Anda di halaman 1dari 18

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2019, 52, 337–354 NUMBER 2 (SPRING)

An analysis of treatment integrity of response interruption and


redirection
CANDICE L. COLÓN
WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY THE MAY INSTITUTE, MAY CENTER SCHOOL FOR AUTISM AND
DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

WILLIAM H. AHEARN
THE NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY

Response interruption and redirection has been shown to effectively decrease stereotypy, but its
application outside an experimental setting has not been well studied. In Experiment 1, decreases
in automatically maintained vocal stereotypy were obtained following treatment in a controlled
setting for 3 participants diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Descriptive data on the con-
sistency and accuracy of response interruption and redirection were then collected in the class-
room setting. Results showed that the consistency of treatment implementation varied across
participants and staff members. Failure to implement the treatment was the most common
error. However, when response interruption and redirection was implemented, the components
were generally carried out as prescribed. In Experiment 2, we conducted a parametric analysis in
a controlled setting to test the impact of consistency errors on response interruption and redirec-
tion. The results indicated that response interruption and redirection was generally effective at
50% treatment implementation or higher. Furthermore, we observed treatment effects when
25% implementation sessions were interspersed with 100% treatment implementation sessions.
Application of response interruption and redirection in light of previous studies and clinical
implications are discussed.
Key words: automatic reinforcement, parametric analysis, response interruption and redirec-
tion, stereotypy, treatment consistency, treatment integrity

Response interruption and redirection behavior and redirecting to appropriate


(RIRD) is a variant of response blocking that responses (Ahearn, Clark, MacDonald, &
has been used to treat automatically maintained Chung, 2007). Martinez and Betz (2013)
stereotypic behavior. In general, RIRD is a reviewed eight RIRD studies published in the
multiple component procedure that entails Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis from 2007
interrupting each instance of the target through 2012 and concluded that RIRD con-
sistently produced a decrease in automatically
This paper was submitted to the Department of Psy- maintained stereotypic behavior.
chology in the School of Arts and Sciences at Western
New England University by the first author under the Likewise, a meta-analysis conducted by Van-
supervision of the second author in partial fulfillment of derkerken, Heyvaert, Maes, and Onghena
the requirements for a doctoral degree in behavior analy- (2013) indicated that single-case studies for
sis. We extend appreciation to Gregory Hanley, Amanda
Karsten, and Eileen Roscoe for their feedback on earlier treating automatically maintained stereotypic
versions of this manuscript. We also thank Nicole Barlow, behavior (including several RIRD studies pub-
Brittany Bolin, Nicole Lewis, Christina Mule, Veronique lished from 2007 through 2011) produced clin-
Parent, and Ashley Wallace for their help with data collec-
tion and interobserver agreement. The first author is now ically and statistically significant effects when
at Behavioral Concepts (BCI). interventions included both skill training
Address correspondence to: Candice Colón-Kwedor, (e.g., teaching appropriate replacement behav-
Behavioral Concepts, 345 Greenwood Street, Worcester,
MA 01607 (email: ccolonkwedor@bciaba.org) ior) and decelerative procedures. Two studies
doi: 10.1002/jaba.537 not included in this meta-analysis (due to their
© 2019 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
337
338 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

publication dates) compared interventions with In addition, past RIRD studies have been con-
and without an RIRD component. For ducted in controlled settings; therefore, the
instance, Colón, Ahearn, Clark, and Masalsky extent to which RIRD is effective and practical
(2012) directly introduced verbal operant train- in nonexperimental settings has not been thor-
ing to increase appropriate vocalizations and oughly examined (Martinez & Betz, 2013).
decrease vocal stereotypy. Although appropriate Treatment integrity has been defined as the
vocalizations increased, reductions in vocal ste- extent to which an independent variable is
reotypy were not clinically significant for two implemented as intended (Gresham, 1989;
of the three participants. Therefore, RIRD was Gresham, Gansle, & Noell, 1993). Clinicians
introduced and resulted in decreased vocal ste- often choose evidence-based procedures that
reotypy while appropriate vocalizations per- have been evaluated in controlled settings with
sisted. Similarly, as a means of providing an near perfect integrity; however, the generality
alternative behavior and decreasing the motiva- of such treatment effects in the natural setting
tion for engagement in vocal stereotypy, Love, under conditions of less than optimal treatment
Miguel, Fernand, and LaBrie (2012) arranged integrity are often unstudied. Relatedly, Fry-
noncontingent access to toys that provided ling, Wallace, and Yassine (2012) emphasized
matched stimulation. Matched stimulation and the need for descriptive treatment integrity data
RIRD were then compared as a package and collected in natural settings, to determine natu-
alone. Suppression of vocal stereotypy was rally occurring errors (rather than arbitrarily
obtained across all treatment conditions; how- selected errors) in need of further empirical
ever, appropriate vocalizations were highest evaluation. Hence, descriptive research may
when RIRD was a component of the treat- help to ensure ecological validity, or the degree
ment. These studies demonstrate that RIRD to which an analysis is representative of errors
optimizes treatment results. that occur in practice and predictive of phe-
Although RIRD has been shown to be effica- nomena that may occur in the clinical or edu-
cious, several researchers have suggested that cational setting (Brooks & Baumeister, 1977;
RIRD may require effortful responding to Gaylord-Ross, 1979). For example, Carroll,
implement (Duffy-Cassella, Sidener, Sidener, & Kodak, and Fisher (2013) included a descrip-
Progar, 2011; Kliebert, Tiger, & Toussaint, tive assessment of treatment integrity errors
2011; Miguel, Clark, Tereshko, & Ahearn, during discrete trial instruction in a classroom
2009). For example, if a student engages in setting. The experimenters demonstrated that
30 instances of stereotypy per hour, the teacher the three most commonly observed errors (fail-
must be available to implement RIRD 30 times ure to deliver a tangible following correct
per hour. In most schools, staff members are responses, failure to deliver a controlling
required to engage in various setting demands prompt, failure to present the correct instruc-
(e.g., attending to other students, data collec- tion once) resulted in less than optimal skill
tion, adherence to scheduled activities) and are acquisition outcomes for participants with
not likely to be trained in experimental autism spectrum disorder (see also Donnelly &
methods (McIntyre, Gresham, DiGennaro, & Karsten, 2017).
Reed, 2007). These demands paired with Following a descriptive assessment, empirical
resource-intensive procedures (characterized by questions regarding treatment outcomes can be
long treatment durations or high rates of target answered by manipulating the consistency of
behavior) may increase the likelihood of treat- treatment implementation (McIntyre et al.,
ment integrity failures (Hinz, McGee, Hui- 2007; Peterson, Homer, & Wonderlich, 1982).
tema, Dickinson, & Van Enk, 2014). For instance, intermittent schedules of
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 339

treatment delivery may reflect a pattern that is integrity condition, but treatment effects were
representative of lapses in treatment integrity. not maintained at lower integrity values.
Parametric analyses investigate such phenom- The purpose of the current study was to
ena via the manipulation of a specific parameter evaluate the extent to which treatment integrity
of an independent variable to determine the impacts RIRD treatment outcome. We evalu-
extent to which it influences responding ated the effectiveness of RIRD on each partici-
(e.g., Ahrens, Lerman, Kodak, Worsdell, & pant’s automatically maintained behavior,
Keegan, 2011; Kliebert et al., 2011; St. Peter trained classroom staff to implement the RIRD
Pipkin, Vollmer, & Sloman, 2010; Vollmer, procedure, and conducted treatment integrity
Roane, Ringdahl, & Marcus, 1999). Therefore, observations in the classroom setting
parametric analyses can be used to determine (Experiment 1). We then conducted a paramet-
the effects of a range of treatment integrity ric analysis in a controlled setting to determine
values on treatment outcome. the impact of the errors observed during treat-
Lerman and Iwata (1996) and Smith, Russo, ment integrity observations (Experiment 2).
and Le (1999) used parametric analyses to Specifically, we used the results of the treat-
investigate the behavioral mechanism responsi- ment integrity observations to inform the type
ble for the effects of response blocking, a proce- of error investigated (consistency rather than
dure similar to RIRD. Although it was not accuracy) and the range of consistency errors
their purpose, these studies illustrate the possi- tested.
ble effects of degraded treatment integrity on
treatment outcome. A single participant was
EXPERIMENT 1
exposed to various treatment implementation
values. The results indicated that response Method
blocking decreased hand mouthing across all Participants, setting, and materials. All partici-
values. Smith et al. conducted a systematic rep- pants attended a private school program that
lication of Lerman and Iwata in which they served approximately 160 day and residential
attempted to implement continuous response students. The program specialized in the educa-
blocking following baseline. However, 100% of tion and treatment of children diagnosed with
responses could not be blocked due to the autism spectrum disorder. A request for partici-
rapid rate of eye poking emitted by the partici- pants was sent to the program’s clinical and
pant. Nonetheless, eye poking decreased under educational team leaders. Participants were
this condition. Intermittent blocking showed nominated by their clinical and educational
that eye poking decreased during implementa- team due to engagement in vocal stereotypy
tion at 80%, but remained near baseline levels that interfered with daily activities
during 50% and 67% integrity. Taken (e.g., interference with attention to task, social
together, these studies indicate that treatment interactions, and vocational jobs in the
effects can be obtained at degraded levels of community).
treatment integrity, but they differ in the values The participants selected were all diagnosed
with which treatment effects were maintained. with autism spectrum disorder by a clinician
In a manner similar to the aforementioned unassociated with the clinical or research teams.
studies, Ahrens et al. (2011) conducted a para- Individualized Education Plan goal summaries
metric analysis of RIRD using the values 50%, and teacher reports were used to assess all par-
25%, and 10% with one participant following ticipants’ verbal repertoires. Kent was a
an RIRD treatment analysis. The results indi- 21-year-old male who engaged in prompted
cated that RIRD was effective during the 50% echoics, but whose communication was
340 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

primarily nonvocal. Therefore, his communica- duration was measured via a digital timer.
tion was supported with a voice output device. During treatment integrity observations, we
Cora was a 16-year-old female who emitted collected data on the frequency of opportunities
spontaneous one-word mands and occasional to implement RIRD, which was defined as
tacts. Noah was a 15-year-old male who emit- each episode of vocal stereotypy. Opportunities
ted spontaneous mands in the form of full sen- to implement RIRD were separated by the
tences, participated in three conversational absence of vocal stereotypy for 2 s or more.
volleys, and demonstrated prompted tacts. Each Vocal stereotypy that occurred during RIRD
participant was assigned to a different class- implementation was excluded from data collec-
room within the private school setting and was tion. To summarize treatment consistency
taught in a 1:2 staff to student ratio. (Peterson et al., 1982; Vollmer, Sloman, &
We trained 10 staff members to implement St. Peter Pipkin, 2008), we divided the number
RIRD and subsequently observed the degree to of occurrences of treatment implementation by
which the staff members implemented RIRD the total number of opportunities to implement
following training. The staff members were the procedure and multiplied by 100. Further-
23 to 29 years old and had a bachelors or mas- more, each time the RIRD procedure was
ter’s degree. They were accustomed to frequent implemented, accuracy data were collected. To
treatment integrity observations. calculate treatment accuracy, we divided the
For Kent, we conducted experimental ses- number of occurrences of each component by
sions in a room (1.5 m by 3 m) equipped with the total number of opportunities to implement
a video camera, a table, and chairs. For Cora the component and multiplied by 100. In addi-
and Noah, we conducted experimental sessions tion, we calculated overall accuracy by dividing
behind partitions in a classroom equipped with the number of components implemented by
a video camera, a table, and chairs. We con- the total number of components in the RIRD
ducted treatment integrity observations in each procedure and multiplied by 100.
participant’s classroom as they participated in An independent observer collected data
regularly scheduled activities. in vivo or from video recordings during a mini-
Response measurement and interobserver agree- mum of 30% of sessions for interobserver
ment. During the treatment analysis, we agreement purposes. Interobserver agreement
employed a second by second continuous dura- was calculated by dividing the number of 1-s
tion data collection procedure to measure vocal intervals with agreements by the total number
stereotypy, which is reported as the proportion of intervals with agreements plus disagreements
of the session with vocal stereotypy (i.e., total and multiplying by 100%. Agreements were
duration with vocal stereotypy divided by total defined as both observers indicating that vocal
session duration). Vocal stereotypy was defined stereotypy occurred during the same 1-s inter-
as any instance of noncontextual vocalizations, val. Disagreements were defined as only one
including repetitive babbling, grunts, squeals, observer indicating that vocal stereotypy
and phrases unrelated to the present situation, occurred during a 1-s interval. During the
emitted at or above conversation level. treatment analysis, the mean interobserver
We recorded the frequency and duration of agreement for vocal stereotypy was 99% for
each RIRD implementation to capture the Kent, 93% for Cora, and 89% for Noah (range
effort required to implement treatment. RIRD across participants, 78%-100%). The mean
implementation was defined as the therapist’s agreement for the frequency of treatment
immediate (i.e., within 2 s of the target behav- implementation was 100% for all participants.
ior) presentation of the RIRD procedure, and The mean agreement for the duration of
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 341

treatment implementation was 98% for Kent, stereotypy. During each session, the participant
99% for Cora, and 97% for Noah (range across and the experimenter sat at a table. Sessions
participants, 94%-100%). The mean agree- lasted 5 min. Throughout baseline and treat-
ment for accuracy was 98% for Kent, 97% for ment sessions, each participant had access to
Cora, and 95% for Noah (range across partici- the preferred leisure item identified via the
pants, 88%-100%) during treatment integrity preference assessment.
observations. The mean agreement for consis- Baseline. There were no programmed conse-
tency was 94% for Kent, 98% for Cora, and quences for vocal stereotypy during baseline.
91% for Noah (range across participants, The experimenter sat at the table with the par-
75%-100%). ticipant and collected data. The experimenter
Functional analysis. We conducted a func- did not interact with the participant unless the
tional analysis of vocal stereotypy using a subset participant engaged in a mand. If the partici-
of the conditions described in Iwata, Dorsey, pant manded, the experimenter responded by
Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982/1994). stating when the item would be available fol-
We omitted the play condition and alternated lowing the session.
5-min sessions across three conditions (atten- RIRD. During treatment conditions, contin-
tion, escape, and no interaction) to determine gent on each instance of vocal stereotypy, the
whether the maintaining variable(s) of vocal experimenter paused the session timer, removed
stereotypy were likely to be socially mediated the preferred item, and provided vocal prompts.
(similar to Roscoe, Carreau, MacDonald, & Vocal-verbal behavior appropriate for each par-
Pence, 2008). Persistent responding in the no- ticipant’s verbal repertoire was prompted. For
interaction condition relative to the attention example, a vocal prompt to engage in either
and escape conditions indicates presumed intraverbals (e.g., “Where do you live?”, “Where
maintenance by automatic reinforcement. Fur- do you go to school?”), to repeat a tact
thermore, as noted in Ahearn et al. (2007), (e.g., “The block is blue” in reference to the pre-
RIRD would be contraindicated in the case of ferred item), or to engage in one-word echoic
attention-maintained behavior as the treatment responses (e.g., “go”, “dog”, “book”) were pro-
necessitates that attention in the form of vocal vided. For Kent, echoics of familiar words were
redirection be provided contingent on the tar- prompted. For Cora and Noah, tacts or intraver-
get behavior. For each participant, the results bals were prompted. If the participant did not
of the functional analysis supported the respond within 2 s of the prompt, the experi-
hypothesis that vocal stereotypy occurred inde- menter prompted a different vocal response. For
pendent of social consequences (functional example, if the participant did not respond
analysis data available from first author). within 2 s of the prompt, “Say dog,” the experi-
Preference assessment. We conducted a paired- menter said, “Say book.” The procedure was
stimulus assessment (Fisher et al., 1992) with implemented until the participant responded to
each participant to identify preferred leisure three consecutive prompts in the absence of
items for use during the treatment analysis. vocal stereotypy. At the end of the procedure,
Classroom staff members who interacted regu- the experimenter delivered verbal praise for
larly with each participant identified the eight using appropriate language (e.g., “Good job
leisure items that were presented. For each par- talking to me!”), returned the preferred leisure
ticipant, the most highly preferred item was item, and resumed the session timer to ensure a
selected for use in the study. 5 min sample for comparison to baseline.
Treatment analysis. A withdrawal design was Staff training. We provided staff members
used to analyze the effects of RIRD on vocal with written RIRD guidelines (Flesch–Kincaid
342 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

readability grade level of 9.5), which included document the accuracy and consistency with
the definition of the target behavior, data col- which staff members implemented RIRD in
lection procedure, and treatment times the classroom setting. During the treatment
(e.g., work sessions, group activities, leisure integrity observations, staff members had access
activities) versus nontreatment times to the written guidelines, but did not receive
(e.g., toileting, meals, earned breaks). Staff feedback from the observer or their supervisor.
members could access these guidelines through- Each observation was approximately 15 min in
out the day. Following a review of the written duration. Observations were conducted over a
guidelines, staff members were given the oppor- period of approximately two months for each
tunity to ask questions. Next, staff members participant. During that period, the 10 staff
viewed a 5-min video that depicted the lead members were observed an average of 7.7 times
experimenter implementing a treatment analy- each (range, 4-10) for an average total duration
sis session that included at least three instances of 115 min each (range, 60-150 min). Data
of the RIRD implementation with the relevant were collected at least twice per week across
participant. Following video modeling, we pro- various days, times of day, activities, and
vided the staff members the opportunity to trained staff members (Vollmer et al., 2008).
practice RIRD implementation a minimum of
three times while the lead experimenter served
as the simulated consumer. While one staff Results
member role-played, the other staff members Figure 1 shows the percentage of the session
observed the role play. During role plays, the duration with vocal stereotypy during the treat-
simulated consumer responded in ways that ment analysis. For Kent (top panel), Cora (mid-
exposed staff members to a range of scenarios dle panel), and Noah (bottom panel), an
that may occur when implementing RIRD with ascending trend or moderate levels of vocal ste-
actual consumers. For example, after the staff reotypy were observed during baseline
member initiated RIRD, the simulated con- (M = 24.7%, 17.8%, and 43.5%, respectively),
sumer engaged in two prompted responses, followed by an immediate decrease in vocal ste-
then engaged in vocal stereotypy. This was reotypy to low levels when RIRD was intro-
done to ensure staff members continued to duced (M = 0.7%, 2.5%, and 11.7%,
prompt responses until the simulated consumer respectively). There was then an increase in
engaged in three consecutive prompted vocal stereotypy upon reintroduction of baseline
responses before providing praise and ending conditions for all participants (M = 34.2%,
RIRD. At the end of each role play, we pro- 22.8%, and 48.3%, respectively). We observed
vided positive and corrective feedback specific a replication of the treatment effect in the sec-
to steps implemented correctly and incorrectly, ond RIRD condition as vocal stereotypy
respectively. Positive feedback specified the decreased to low levels for all participants
steps implemented accurately, whereas correc- (M = 2.7%, 1.8%, and 10.5%, respectively).
tive feedback denoted steps implemented inac- For Kent, the average frequency and duration
curately and included a description of how to of RIRD implementation per session was 1.3
implement the step correctly in the future. To and 13 s, respectively. For Cora, the average fre-
complete the training, each staff member was quency and duration of RIRD implementation
required to independently demonstrate 100% per session was 5.5 and 18.6 s, respectively. In
accuracy for at least two consecutive role plays. comparison to Kent and Cora, the average fre-
Treatment integrity observations. Following quency of RIRD implementation per session for
staff training, we conducted observations to Noah was more frequent (M = 27.2) and the
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 343

Figure 1. Percentage of session duration with vocal stereotypy during baseline (BL) and the response interruption
and redirection (RIRD) treatment analysis (Experiment 1).

average duration per RIRD implementation opportunities to implement the procedure varied
across RIRD conditions was 9 s. (M = 14.3, range, 1-82), and treatment consis-
Figure 2 depicts the percentage of treatment tency was variable (M = 60.0%, range, 0%-
implementation (consistency) and opportunities 100%). For Cora (middle panel), opportunities
to implement treatment (vocal stereotypy epi- to implement the procedure were similar to
sodes) during classroom observations across staff Kent, but less variable (M = 4.2, range, 1-12).
members. For Kent (top panel), the Overall, treatment consistency was high
344 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

Figure 2. Percentage of treatment consistency and opportunities to implement the RIRD procedure for all partici-
pants during treatment integrity observations (Experiment 1).

(M = 89.7%, range, 50%-100%). For Noah, implemented across participants during obser-
(bottom panel) there were frequent opportunities vations. For Kent and Cora, all components
to implement the procedure (M = 29.1, range, were implemented with high integrity
2-49). Overall, treatment consistency was low (M = 94%, range, 86%-100% and M = 99%,
and variable (M = 41.1%, range, 0%-100%). range, 97%-100%, respectively). Although staff
Table 1 shows the average accuracy with members implemented component 5 (i.e.,
which each RIRD component was repeating the procedure until the participant
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 345

Table 1 1. The participants were three teenagers diag-


Average Implementation Accuracy (%) - Experiment 1 nosed with autism spectrum disorder by an
independent clinician. Cora (from Experiment
Component Description Kent Cora Noah
1) participated in this experiment. Morris was a
1 Remove preferred item 92 97 ---
(if applicable)
14-year-old male who emitted spontaneous
2 Prompt/gain attention 100 100 86 one- and two-word mands and prompted tacts.
of participant Chad was a 15-year-old male who emitted
(if necessary)
3 Provide prompts for 99 100 96 echoics, mands, and prompted tacts consisting
appropriate of a combination of one-word approximations
vocalizations
4 If no response within 90 100 100 and picture exchanges. He did not participate
2 to 3 s provide the in conversational volleys.
next prompt
5 Repeat components 86 98 79 All participants attended the same private
3 and 4 until the school program, were assigned to different
participant complies
with three responses
classrooms, and were taught in a 1:2 staff to
in the absence of student ratio. We conducted sessions with Cora
the target response behind partitions in a classroom equipped with
6 Deliver praise for 97 100 85
appropriate a video camera, a table, and chairs. We con-
vocalizations ducted sessions with Morris and Chad in an
All components 94 99 89
experimental room (1.5 m by 3 m) equipped
with a video camera, a table, and chairs.
complies with three responses in the absence of Response measurement and interobserver agree-
the target response) below 80% integrity for ment. During the treatment analysis, we
Noah, they implemented all other components employed a second-by-second continuous dura-
with high integrity (M = 89%, range, 79%- tion data collection procedure to measure vocal
100%). Component 1 was not applicable for stereotypy, which is reported as the proportion
Noah, as his preferred activity was building of the session with vocal stereotypy (i.e., total
with blocks, and he independently paused duration with vocal stereotypy divided by total
engagement with the activity each time the pro- session duration). Similarly to Experiment
cedure was initiated. 1, we also collected data on the frequency and
To summarize, the results of Experiment duration of each RIRD implementation. In
1 suggest that, on average, staff members addition, we calculated the average latency to
implemented RIRD with low and variable con- the first instance of vocal stereotypy and the
sistency. However, when staff members imple- average intertreatment time (ITT) as a measure
mented RIRD, they did so with high accuracy of treatment potency. Latency was defined as
(except for component 5 for Noah). Therefore, the duration from the onset of the session to
the purpose of Experiment 2 was to conduct a the onset of the first instance of vocal stereo-
parametric analysis of RIRD implementation typy. ITT was defined as the duration from the
consistency wherein we evaluated vocal stereo- offset of each treatment implementation to the
typy across four levels of consistency. onset of subsequent occurrence of vocal
stereotypy.
An independent observer collected data dur-
EXPERIMENT 2
ing a minimum of 30% of sessions for interob-
Method server agreement purposes in vivo or from
Participants and setting. We recruited partici- video. Agreement for stereotypy duration was
pants in the same manner as for Experiment calculated as in Experiment 1. During the
346 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

treatment analysis, the mean agreement for identified via the preference assessment during
vocal stereotypy was 97% for Chad and 98% sessions.
for Morris (range across participants, 92%- Baseline. Baseline was identical to that in
99%). The mean agreement for the frequency Experiment 1.
of treatment implementation was 100% for RIRD. Contingent on each instance of vocal
Chad and Morris. The mean agreement for the stereotypy, the experimenter paused the session
duration of treatment implementation was timer, removed the preferred item, and pro-
100% for Chad and 97% (range, 96%-100%) vided vocal prompts. Vocal-verbal behavior
for Morris. During the parametric analysis, the appropriate for each participant’s verbal reper-
mean agreement for vocal stereotypy was 97% toire was prompted (echoics for Chad, tacts or
for Cora, 95% for Chad, and 96% for Morris intraverbals for Morris). If the participant did
(range across participants, 86%-100%). The not respond within 2 s of the prompt, the
mean agreement for the frequency of treatment experimenter prompted a different vocal
implementation was 100% for both Cora and response. The procedure was implemented
Chad, and 99% (range, 99%-100%) for Mor- until the participant responded to three consec-
ris. The mean agreement for the duration of utive prompts in the absence of vocal stereo-
treatment implementation was 99% for Cora, typy. At the end of the procedure, the
97% for Chad, and 97% for Morris (range experimenter delivered verbal praise for using
across participants, 94%-100%). The mean appropriate language and returned the preferred
agreement for the average latency and average leisure item. The session timer was then
ITT was 98% for Cora, 97% for Chad, and resumed to ensure a 5 min sample for compari-
97% for Morris (range across participants, son to baseline.
95%-100%). Parametric analysis. During the parametric
Functional analysis. As in Experiment 1, we analysis, we implemented three treatment
conducted a functional analysis of vocal stereo- integrity conditions (75%, 50%, and 25%)
typy using a subset of the conditions described sequentially, in alternation with baseline. We
in Iwata et al. (1982/1994). For each partici- implemented RIRD for three of every four
pant, the results supported the hypothesis that instances of vocal stereotypy during the 75%
vocal stereotypy occurred independent of social condition, every other instance of vocal stereo-
consequences (functional analysis data available typy during the 50% condition, and one of
from the first author). every four instances of vocal stereotypy during
Preference assessment. As in Experiment 1, we the 25% condition. Following the 25% condi-
conducted a paired-stimulus preference assess- tion, we reintroduced the RIRD treatment with
ment (Fisher et al., 1992) with each participant 100% integrity to determine if treatment
to identify preferred leisure items for use during effects could be recovered. In the last condi-
the analysis, and the most highly preferred tion, we conducted a series of two 25% treat-
items were selected for use in the study. ment integrity sessions in alteration with one
Experimental design. We employed a with- 100% integrity session (i.e., booster session) to
drawal design to analyze the effects of RIRD determine whether interspersing booster ses-
on vocal stereotypy and to evaluate the effects sions could also support recovery of previous
of treatment integrity manipulations via para- treatment effects. To assist with experimenter
metric analysis conditions. During each session, adherence to condition-specific integrity levels,
the participant and the experimenter sat at a the experimenter used a deck of cue cards that
table together. Sessions lasted 5 min. Each par- indicated when to implement or withhold
ticipant had access to the preferred leisure item RIRD (e.g., during the 50% treatment
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 347

implementation condition, every other card treatment analysis. During baseline, Chad (top
signaled that RIRD should be implemented). panel) demonstrated an increasing trend for
After each instance of vocal stereotypy, the vocal stereotypy (M = 14.4%) and Morris (bot-
experimenter moved the card on top of the tom panel) emitted vocal stereotypy at a high
deck to the bottom. Because vocal stereotypy level (M = 68.2%). Once RIRD was imple-
was reported as a proportion of session time, if mented, there was a decrease in vocal stereo-
the planned schedule was not met within a ses- typy to low levels for Chad and Morris
sion, the session was extended until the sched- (M = 3.5% and 6.6%, respectively). Upon the
ule requirement was met. However, this rarely removal of RIRD, we observed an immediate
occurred due to the frequency of vocal increase in vocal stereotypy for Chad and Mor-
stereotypy. ris (M = 35.6% and 65.8%, respectively).
Once RIRD was reintroduced, vocal stereotypy
Results decreased to near zero levels (M = 1.0%) for
Figure 3 shows the percentage of the session Chad and returned to the approximate level
duration with vocal stereotypy during the observed in the initial RIRD condition

Figure 3. Percentage of session duration with vocal stereotypy during baseline (BL) and the response interruption
and redirection (RIRD) treatment analysis for Chad and Morris (Experiment 2).
348 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

Table 2 average ITT per condition decreased from RIRD


Average Latency and ITT during Treatment Analysis for 1 (M = 25.5 s) to RIRD 2 (M = 13.8 s).
Experiment 2 Figure 4 shows the percentage of session
Participant Condition Average Latency Average ITT
duration with vocal stereotypy during the para-
metric analysis. For all three participants, the
Cora
Baseline 12.0 same pattern of responding was evident. How-
RIRD 1 14.7 48.4 ever, the results for Cora (top panel) and Chad
Baseline 36.7
RIRD 2 49.0 62.0
(middle panel) were more robust than those for
Chad Morris (bottom panel). Vocal stereotypy was
Baseline 33.0 emitted at a lower level during both the 75%
RIRD 1 14.3 29.9
Baseline 10.0 (M = 6.8%, M = 4.1%, and M = 13.8%,
RIRD 2 72.1 35.3 respectively) and the 50% (M = 2.6%,
Morris
Baseline 8.7 M = 5.3%, and M = 15.4 % respectively) con-
RIRD 1 22.8 25.5 ditions in comparison to the first two baseline
Baseline 0.3
RIRD 2 1.5 13.8 conditions (M = 29.2%, M = 42.3%;
M = 11.8%, M = 14.2%; and M = 40.2%,
Note. ITT = intertreatment time; RIRD = response inter- M = 38.4%, respectively). For all three partici-
ruption and redirection
pants, there was also an increase in vocal stereo-
typy when baseline was introduced for the
(M = 9.7%) for Morris. The average frequency third time (M = 22.3%, M = 30.3%, and
of RIRD implementation per session was 6.8 M = 39.2%, respectively). During the 25%
for Chad and 15.0 for Morris. The average condition, near-baseline levels of vocal stereo-
duration per RIRD implementation across con- typy were observed for all participants
ditions was 20 s for Chad and 13 s for Morris. (M = 21.3%, M = 18.3%, and M = 44.5%,
Table 2 shows the average latency to the first respectively). RIRD was then reintroduced at
instance of vocal stereotypy and the average ITT 100% integrity and low levels of vocal stereo-
for each condition of the treatment analysis. For typy were recovered (M = 1.7%, M = 3.4%,
Cora, the average latency increased across condi- and M = 5.8%, respectively). During the 25%
tions as the treatment analysis progressed. plus booster session condition, vocal stereotypy
Although the average latency increased in dura- increased in comparison to the 100% condition
tion overtime, a comparison of baseline and responding resembled the results obtained
(M = 12.0 s, M = 36.7 s) and treatment during the 50% condition (M = 4.2%,
(M = 14.7 s, M = 49.0 s) latency did not show M = 6.1%, and M = 15.5%, respectively).
a great difference. The average ITT per condi- For Cora, the average frequency of RIRD
tion increased from RIRD 1 (M = 48.4 s) to implementation per session was 3.6 and the
RIRD 2 (M = 62.0 s). For Chad and Morris, average duration per RIRD implementation
the difference between baseline and treatment was 9.7 s. There was a decreasing trend in the
latency was also not substantial. For Chad, the average duration per treatment implementation
average latency increased from RIRD as the parametric analysis progressed. For Chad,
1 (M = 14.3 s) to RIRD 2 (M = 72.1 s), and as the average frequency of RIRD implementation
for Cora, the average ITT per condition per session was 4.5 and the average duration
increased from RIRD 1 (M = 29.9 s) to RIRD per RIRD implementation was 8.8 s. Morris’
2 (M = 35.3 s). For Morris, however, the aver- average frequency of RIRD implementation per
age latency decreased from RIRD session was high (10.0) and the average dura-
1 (M = 22.8 s) to RIRD 2 (M = 1.5 s), and the tion per RIRD implementation (20.7 s) was
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 349

Figure 4. Percentage of session duration with vocal stereotypy during baseline (BL) and the response interruption
and redirection (RIRD) parametric analysis for Experiment 2 participants. The percentages specify the percentage of
vocal stereotypy instances for which RIRD was implemented.

consistently longer in comparison to Cora and Furthermore, despite previous exposure to


Chad’s. decreased levels of treatment integrity, optimal
The parametric analysis indicated that RIRD treatment effects were observed once RIRD
suppressed vocal stereotypy when implemented with consistent 100% treatment integrity was
during at least 50% of opportunities. When we reintroduced for all participants.
interspersed 100% treatment integrity booster
sessions with 25% treatment integrity sessions,
the level of vocal stereotypy was similar to that GENERAL DISCUSSION
observed during the 50% condition. However, The current study evaluated the extent to
treatment effects for Morris were modest in which treatment integrity impacts RIRD treat-
comparison to the results of Cora and Chad. ment outcome. First, we determined the types
350 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

of RIRD treatment integrity errors that occur significant as continuous punishment. The
in the natural setting and then we analyzed results of the parametric analysis support these
such errors in a controlled setting. Experiment findings as continuous implementation of
1 indicated that following training, staff mem- RIRD produced greater response suppression in
bers implemented RIRD inconsistently contin- comparison to intermittent implementation.
gent on instances of stereotypy in the natural Given that RIRD was effective at suppres-
setting. However, when staff members did sing vocal stereotypy when implemented at
implement RIRD, they did so with high accu- least 50% of the time, these findings replicate
racy. In Experiment 2, we conducted a para- those of Lerman and Iwata (1996) and Ahrens
metric analysis to determine the degree to et al. (2011), but not those of Smith et al.
which systematic manipulations of RIRD (1999). The current study and those conducted
implementation consistency integrity would by Lerman and Iwata and by Ahrens et al. first
influence RIRD effectiveness. The results of implemented treatment with 100% integrity
Experiment 2 indicated that the impact of and continued to observe treatment effects
treatment inconsistency was minimal if RIRD when integrity levels were decreased to as low
was implemented following at least 50% of as 25%. However, as previously mentioned, the
instances of stereotypic behavior. participant in the Smith et al. study was not
The results of the treatment analyses in exposed to a recent history of 100% treatment
Experiments 1 and 2 replicated previous integrity due to an inability to block every
research (e.g., Ahearn et al., 2007; Ahrens response the participant emitted during the
et al., 2011) by demonstrating that RIRD was continuous blocking condition. Subsequently,
effective in decreasing automatically maintained Smith et al. only observed treatment effects
vocal stereotypy. Previous studies (e.g., Ahrens with integrity levels of 80% or 100%. There-
et al., 2011; Shawler & Miguel, 2015) have fore, one potential explanation for the differ-
indicated that one of the behavioral mecha- ence in findings between Smith et al. and those
nisms responsible for RIRD’s effectiveness may of Lerman and Iwata, Ahrens et al., and the
be positive punishment. When using RIRD to current study may be the difference in whether
treat automatically maintained vocal stereotypy, experimenters were able to establish a history of
reinforcement remains available contingent on continuous punishment. Basic studies (Banks,
each instance of vocal stereotypy because 1966; Shemer & Feldon, 1984) have found
extinction is not feasible. Given this concurrent that exposure to intermittent punishment prior
schedule arrangement, one might predict that to continuous punishment does not lead to suf-
RIRD would be most effective if implemented ficient response suppression. Applied studies
following each instance of the target response. (Rollings & Baumeister, 1981; Romancyzk,
However, the results of the parametric analysis 1977) have shown intermittent punishment to
conducted in Experiment 2 suggest that a mean be effective only following recent exposure to
at or above 50% implementation consistency continuous punishment. Further research is
may constitute a probability strong enough to required to investigate whether treatment
compete with continuous automatic reinforce- effects would be stronger if treatment failure
ment. Although few studies have examined the conditions were directly preceded by a 100%
effects of intermittent punishment without the integrity condition rather than by baseline
use of extinction, Azrin, Holz, and Hake (St. Peter Pipkin et al., 2010; Vollmer et al.,
(1963) demonstrated that intermittent punish- 1999). A second potential explanation for the
ment without the use of extinction did not pro- difference in findings may be related to the
duce response suppression as rapid or behavioral mechanism responsible for treatment
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 351

effects. That is, Ahrens et al. and Lerman and reinforcing compliance during RIRD. Such
Iwata attributed treatment effects for their par- studies will likely provide valuable information
ticipants to positive punishment, whereas that may influence the integrity with which
Smith et al. indicated that extinction was likely RIRD is implemented in clinical practice.
responsible for their results. The results of the current studies provide
Although positive punishment is likely one some preliminary support for the assertion that
of the behavioral mechanisms responsible for the amount of time required to implement a
RIRD’s effectiveness, other variables may con- treatment may affect how susceptible it is to
tribute as well. First, access to preferred items, lapses in treatment integrity (Gresham, Mac-
in addition to RIRD, has been arranged in the Millan, Beebe-Frankenberger, & Bocian,
majority of previous RIRD studies 2000). Specifically, Noah engaged in higher
(Martinez & Betz, 2013). Access to preferred rates of vocal stereotypy (M = 29.1), producing
items presents an opportunity for individuals to an average of twice as many opportunities to
allocate responding towards these stimuli that implement the RIRD procedure when com-
in turn may suppress stereotypy (e.g., Ahearn, pared to the rates of vocal stereotypy emitted
Clark, DeBar, & Florentino, 2005; Ringdahl, by Kent and Cora (M = 14.3 and M = 14.0,
Vollmer, Marcus, & Roane, 1997). Although respectively). Noah’s treatment analysis data
we provided access to a preferred item during also indicate that the frequency and the dura-
baseline and RIRD, future researchers could tion of RIRD were considerably higher for him
evaluate the effectiveness of RIRD with and in comparison to Kent and Cora. These find-
without the availability of preferred items or ings in conjunction with the low rates of treat-
conduct a competing items assessment to ment consistency observed during Noah’s
decrease the likelihood of using items that may classroom observations highlight the concerns
compete with the target behavior. Second, akin set forth by past researchers regarding the
to several previous studies (e.g., Ahrens et al., potential impact effortful treatment application
2011, Duffy-Casella et al., 2011), we removed may have on treatment integrity. These results
the preferred item contingent on instances of suggest that additional research is needed to
stereotypy prior to implementing RIRD. This generate solutions for such treatment chal-
procedural element may function as negative lenges. One approach that may decrease the
punishment and has been shown to suppress duration of RIRD (thereby potentially decreas-
stereotypy when implemented in isolation ing the response effort required by teachers)
(Toper-Korkmaz, Lerman, & Tsami, in press). without diminishing treatment effects is to
Future studies are needed to clarify the condi- implement RIRD with one demand rather than
tions under which item removal should be used three demands. Saini, Gregory, Uran, and Fan-
in conjunction with RIRD. Third, praise for tetti (2015) found RIRD with one demand to
compliance with demands, which potentially be effective in reducing stereotypy and to be
functions as positive reinforcement for engage- less intrusive than RIRD requiring three
ment in the appropriate behavior, was included demands. Future researchers should determine
in our study as well as in the majority of studies whether this procedural variation can bolster
reviewed by Martinez and Betz (2013). Future treatment integrity in nonexperimental settings.
researchers should systematically evaluate the Implementing RIRD with matched stimulation
added value of providing access to a preferred or directly training appropriate replacement
item and removing access to the item following behavior may also decrease the duration of
instances of stereotypy in conjunction with RIRD. In regard to the former, Love et al.
RIRD; they might also determine the effects of (2012) demonstrated that arranging matched
352 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

stimulation in conjunction with RIRD may simulated systematic schedule thinning by


function as an abolishing operation, thereby implementing variations of treatment integrity
decreasing the rate of stereotypy and concomi- on a continuum (Northup, Fisher, Kahng,
tantly the frequency of RIRD implementation. Harrell, & Kurtz, 1997; Stephenson & Hanley,
In regard to the latter, the direct training of 2010; Vollmer et al., 1999) and obtaining
appropriate replacement behavior (e.g. Colón behavioral stability before titrating up or down
et al., 2012) in conjunction with RIRD may the schedule. Future research could take steps
assist with suppression of vocal stereotypy by to identify potential sequence effects (see Hig-
making appropriate behavior more probable gins Hains, & Baer, 1989). In addition, the
and stereotypy less probable. Future studies Experiment 1 results indicate that treatment
should also determine the effects of treatment implementation in the natural environment is
integrity failures on appropriate behavior variable. Future research investigating variable
(e.g., appropriate vocalizations). or random levels of treatment consistency may
During the parametric analysis, we observed be important to understand how naturally
a decrease in latency and ITT for Morris, but occurring patterns of treatment implementation
not for Kent and Cora. It is possible that RIRD impact treatment outcome. Furthermore,
was not potent enough to compete with a con- although we used cue cards to assist with
tinuous schedule of automatic reinforcement adherence during the parametric analysis, pro-
and habituation to RIRD may have occurred. cedural integrity data were not collected.
Relatedly, Cipani, Brendlinger, McDowell, and The findings from the current study in con-
Usher (1991) compared the effect of contin- junction with existing research (e.g., Ahrens
gent application of lemon juice to manual guid- et al., 2011; Lerman & Iwata, 1996; Roll-
ance overcorrection without the use of ings & Baumeister, 1981; Romancyzk, 1977;
extinction on stereotypy and self-injurious St. Peter Pipkin et al., 2010; Vollmer et al.,
behavior. They found that lemon juice 1999) generally indicate that providing expo-
decreased these behaviors to near-zero levels on sure to 100% treatment integrity prior to the
both a continuous schedule and a variable ratio onset of possible treatment integrity failures
4 schedule. Although overcorrection decreased may maximize the likelihood of clinically sig-
the target responses when implemented contin- nificant treatment success. However, it
uously, it did not produce sufficient response remains unknown how recent or how much
suppression on a variable ratio 4 schedule. Fur- exposure to 100% integrity is necessary to
ther investigation is necessary to clarify under produce successful treatment outcomes.
what conditions continuous and noncontinuous Although further replication, particularly in
applications of such procedures produce suffi- the natural setting, is necessary, the booster
cient treatment outcomes. condition in Experiment 2 offers a potential
A potential limitation to the current study is strategy to ameliorate poor treatment integrity.
that the parametric analysis always occurred in Moreover, although bolstering integrity to a
the following order: 75%-50%-25%. This mean of 50% or greater sufficiently salvaged
sequence may have led to outcomes that would previous treatment effects in the current study,
have differed had we conducted conditions in future research regarding the necessary param-
different sequences (e.g., 25%-75%-50%). We eters of effective booster sessions is warranted.
elected to conduct conditions in a fixed Until further research is conducted, it is
sequence for the purpose of replication across important for clinicians to monitor and ensure
participants, and to mirror other parametric that the level of ongoing treatment integrity
analyses of treatment consistency that have produces acceptable treatment effects.
TREATMENT INTEGRITY 353

REFERENCES Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G.,


Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992).
Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., DeBar, R., & A comparison of two approaches for identifying
Florentino, C. (2005). On the role of preference in reinforcers for persons with severe and profound dis-
response competition. Journal of Applied Behavior abilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25,
Analysis, 38, 247-250. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. 491-498. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-491
2005.36-04 Fryling, M. J., Wallace, M. D., & Yassine, J. N. (2012).
Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., MacDonald, R. P. F., & Impact of treatment integrity on intervention effec-
Chung, B. I. (2007). Assessing and treating vocal ste- tiveness. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45,
reotypy in children with autism. Journal of Applied 449-453. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-449
Behavior Analysis, 40, 263-275. https://doi.org/10.
Gaylord-Ross, R. J. (1979). Mental retardation research,
1901/jaba.2007.30-06
ecological validity, and the delivery of longitudinal
Ahrens, E. N., Lerman, D. C., Kodak, T., education programs. The Journal of Special Education,
Worsdell, A. S., & Keegan, C. (2011). Further evalu- 13, 69-80.
ation of response interruption and redirection as
Gresham, F. M. (1989). Assessment of treatment integrity
treatment for stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior
in school consultation and pre-referral intervention.
Analysis, 44, 95-108. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
School Psychology Review, 18, 37-50.
2011.44-95
Azrin, N. H., Holz, W. C., & Hake, D. F. (1963). Gresham, F. M., Gansle, K. A., & Noell, G. H. (1993).
Fixed-ratio punishment. Journal of the Experimental Treatment integrity in applied behavior analysis with
Analysis of Behavior, 6, 141-148. https://doi.org/10. children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26,
1901/jeab.1963.6-141 257-263. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-257
Banks, R. K. (1966). Persistence to continuous punish- Gresham, F. M., MacMillan, D. L., Beebe-
ment following intermittent punishment training. Frankenberger, M. E., & Bocian, K. M. (2000).
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 373-377. Treatment integrity in learning disabilities interven-
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023016 tion research: Do we really know how treatments are
Brooks, P. H., & Baumeister, A. A. (1977). A plea for implemented? Learning Disabilities Research &
consideration of ecological validity in the experimen- Practice, 15, 198-205. https://doi.org/10.1207/
tal psychology of mental retardation: A guest edito- SLDRP1504_4
rial. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 81(5), Higgins Hains, A., & Baer, D. M. (1989). Interaction
407-416. effects in multielement designs: Inevitable, desirable,
Carroll, R. A., Kodak, T., & Fisher, W. W. (2013). An and ignorable. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
evaluation of programmed treatment integrity errors 22, 57-69. https://doi.org/10.1901/ jaba.1989.22-57
during discrete-trial instruction. Journal of Applied Hinz, K. L., McGee, H. M., Huitema, B. E.,
Behavior Analysis, 46, 379-394. https://doi.org/10. Dickinson, A. M., & Van Enk, R. A. (2014).
1002/jaba.49 Observer accuracy and behavior analysis: Data collec-
Cipani, E., Brendlinger, J., McDowell, L., & Usher, S. tion procedures on hand hygiene compliance in a
(1991). Continuous vs. intermittent punishment: A neurovascular unit. American Journal of Infection
case study. Journal of Developmental and Physical Control, 42, 1067-1073. https://doi-org.silk.library.
Disabilities, 3, 147-156. https://doi.org/10.1007/ umass.edu/10.1016/j.ajic.2014.06.017
BF01045930 Iwata, B.A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J.,
Colón, C. L., Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., & Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a
Masalsky, J. (2012). The effects of verbal operant functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied
training and response interruption and redirection on Behavior Analysis, 27, 197-209. (Reprinted from
appropriate and inappropriate vocalizations. Journal of Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 107-120. https://doi. 2, 3-20, 1982). https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.297
org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-107 Kincaid, J. P., Fishburne, R. P., Rogers, R. L., &
Donnelly, M. G., & Karsten, A. M. (2017). Effects of Chissom, B. S. (1975). Derivation of new readability
programmed teaching errors on acquisition and dura- formulas (automated readability index, fog count,
bility of self-care skills. Journal of Applied Behavior and Flesch reading ease formula) for Navy enlisted
Analysis, 50, 511-528. https://doi.org/10.1002/ personnel. Institute for Simulation and Training.
jaba.390 Paper 56.
Duffy-Cassella, M., Sidener, T. M., Sidener, D. W., & Kliebert, M. L., Tiger, J. H., & Toussaint, K. A. (2011).
Progar, P. R. (2011). Response interruption and redi- An approach to identifying the conditions under
rection for vocal stereotypy in children with autism: which response interruption will reduce automatically
A systematic replication. Journal of Applied Behavior reinforced problem behavior. Behavior Analysis in
Analysis, 44, 169-173. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. Practice, 4, 17-26. https://doi.org/10.1007/
2011.44-169. BF03391771
354 CANDICE L. COLÓN and WILLIAM H. AHEARN

Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). A methodology and redirection as treatment for stereotypy. Journal of
for distinguishing between extinction and punish- Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 96-106. https://doi.
ment effects associated with response blocking. Jour- org/10.1002/jaba.186
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 231-233. https:// Shawler, L. A., & Miguel, C. F. (2015). The effects of
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-231 motor and vocal response interruption and redirec-
Love, J. J., Miguel, C. F., Fernand, J. K., & LaBrie, J. K. tion on vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocaliza-
(2012). The effects of matched stimulation and tions. Behavioral Interventions, 30, 112-134. https://
response interruption and redirection on vocal stereo- doi.org/10.1002/bin.1407
typy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 549-
564. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-549. Shemer, A., & Feldon, J. (1984). Long-term partial rein-
Martinez, C. K., & Betz, A. M. (2013). Response inter- forcement extinction effect and long-term partial
ruption and redirection: Current research trends and punishment effect in a one-trial-a-day paradigm.
clinical application. Journal of Applied Behavior Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22, 221-224.
Analysis, 46, 549-554. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Smith, R. G., Russo, L., & Le, D. D. (1999). Distin-
jaba.38 guishing between extinction and punishment effects
McIntyre, L. L., Gresham, F. M., DiGennaro, F. D., & of response blocking: A replication. Journal of Applied
Reed, D. D. (2007). Treatment integrity of school- Behavior Analysis, 32, 367-370. https://doi.org/10.
based interventions with children in the Journal of 1901/jaba.1999.32-367
Applied Behavior Analysis 1991-2005. Journal of Stephenson, K. M., & Hanley, G. P. (2010). Pre-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 659-672. https://doi. schoolers’ compliance with simple instructions: A
org/10.1901/jaba.2007.659-672 descriptive and experimental evaluation. Journal of
Miguel, C. F., Clark, K., Tereshko, L., & Ahearn, W. H. Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 229-247. https://doi.
(2009). The effects of response interruption and redi- org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-229
rection and sertraline on vocal stereotypy. Journal of
St. Peter Pipkin, C., Vollmer, T. R., & Sloman, K. N.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 883-888. https://doi.
(2010). Effects of treatment integrity failures during
org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-883
differential reinforcement of alternative behavior: A
Northup, J., Fisher, W., Kahng, S. W., Harrell, R., &
translational model. Journal of Applied Behavior
Kurtz, P. (1997). An assessment of the necessary
Analysis, 43, 47-70. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
strength of behavioral treatments for severe behavior
2010.43-47
problems. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Disabilities, 9, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1023/A: Toper-Korkmaz, O., Lerman, D. C., & Tsami, L.
1024984526008 (in press). Effects of toy removal and number of
Peterson, L., Homer, A. L., & Wonderlich, S. A. (1982). demands on vocal stereotypy during response inter-
The integrity of independent variables in behavior ruption and redirection. Journal of Applied Behavior
analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, Analysis.
477-492. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1982.15-477 Vanderkerken, L., Heyvaert, M., Maes, B., &
Ringdahl, J. E., Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Onghena, P. (2013). Psychosocial interventions for
Roane, H. S. (1997). An analogue evaluation of envi- reducing vocal challenging behavior in persons with
ronmental enrichment: The role of stimulus prefer- autistic disorder: A multilevel meta-analysis of single-
ence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, case experiments. Research in Developmental
203-216. Disabilities, 34, 4515-4533. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Rollings, J. P., & Baumeister, A. A. (1981). Stimulus con- j.ridd.2013.09.030
trol of stereotypic responding: Effects on target and Vollmer, T. R., Roane, H. S., Ringdahl, J. E., &
collateral behavior. American Journal of Mental Marcus, B. A. (1999). Evaluating treatment chal-
Deficiency, 86, 67-77. lenges with differential reinforcement of alternative
Romanczyk, R. G. (1977). Intermittent punishment of behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 9-
self-stimulation: Effectiveness during application and 23. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-9
extinction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 45, 53-60. https://doi.org/10. Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & St. Peter Pipkin, C.
1037/0022-006X.45.1.53 (2008). Practical implications of data reliability
Roscoe, E. M., Carreau, A., MacDonald, J., & and treatment integrity monitoring. Behavior Analysis
Pence, S. T. (2008). Further evaluation of leisure in Practice, 1, 4-11. https://doi.org/10.1007/
items in the attention condition of functional ana- BF03391722
lyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41,
351-364. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-351 Received December 12, 2016
Saini, V., Gregory, M. K., Uran, K. J., & Fantetti, M. A. Final acceptance July 14, 2018
(2015). Parametric analysis of response interruption Action Editor, Jason Vladescu

Anda mungkin juga menyukai