.html (accessed
Pape, Robert A. 2003. “The Strategic Logic of Suicide October 31, 2016).
Terrorism.” American Political Science Review 97 (3): Tajfel, Henri. 1981. Human Groups and Social Categories.
343–61. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Piazza, James A. 2015. “Terrorist Suspect Religious Identity Tajfel, Henri. 1982. “Social Psychology of Intergroup
and Public Support for Harsh Interrogation and Detention Relations.” Annual Review of Psychology 31:1–39.
Practices.” Political Psychology 36:667–90. doi:10.1111/ Tajfel, Henri, and John C. Turner. 1979. “An integrative
pops.12190. theory of intergroup conflict.” The Social Psychology of
Schlenger, William E., Juesta M. Caddell, Lori Ebert, B. Intergroup Relations 33 (47): 74.
Kathleen Jordan, Kathryn M. Rourke, David Wilson, Lisa Tajfel, Henri, and John C. Turner. 1986. “The Social Identity
Thalji, J. Michael Dennis, John A. Fairbank, and Richard Theory of Intergroup Behavior.” In Psychology of
A Kulka. 2002. “Psychological Reactions to Terrorist Intergroup Relations, edited by Stephen Worchel and
Attacks: Findings from the National Study of Americans’ William G. Austin, 7–24. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Reactions to September 11.” Journal of American Medical Tatum, Sophie. 2016. “France Attack Highlights Differences
Association 288 (5): 581–88. in Trump, Clinton Terror Responses.” CNN Politics, July
Schuster, Mark A., Bradley D. Stein, Lisa H. Jaycox, Rebecca 15. http://www.cnn.com/2016/07/14/politics/donald-trump-
L. Collins, Grant N. Marshall, Marc N. Elliott, Annie J. hillary-clinton-nice-france-response/ (accessed November
Zhou, David E. Kanouse, Janina L. Morrison, and Sandra H 1, 2016).
Berry. 2001. “A National Survey of Stress Reactions after Triandafyllidou, Anna. 1998. “National Identity and the
the September 11, 2001, Terrorist Attacks.” New England ‘Other.’” Ethnic and Racial Studies 21 (4): 593–612.
Journal of Medicine 345 (20): 1507–12. Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1973. “Availability:
Shearlaw, Maeve. 2015. “Paris Attacks: How Far Does Our A Heuristic for Judging Frequency and Probability.”
Global Solidarity Extend?” The Guardian, November 16. Cognitive Psychology 5 (2): 207–32.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai