Anda di halaman 1dari 21

School Psychology Review,

2014, Volume 43, No. 1, pp. 86-105

Students' Perceptions of Relatedness in the Classroom:


The Roles of Emotionally Supportive Teacher-Child
Interactions, Children's Aggressive-Disruptive Behaviors,
and Peer Social Preference

Rebecca A. Madill and Scott D. Gest


The Pennsylvania State University

Philip C. Rodkin
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

Abstract. This study examines the roles of emotionally supportive teacher-child


interactions and child characteristics (aggressive-disruptive behavior and low
peer social preference) in first-, third, and fifth-grade children's perceptiotis of
teacher closeness and sense of peer community. Results from a series of multi-
level models suggest that emotionally supportive teacher-child interactions are
associated with children's perceptions of closeness with their teachers (for boys
and girls), as well as a greater sense of peer community (for boys only).
Aggressive-disruptive children and boys tended to perceive less-close relation-
ships with their teachers. Results indicate that classroom-level factors, particu-
larly the supportive qualities of teachers' interactions with students, are associated
with children's perceived closeness with teachers and sense of peer community,
above and beyond the influence of children's individual characteristics.

The elementary school classroom is a "Eeelings of belonging may have an energetic


primary context in which children develop function, awakening enthusiasm, interest, and
social relationships (Maddox & Prinz, 2003), willingness to participate in academic activi-
providing opportunities to experience a sense ties" (p, 158). An important task of educa-
of belonging that is fundamental to emotional tional research is to identify the determinants
and physical health (Baumeister & Leary, of children's perceptions of relatedness, de-
1995). Children who feel connected to their fined as "secure and satisfying connections
teachers and classmates have greater intrinsic with others" (Deci, Vallerland, Pelletier, &
motivation for learning and are more engaged Ryan, 1991, p. 327), in the classroom context.
(Battistich, Solomon, Kim, Watson, & Schaps, According to the bioecological model of
1995; Hughes, 2011; Furrer & Skinner, 2003). human development (Bronfenbrenner & Mor-
As Furrer and Skinner eloquently argue, ris, 2006), development is influenced by com-

Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Rebecca A. Madill, Human Development and
Family Studies, The Pennsylvania State University, 208 Beecher-Dock House, University Park, PA 16802;
e-mail: ram436@psu.edti.
Copyright 2014 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015

86
Relatedness in the Classroom

plex interactions among person characteristics surement efforis have focused on direct obser-
(individual characteristics), proximal pro- vation of teacher-child interactions, thereby
cesses (interactions with one's environment), bringing measurement operations closer to the
contexts, and time. In this study, we consider daily proximal interactional processes that
the role that person characteristics and proxi- drive children's development (Bronfenbrenner
mal processes known as emotionally support- & Morris, 2006).
ive teacher-child interactions (ESTCIs; The core components of ESTCIs are
Pianta, La Paro, & Hamre, 2008) may have in creating positive climates, minimizing nega-
the development of children's sense of tive experiences, responding to children's in-
relatedness. dividual needs, and incorporating children's
There is compelling evidence suggest- interests into lessons (Pianta, La Paro, &
ing that children's perceptions of their class- Hamre, 2008). Observed ESTCIs have been
room relationships contribute to emerging pat- associated with teacher reports of closer and
terns of achievement motivation and academic less-conflicted teacher-child relationships
performance. For example, Hughes (2011) (O'Connor, 2010; Hamre, Pianta, Downer, &
found that children's reports of relational Mashburn, 2008), as well as growth in reading
warmth with their teachers predicted changes skills in elementary school (Pianta, Belsky et
in perceived academic competence, math al., 2008).
achievement, and school bonding; however,
teacher-reported relationship quality was not Children's Perceptions of Relatedness
associated with these outcomes. Moreover,
Teacher-Child Closeness
children's perceptions of the quality of rela-
tionships in the classroom community were Researchers working from attachment
associated with greater intrinsic motivation for theory (Bowlby, 1982) consider children's
learning (Battistich et al., 1995), and adoles- representations of closeness and conflict in
cents' perceptions of emotional support from their relationships with their teachers to be the
peers were associated with student motivation, critical elements of the teacher-child relation-
beyond the effect of perceived emotional sup- ship that drive children's behavior. For exam-
port from the teacher (Wentzel, Battle, Rus- ple, second- and third-grade children's percep-
sell, & Looney, 2010). tions of closeness to their teachers predicted
High-quality daily interactions between changes in achievement the following year
the teacher and child are thought to be a pri- (Hughes, 2011). However, the relative impor-
mary mechanism through which teachers in- tance of different dimensions of teacher sup-
fluence children's development (Pianta, Bel- port, including emotional support, academic
sky, Vandergrift, Houts, & Morrison, 2008). support, clear behavioral and academic expec-
Early measurement efforts (Birch & Ladd, tations, and provision of a safe classroom
1998; Pianta, 2001) focused on teacher reports environment, are likely important areas for
of closeness from an attachment perspective, research as well (Wentzel et al., 2010).
suggesting that closeness allowed teachers to Wentzel et al. found each dimension to ac-
function as a secure base (e.g., a source of help count for unique variance in adolescents' mo-
if the child struggles) and allowed the child to tivation, but there is not yet evidence among
be more engaged in school activities (Myers & young children that specific forms of support
Pianta, 2008). Indeed, teacher ratings of close- also function differentially or are more predic-
ness have been linked to concurrent classroom tive than a single teacher-student closeness
engagement and competencies (Baker, 2006; index.
Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Pianta & Stuhlman, Teacher and child reports of closeness
2004), though the evidence that teacher-child are often discrepant. For example, Wu,
closeness predicts future competencies is Hughes, and Kwok (2010) found a trivial cor-
mixed (Hughes, Luo, Kwok, & Loyd, 2008; relation between teacher and child reports of
Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004). More recent mea- relational warmth in a sample of at-risk third
87
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

graders (r = 0.08). Given the unique impor- quality have significant between-classroom
tance of child-reported relational warmth for differences (i.e., children in the same class-
certain motivational outcomes (Hughes, room provide similar reports), it is important
2011), observed ESTCIs might be associated to examine features of the classroom, such as
not only with teacher-reported closeness, as ESTCIs, that may affect the quality of chil-
has previously been shown (O'Connor, 2010) dren's relationships (Danielsen, Wiium, Wil-
but also with children's perceptions of the helmsen, & Wold, 2010). For example,
relationship. O'Connor (2010) found that in classrooms
with more ESTCIs, children not only had
Sense of Peer Community closer relationships with their teachers (based
on teacher report) but also exhibited less of a
We define a positive peer community as decline in closeness across the elementary
a classroom with high levels of perceived school grades. Moreover, children had a
prosocial behavior among children: Children greater sense of community when teachers
perceive their classmates to respect, care were warm, emphasized cooperation, and en-
about, and support one another. Children's couraged students to share their ideas, all of
perceptions of peer community have a general which are elements of ESTCIs (Schaps, Bat-
reference, which is distinct from the personal tistich, & Solomon, 2004). Finally, teachers
reference of their perceptions of peer support who received professional development that
(i.e., the degree to which a child personally addressed high-quality teacher-child interac-
receives emotional support from peers; Good- tion had more positive interactions among
enow, 1993; Wentzel et al., 2010). Our notion their students, perhaps because children ob-
of peer community draws from the work of served their teachers' positive and fair inter-
Battistich et al. (1995) on positive classroom actions with students and modeled this inter-
communities, which are associated with action style with peers (Mikami, Gregory,
greater intrinsic motivation. In light of evi- Allen, Pianta, & Lun, 2011).
dence that support from teachers and support
from peers are uniquely predictive of several ESTCIs are generally considered to be
outcomes (Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Good- positive features of classrooms, but they have
enow, 1993; Wentzel et al., 2010), we adapted not yet been linked to children's own percep-
the measure of classroom community of Bat- tions of relatedness. Identifying such a link
tistich et al. to focus almost exclusively on would open the door for research on interven-
perceptions of peer relationships. By distin- tions that increase teachers' ESTCIs in an
guishing perceptions of teacher closeness from attempt to improve children's relationships
perceptions of peer community, we can exam- with teachers and peers and, ultimately, chil-
ine whether the benefits of ESTCIs extend dren's motivation and engagement.
beyond the teacher-child relationship into the
peer community. Determinants of Relatedness
at the Child Level
ESTCIs as Classroom-Level
Within the bioecological framework, we
Determinants of Teacher-ChUd
focus on children's demand characteristics,
Closeness and Peer Community
which are features that "invite or discourage
The existing body of research on EST- reactions from the social environment that can
CIs has focused largely on children's aca- disrupt or foster processes of psychological
demic outcomes (e.g., Connor, Son, Hindman, growth" (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006, p.
& Morrison, 2005), with much less study of 796). In this section, we describe several child
possible mediating factors such as children's characteristics thought to influence teacher-
sense of relatedness. Given that children's re- child relationship quality and children's sense
ports of both teacher and peer relationship of peer community.
Relatedness in the Classroom

Gender and Age reported by both the teacher (Wu et al., 2010)
and child (Gest et al., 2005). Neither study
Girls are more likely than boys to have controlled for children's behavior problems,
close relationships with teachers, according to which may have accounted for the correlation.
both teacher and child reports of relationship Stronger evidence suggests that children with
quality (Gest, Welsh, & Domitrovich, 2005; low sociometric status believe that their peers
Jerome, Hamre, & Pianta, 2009). Teacher- do not support them (Salmivalli & Isaacs,
child relationships become less close across 2005) and often feel increasingly lonely across
the elementary school grades (Baker, 2006; the elementary school grades (Fontaine et al.,
O'Connor, 2010). It may be that teachers of 2009). What has not yet been explored is
younger students see the provision of emo- whether less-preferred children also perceive a
tional support as a core component of their poor peer community.
jobs, whereas upper-grade teachers believe
they should focus on instruction (Jerome et al., Potential Moderation Effects of ESTCIs
2009). Younger children also report stronger
peer communities (Gest et al., 2005). Hamre and Pianta (2005) posited in their
academic risk model that children who have
Aggressive-Disruptive Behavior the greatest risk for academic failure may ben-
efit the most from ESTCIs. We extend this
Some children are at risk for poor rela- model and suggest that ESTCIs will be espe-
tionships with teachers and peers because of cially beneficial to children with characteris-
behavior problems (Campbell, Spieker, tics that put them at risk for negative relation-
Burchinal, Poe, & NICHD Early Child Care ships (e.g., aggressive-disruptive children,
Research Network, 2006; Gest et al., 2005). less-preferred children, and boys). Such inter-
Dealing with disruptive children's classroom actions between children's demand character-
interruptions may frustrate teachers, resulting istics and their experiences of proximal pro-
in a strained relationship between the teacher cesses are a critical component of the bioeco-
and disruptive child (O'Connor, 2010). More- logical theory (Bronfenbrenner & Morris,
over, whereas most children show increases in 2006). For example, Buyse, Verschueren,
the quality of their friendships across middle Doumen, Van Damme, and Maes (2008)
childhood, physically aggressive children ac- found that children who were aggressive in
tually show decreases in their friendship qual- kindergarten were most likely to develop close
ity between the ages of 9 and 12 years (Camp- teacher-rated relationships with their first-
bell et al., 2006). grade teachers when the first-grade teachers
had high ESTCIs. ESTCIs may also be impor-
Peer Acceptance, Rejection, and Social tant for at-risk children's perceptions of peer
Preference community; such children may benefit the
most from the increased social skills and pos-
Whereas a child's perceived peer com-
itive peer interactions that have been linked to
munity refiects his or her sense of prosocial
high ESTCIs (Mikami et al., 2011; Mashbum
behavior among classmates, the sociometric
et al., 2008).
indices of peer acceptance, rejection, and so-
cial preference ("like most" minus "like Present Study
least") reflect the consensus of peers regarding
how liked or disliked the child is in the class- Given the importance of ESTCIs, we
room (Asher & Coie, 1990). The role of so- build on past research by considering child
ciometric status in the teacher-child relation- characteristics (gender, social preference, ag-
ship has not been well studied, but two corre- gressive-disruptive behavior) that may affect
lational analyses found that children who were relatedness and explore whether ESTCIs are
less preferred by their classmates tended to especially important for children at risk for
have less-close teacher-child relationships, as negative relationships. On the basis of the
89
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

literature reviewed earlier, we expected that participants were economically disadvan-


(a) children in classrooms with more ESTCIs taged. The average class sizes in Pennsylvania
would report closer relationships with their (M = 18 students, range = 12-25 students)
teachers and a stronger sense of peer commu- were slightly smaller than those in Illinois
nity; (b) older children, boys, aggressive-dis- (M = 20 students, range = 13-27 students).
ruptive children, and less-preferred children
would report lower scores on these measures Measures
of relatedness; and (c) the association between
ESTCIs and relatedness would be strongest for Social relatedness: Teacher-child
boys, aggressive-disruptive children, and closeness. Teacher-child closeness was con-
less-preferred children. ceptualized as the degree to which students
perceived a warm, caring relationship with
Method their teachers. Five items were adapted for
student report from the closeness subscale of
Participants the teacher-reported Student-Teacher Rela-
Children and teachers were involved in tionship Scale (Pianta, 2001), forming an in-
a study that examined social ecologies and ternally consistent scale at each assessment
child outcomes in elementary school class- point (a, = .84, a2 = .89). Intraclass corre-
rooms. Forty-one classrooms participated, lation coefficients (ICCs) indicated that scores
providing a total of 794 students in first, third, were similar across 8 weeks (averaged-wave
and fifth grades. Written consent was obtained ICC = 0.79, single-wave ICC = 0.65). Items
from classroom teachers and from the parents included "I trust my teacher," "I like being
of 645 students. After nonparticipating and around my teacher," "My teacher is kind to
absent students were accounted for, a total of me," "I feel safe when my teacher is around,"
635 students (80% of all possible students) and "My teacher respects me." Each item was
participated in at least one of the two survey rated on a scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always).
administrations {T¡ = 76%, T2 = 76%). One Scale scores were averaged across the two
classroom was excluded because of low stu- assessments. Because the single-wave ICC
dent participation (32%), and one child did not was in the upper end of "fair to good reliabil-
respond to the measures used in this study, ity" (0.40-0.75) as defined by Fleiss (1986,
which brought the final sample to 40 class- p. 7), we used the scale score from a single
rooms (15 first-grade classrooms, 14 third- time point for children who participated only
grade classrooms, and 11 fifth-grade class- once. Gest et al. (2005) established the validity
rooms) and 628 children (52% male). and utility of a similar measure (i.e., 4 of the 5
items were identical, whereas "My teacher
Schools were located in small-sized to
treats me fairly" was used in place of "My
midsized urban areas in central Illinois and
teacher respects me") in a sample of third to
rural areas in central Pennsylvania. Partici-
fifth graders and found that teacher-rated ag-
pants in Illinois were drawn from three
gression predicted declines in child-reported
schools and were racially diverse (45% Afri-
teacher closeness across a school year. Find-
can American, 41% White, 3% Asian, 5%
ings were in line with O'Connor (2010), who
Hispanic, and 6% other race). A total of 83%
also found that children with behavior prob-
of the participating children in Illinois were
lems had less-close relationships with their
considered economically disadvantaged based
teachers. At each time point in our study,
on receipt of free or reduced-price lunches. In
child-reported teacher-child closeness (the
Pennsylvania, students were drawn from four
average of the five items) was only modestly
schools within one school district. Reflecting
correlated with a four-item teacher-reported
the overall racial makeup of the district, par-
measure of closeness and confiict based on the
ticipating students in Pennsylvania were ra-
Student-Teacher Relationship Scale of Pianta
cially homogeneous (97% White, 1% Black,
(2001) (r,/ = 0.25, r,2 = 0.21); these correla-
1% Hispanic, and 0.3% Asian), and 43% of
90
Relatedness in the Classroom

tions were slightly higher than those found by the four items at each time point. Gest et al.
Wu et al. (2010) in a sample of at-risk third found that the scale showed acceptable inter-
graders. nal consistency at both time points, as well as
moderate stability across 6 months.
Social relatedness: Peer community.
Peer community was conceptualized as the Validity of relatedness scales. To ver-
degree to which students felt that their class- ify that our measures of teacher and peer re-
room was a place in which children respected latedness were indeed measuring distinct as-
and helped one another. Five items, drawn pects of relatedness, we examined the corre-
primarily from the Sense of Community Scale lations among scales across time. Specifically,
(Battistich et al., 1995) and reworded to focus we compared the stability of each scale to its
on the classroom context rather than the T¡-T2 cross-domain correlations, using Hotell-
school context, formed an internally consistent ing's test for dependent correlations. In the
scale (a, = .83, a2 = .82). Items included case of Tj teacher-child closeness, the stability
"Kids in my classroom work together to solve correlation {r = 0.68) was significantly stron-
problems," "People care about each other in ger than the correlation with Tj peer commu-
my classroom," "Kids in my classroom do nity [r = 0.37; t¿;i^53l) = 9.09, p < .001].
nice things for each other," "Kids in my class- Similarly, the stability of peer community
room help each other," and "My teacher and (r = 0.63) was significantly stronger than its
classmates treat each other with respect." We correlation with Tj teacher closeness
also calculated the scale dropping the final [r = 0.36; idiff(529) = 7.44, p < .001].
item because it mentions the teacher (and our
focus was on peers), but nearly all results that Child characteristics. Children's ag-
were initially significant at the p < .05 level gressive-disruptive behavior and social pref-
remained significant (the main effect of erence were assessed using peer nomination
teacher sensitivity/regard for student perspec- measures. Each participating child was asked
tives dropped from p = .04 to p = .07). We to circle the names of classmates who fit the
retained this item in the scale because it in- description provided in each item. For every
cludes reference to peers and does not substan- item, proportion scores were computed for
tially alter results. Each item was rated on a each child by dividing the number of received
scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always), and the five nominations by the number of participating
items were averaged to obtain a score at each children (other than the self). Thus, a child
time point. As with teacher-child closeness, who receives 10 nominations for "starts
scores from both time points were averaged fights" and has 20 participating classmates
before analysis and single scores were used would have a proportion score of 0.5 (i.e., 10
for children who participated only once of 20). In other words, half of all classmates
(averaged-wave ICC = 0.78, single-wave believe that child starts fights. The social pref-
ICC = 0.63). erence scale and aggression scale were each
computed using the proportion scores for sev-
Evidence for the potential reliability of
eral items, as described below.
our peer community scale comes from the
study of children's perceived school support- Two peer nomination items were used to
iveness performed by Gest et al. (2005), which assess social preference; Children indicated
used four of the items described earlier (ex- which of their classmates they would (a) "like
cluding "do nice things"), with the original most to play with" and (b) "like least to play
school-focused stems (e.g., "kids at my with." Social preference was calculated as the
school" rather than "kids in my classroom"). "like most" proportion score minus the "like
In their study, third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade least" proportion score, consistent with tradi-
children responded to the four items during tional practice in peer relations research
both the fall and spring of a single academic (Asher & Coie, 1990). Aggressive-disruptive
year. Scores were calculated as the average of behavior was measured with three peer-nom-

91
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

ination items: "These kids start fights," "These cluded peer-to-peer interactions (positive cli-
kids make fun of other kids," and "These kids mate/negative climate [reverse scored], PC/
get in trouble" (a, = .90, aj = .89). Across NC) and emotional support based on pure
the two assessments, stability was very strong teacher-child interactions (teacher sensitivity/
for both social preference (averaged-wave regard for student perspectives, TS/RSP).
ICC = 0.83, single-wave ICC = 0.72) and Each of these new composite scales was cre-
aggressive-disruptive behavior (averaged- ated by averaging the two dimensions in-
wave ICC = 0.93, single-wave ICC = 0.87), volved, retaining the original CLASS scale of
so the averages of the cross-wave scores were 1-7. The PC/NC and TS/RSP scales ranged
used for analyses. from 3.44-6.13 and 5.25-6.88, respectively.
Observers attended a 2-day CLASS
Ohserved teacher-child interac- training taught by a certified CLASS trainer.
tions. Teacher-student interaction quality was Reliability was assessed using five 20-minute
measured with the Classroom Assessment videos that had previously been given a gold-
Scoring System (CLASS) for kindergarten standard score. After the standard CLASS cer-
through Grade 3 (CLASS K-3; Pianta, La tification protocol, observers were considered
Paro, & Hamre, 2008). Each classroom was reliable when (a) at least 80% of their scores
observed for four 20-minute cycles, with 10 were within one point of the gold standard and
minutes allowed for scoring between each cy- (b) at least three of the five cycles for each
cle. The CLASS assesses 10 dimensions of dimension were within one point of the gold
interaction quality, with dimension scores of 1 standard.
to 7 representing low to high levels. The di- Of the 160 20-minute CLASS cycles
mensions are organized into three domains: observed for this study, 142 were scored by
emotional support, instructional support, and two observers. We selected this dual-observer
classroom organization. Given our belief that system to maximize the reliability of the
emotionally supportive interactions provide a scores used in analyses because rater variance
sense of security and attachment, and should is a critical source of measurement error for
therefore influence perceptions of relatedness, the CLASS procedure (Raudenbush, Martinez,
we focus on the four dimensions within the Bloom, Zhu, &Lin, 2008). The percent-within-
emotional support domain. Observers pro- one-point criterion indicated acceptable reli-
vided scores based on global ratings of quality ability based on cycle-specific ratings (posi-
rather than frequency counts of specific behav- tive climate = 86%, negative climate = 99%,
iors. The positive climate dimension describes teacher sensitivity = 77%, and regard for stu-
the extent to which classroom interactions dent perspectives = 74%). We also computed
(both teacher-child and child-child) were ICCs as a more stringent test of interrater
warm and respectful. The negative climate agreement based on each observer's average
dimension rates the frequency and intensity of score across the four 20-minute cycles: ICCs
teacher behaviors such as anger, yelling, and fell in the upper end of the fair-to-good range
teasing, as well as observed negativity among defined by Reiss (1986) (PC/NC composite
students. Teacher sensitivity includes teach- ICC = 0.73, TS/RSP composite ICC = 0.62).
ers' ability to anticipate and respond appropri- Ratings were averaged across both observers
ately to children's needs, both academic and to provide scores for analyses.
emotional, and the degree to which students The CLASS K-3 is intended for use in
appear to be comfortable using their teachers kindergarten through third grade, but the Na-
as resources. Regard for student perspectives tional Institute of Child Health and Human
measures the extent to which teachers allow Development's Study of Early Child Care and
children to have autonomy and let children's Youth Development successfully used the
interests guide them. Classroom Observation System (an earlier
We created two composite scales to dis- version of the CLASS) in prekindergarten
tinguish between emotional support that in- through fifth-grade classrooms. The same
92
Relatedness in the Classroom

three-domain solution was found in both lower group. This allowed the intercept to be inter-
and upper grades (Hamre, Pianta, Mashburn, preted as the expected level of relatedness for
& Downer, 2007), and predictive validity was a first-grade boy with average scores on all
demonstrated by emotionally supportive inter- covariates.
actions predicting growth in math and reading Because children in the same classroom
through fifth grade (Pianta, Belsky et al., are likely to be more sii^ilar than children in
2008). different classrooms, classmates' scores are
not independent and regular regression analy-
Procedure sis would be biased (West, Welch, & Galecki,
2007). Multilevel models address this concern
After receiving approval from the uni-
by modeling both classroom-level variance
versity's human subjects review board, we
and unique-individual variance. Our analyses
obtained informed consent from teachers.
follow the recommendations of West et al.
Once a teacher agreed to participate, parent
(2007) for analyzing children nested within
consent forms describing the project were sent
classrooms using SAS PROC MIXED to esti-
home with all children in the class. Participat-
mate multilevel models (SAS Institute, 2008).
ing teachers were reimbursed for their time.
We first ran unconditional models, which pre-
Classrooms were provided a pizza party if at
dict the dependent variable using only the
least 90% of parent consent forms were re-
fixed and random effects of the intercept; no
turned, regardless of how many parents con-
predictors are included in this model. We also
sented to participation. Classroom observa-
estimated a model without a random intercept
tions and student surveys were conducted
and used likelihood ratio tests (LRTs) to de-
early in the spring semester, and students com-
termine whether the random intercept model
pleted a second round of surveys approxi-
was a significant improvement over the model
mately 8 weeks later. We designed the two
without a random intercept. Models were es-
assessments to provide estimates of short-term
timated using restricted maximum likelihood
reliability of measures rather than to model
(REML) estimation. ICCs were calculated to
developmental change, which we would not
describe the proportion of variance in the de-
expect to occur across such closely spaced
pendent variable that is due to variation be-
assessments. Averaging across the two time
tween classrooms, rather than between stu-
points provided us with more reliable mea-
dents (West et al., 2007).
sures for participants who provided data at
both time points (87% of all participants). For
Testing random slopes. Random
the remaining participants, we used scores
slopes would indicate that the association be-
from the time point for which they provided
tween a child characteristic (e.g., aggressive-
data. Surveys were group administered by a
disruptive behavior) and relatedness (teacher
team of trained graduate research assistants for
closeness, peer community) varies across
the students in third and fifth grades. Students
classrooms. We examined the significance of
in first grade responded to survey questions in
the random slopes of gender, social prefer-
an individual interview with a trained research
ence, and aggressive-disruptive behavior in
assistant.
separate models containing all Level 1 fixed
Data Analysis Plan effects and a random intercept estimated with
REML. Using log-likelihood tests (LRTs), we
Appropriate data transformations were determined whether including the random
made for nonnormal distributions. Class size, slope was a significant improvement over a
CLASS scores, social preference, and aggres- model with the slope fixed at zero. Random
sive-disruptive behavior were grand mean slopes with negative or zero variance were not
centered; gender was coded as 0 for male considered in cross-level interactions. Given
and 1 for female. Grade was a categorical our a priori hypothesis for moderation effects,
variable, with first grade as the reference however, we proceeded to test cross-level in-

93
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

teractions when the random-slope estimate participating children infrequently skipped


was positive, regardless of its significance. items (range = I.2%-6.3% missing per item).
We imputed missing values using SAS PROC
Main-effects models. The first and MI (SAS Institute, 2008). Imputing multilevel
second hypotheses, which addressed the main data are more complex than imputing indepen-
effects of gender, aggressive-disruptive be- dent data because the imputation model must
havior, social preference, and ESTCIs on re- acknowledge the possibility of within-cluster
latedness, were tested in four separate models similarity (Graham, 2012). Researchers often
using REML estimation. Each model included include dummy-coded classroom indicators as
fixed effects for either PC/NC or TS/RSP, as auxiliary variables in the imputation model to
well as child-level predictors, classroom-level retain within-cluster similarity, but this ap-
covariates (grade, class size), and a random proach may result in ICCs that are substan-
intercept. A fixed effect for school was also tially larger than those in real life (Andridge,
included by dummy coding the seven schools 2011). On the basis of the recommendations of
because there were too few schools to justify a Graham (2012), we imputed two sets of 20
third level in the multilevel model. The effect datasets: the first set included classroom indi-
of school membership was not a significant cators and the second set did not, although
predictor in any models. other classroom-level variables (e.g., class
size, CLASS scores) were included. Both of
Moderation models. To test our third
our imputation models included several other
hypothesis—that is, ESTCIs are most impor-
auxiliary variables (e.g., child gender, child-
tant for children who are at risk of developing
reported motivation) to improve the imputa-
negative relationships—we estimated a series
tion model. We used SAS PROC MIANA-
of moderation models. Each model included a
LYZE (SAS Institute, 2008) to combine re-
random intercept, the random slope(s) previ-
sults from multilevel models. For LRTs, we
ously identified, all fixed effects, and the in-
calculated the log-likelihood difference for
teraction between any child characteristic(s)
each imputed dataset and then combined
that had random slopes and either PC/NC or
the 40 difference scores and referred them to
TS/RSP. Models were first estimated using
the appropriate x^ distribution(s) to obtain an
maximum likelihood estimation, which is the
overall significance value (Li, Meng, Raghu-
preferred technique for testing the significance
nathan, & Rubin, 1991).
of fixed effects (West et al, 2007). LRTs were
used to determine whether the addition of the Results
interaction term was a significant improve-
ment over a model with the random slope but Descriptive Statistics and Data
without interaction. The parameter estimates Transformations
for the final models were obtained using
REML estimation. Although we did not hy- Descriptive statistics are presented in
pothesize that grade level would moderate the Table 1. On average, children reported high
effects of either TS/RSP or PC/NC on relat- teacher closeness. Teacher closeness was
edness, we tested the possibility that the dif- highest in first grade (M = 4.47, SD = 0.69),
ferent survey formats might have led to dif- followed by third grade (M = 4.31,
ferential associations between ESTCIs and re- SD = 0.78) and fifth grade (M = 3.97, SD =
latedness outcomes in first grade versus third 1). Because most children reported very close
grade or fifth grade. None of these interaction relationships with their teachers, data were
effects were significant, so they were omitted negatively skewed (M = 4.29, median = 4.60)
from the models. and were transformed via exponentiation be-
fore analysis. Children in Pennsylvania per-
Missing data. Analyzing only those ceived closer relationships with their teachers
children who responded to all items can lead (M = 4.37) than did children in Illinois
to biased results (Graham, 2012). In our study. (M = 4.16; t = -2.43, p = .02). Ratings of
94
Relatedness in the Classroom

Tahle 1
Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Measures of Relatedness,
Child Characteristics, and CLASS Scores
4 M SD Minimum Maximum

1. Perceived teacher closetiess .48* -.29* .11* .17* - . 2 3 * 4.260.79 1 5


2. Perceived peer community — -.08* .08* -.03 -.38* 3.640.87 1 5
3. Aggressive-disruptive behavior — -.44* -.29* .20* 0.180.19 0 0.93
4. Social preference — .06 -.08* 0.030.29 - 1 0.77
5. Gender (0 = male, 1 = female) — .02 304 girls, 324 boys
6. Grade — 234 first, 235 third, 159 fifth
CLASS dimension 10 11 12 M SD Minimum Maximum
7. Positive climate (PC) .51* .74* .73* .97* .81* 5.32 0.69 4.13 6.75
8. Negative climate (NC) — -.38* - . 4 1 * - . 7 1 * -.44* 1.20 0.26 1 2
9. Teacher sensitivity (TS) — .64* .71* .89* 4.82 0.67 3.63 6.25
10. Regard for student perspectives
(RSP) — .72* .92* 4.40 0.78 2.88 6.13
11. PC/NC^-" .79* 4.61 0.66 3.44 6.13
12. TS/RSP" — 6.05 0.43 5.25 6.88

Note. CLASS = Classroom Assessment Scoring System. N = 40 classrooms and 628 children. Correlations are based
on nonimputed data.
"Composite scales are the average of the two CLASS dimensions.
""Negative climate was reverse scored before averaging.
*/; < .05.

peer community were lower but still above the were negatively correlated (r = -0.44, p <
scale midpoint; again, first graders had higher .001). Teachers generally provided moderately
scores (first grade: M = 4.04, SD = 0.78; third high ESTCIs (see Table 1). The four dimen-
grade: M = 3.59, SD = 0.82; fifth grade: sions were moderately to strongly
M = 3.20, SD = 0.82). Children in Pennsyl- intercorrelated.
vania perceived slightly more peer community
(M = 3.76) compared with children in Illinois Between-CIassroom Differences in
(M = 3.53), but the difference was not signif- Relatedness
icant (i = -1.61, p = .11). Perceptions of
teacher closeness and peer community were The LRTs for the unconditional models
moderately intercorrelated (/• = 0.48, p < showed significant between-classroom differ-
.001). ences in both teacher closeness and peer com-
On average, children were perceived munity ip < .001). ICCs indicated that 13%
to be aggressive-disruptive by 18% of their of the variance in teacher closeness was due to
classmates (M = 0.18, SD = 0.19); scores between-classroom differences, as was 20% of
were square root transformed to address the the variance in peer community.
positive skew that resulted from very few
highly aggressive children. Social prefer- ESTCIs as Determinants of Children's
ence scores indicated that children were Relatedness
generally liked more than they were disliked
by classmates, but there was wide variabil- Perceived teacher closeness. Results
ity (M = 0.03, SD = 0.29). Aggressive- from the main-effects models are presented
disruptive behavior and social preference in Table 2. In comparison with first graders,
95
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

Table 2
Predicting Perceived Relatedness From Observed Emotionally
Supportive Interactions
Perceived Teacher Closeness Perceived Peer Community

Main Effects CLASS Main Effects CLASS


Only" Moderation^ Only" Moderation''

Predictor b SE b SE b SE b

Level 1
Intercept 95.09* 8.75 95.58* 8.76 4.18* 0.14 4.20* 0.14
First grade (reference group) — — — — — — — —
Third grade -6.42 7.09 -6.36 7.12 -0.40* 0.11 -0.40* 0.11
Fifth grade -26.59* 7.09 -26.75* 8.00 -0.86* 0.12 -0.86* 0.12
Gender 16.61* 4.62 16.59* 4.49 -0.02 0.08 -0.02 0.07
Social preference -3.51 7.92 -3.12 7.92 0.12 0.15 0.15 0.15
Aggressive-disruptive
behavior -30.69* 10.70 -29.84* 10.71 0.10 0.18 0.13 0.18
Level 2
Class size -0.85 1.41 -0.95 1.41 -0.03 0.02 -0.03 0.02
TS/RSP composite'^ 12.16* 5.20 17.79* 6.02 0.16* 0.08 0.31* 0.09
TS/RSP X Gender -10.51 6.69 -0.28* 0.11
TS/RSP X Social
Preference 0.18 0.22
PC/NC composite'^ 12.40 8.98 17.15 10.97 0.21 0.14 0.32 0.17
PC/NC X Gender -7.37 10.48 -0.23 0.18
PC/NC X Social
Preference 0.76* 0.31

Note. CLASS = Classroom Assessment Scoring System; PC/NC = positive climate/negative climate (reversed);
TS/RSP = teacher support/regard for student perspectives. N = 628 children and 40 classrooms. Multilevel modeling
accounts for nesting within classrooms. Models control for the fixed effect of school membership {n = 7); the omnihus
test for effect of school was nonsignificant. Class size, social preference, aggressive-disruptive hehavior, and CLASS
scores are grand mean centered. For gender, 0 indicates male.
'A random intercept and random slope were included for gender.
•"A random intercept and random slopes were included for gender and social preference.
"Effects of TS/RSP and PC/NC were estimated in separate models. Estimates of child-level predictors and class size are
from the TS/RSP model. (Estimates were nearly identical in the PC/NC model.)
*p < .05.

perceptions of teacher closeness were lower associated with closeness. There was no over-
among fifth graders {p < .001) but not third all effect of school assignment on teacher
graders. Girls perceived more closeness than closeness.
boys {p < .001). Social preference was unre- The series of random-slope models (Ta-
lated to teacher closeness, and aggressive- ble 3) indicated that the random slope of gen-
disruptive behavior was associated with less der was positive but not significant {p = .21).
teacher closeness {p < .01). Class size was Moderation models thus explored cross-level
unrelated to closeness. Teachers' observed interactions of ESTCIs with gender. Nonsig-
TS/RSP scores were associated with greater nificant LRTs indicated that the interactions
closeness {p < .05), whereas PC/NC was not did not improve the fit of the TS/RSP or
96
Relatedness in the Classroom

Table 3
Variance Components of Random-Slope Coefficients
Teacher-Child Closeness Peer Community

Random Slope Variance Component SE Variance Component SE

Gender 217.29 183.84 .21 0.06 0.05 .10


Social preference 0" 0.23 0.16 .09
Aggressive-disruptive behavior 0" 0"

"Signifieance is based on likelihood ratio tests in which the —2 restricted maximum likelihood differences were referred
to a mixture of x^ distributions (Xi^, X2^)-
''At least one imputed dataset estimated a variance component that was negative or zero. Because the variance
component was not significant in any single imputed dataset, we concluded that the random effect should be fixed at
zero.

PC/NC models. We concluded that main-ef- Post hoc analyses. To clarify whether
fects models were the preferred model. positive climate or negative climate drove the
null effects of the PC/NC composite score, we
Perceived peer community. The conducted post hoc analyses in which positive
main-effects models predicting sense of peer climate and negative climate were tested in
community are presented in Table 2. Both separate models. Results indicated that posi-
third and fifth graders perceived less peer tive climate was not associated with teacher
community compared with first graders (p < closeness (b = 5.35, p = .34) or peer com-
.001). Gender, social preference, aggressive- munity (b = 0.07, p = .39) whereas negative
disruptive behavior, and class size were unre- climate was marginally associated with
lated to perceived peer community. TS/RSP teacher closeness (b = —25.61, /? = .06) and
was associated with higher perceptions of peer significantly associated with peer community
community (p < .05), but PC/NC was not. (b = -0.52, p = .01).
There was no overall effect of school assign-
ment on sense of peer community. Discussion
The random-slopes models (see Table 3)
indicated that both gender and social prefer- The goal of this study was to examine
ence had positive, nonsignificant slope vari- predictors of children's sense of relatedness to
ance between classrooms. In the TS/RSP teachers and peers in elementary school. We
model, the LRT indicated a significant im- hypothesized that teachers' emotionally sup-
provement when the interactions were in- portive interactions with students would be
cluded (p < .05). The Gender X TS/RSP associated with greater relatedness and that
aggressive-disruptive behaviors and peer re-
interaction was significant (p < .05): TS/RSP
lationship difficulties would be associated
was positively associated with boys' perceived
with lower perceived relatedness. In this sec-
peer community but not girls' perceived peer
tion, we discuss the results in terms of each
community (see Figure la). The interaction
predictor of relatedness.
model was also preferred in the PC/NC inter-
action model (p < .05 for LRTs). Only the Grade and Gender Differences in
social preference X PC/NC interaction was Perceived Relatedness
significant: The association between PC/NC
and peer community was highest among the Children in the lower grades reported
most-preferred children (see Figure lb). closer relationships with their teachers; even

97
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

— 3rd Grade Boys


•-3rd Grade Girls

4 6
Observed Teacher Sensitivity/Regard for Studetit Perspectives
(TS/RSP)

•Low Peer Social


Preference
• High Peer Social
Preference

5.5 6.5
Observed Positive Climate/Negative Ciitnate (PC/NC)

Figure 1. Associations between emotionally supportive teacher-child interac-


tions and children's perceptions of peer community varied across levels of
moderators. (A) For hoys, hut not for girls, teacher sensitivity/regard for
student perspectives (TS/RSP) was associated with greater perceptions of peer
community. (B) Positive climate/negative climate (PC/NC) was more strongly
associated with perceived peer community for children with greater social
preference.
98
Relatedness in the Classroom

SO, fifth graders had relatively high levels of tributing to more negative interactions be-
closeness (M = 3.97 on a scale of 1-5). Fifth tween teachers and boys. Although previous
graders may have more realistic perceptions of evidence suggests that girls perceive a more
their relationships with teachers. Such an ex- supportive peer community (Gest et al., 2005),
planation is in line with research on the devel- we found no gender differences; however, this
opmental trajectory of children's self-concept: was qualified by the interaction effect ad-
Increasing cognitive capacity, including the dressed below.
ability to compare one's abilities with the abil-
ities of peers, is associated with a shift from Teachers' Emotional Support and
idealistic to more realistic academic and social Children's Perceptions of Relatedness
self-concepts from early childhood through It is worth noting that there was no ef-
adolescence (Dweck, 2002). Furthermore, it is fect of school assignment on either teacher
likely that only a small subset of children in closeness or peer community. That is, the
the upper grades perceived negative relation- teacher/classroom to which a child was as-
ships with their teachers. For example, Gest et signed was more important than the school
al. (2005) similarly found that the majority of assignment in determining the quality of chil-
third through fifth graders perceived extremely dren's relationships. Nye, Konstantop-
positive relationships with their teachers. The oulus, and Hedges (2004) similarly found
small number of children who reported more teacher assignment to be more important than
ambivalent feelings about their teachers also school assignment for academic achievement
tended to be more aggressive. Finally, re- from kindergarten through third grade. It may
search suggests that teacher-child relationship be that school membership is a stronger pre-
quality declines after the transition to middle dictor of relatedness in middle school, when
school, perhaps because of the increased em- children begin changing teachers for different
phasis on normative grading and teacher con- subjects. Taken together, these results vali-
trol (Eccles & Roeser, 2009). Our results sug- dated our exploration of teaching practices
gest that relationship quality may already be that tnay explain differences in children's per-
declining in the later elementary school years, ceived relatedness. Indeed, our hypothesis that
although the decrease is not dramatic. children in classrooms with more emotionally
First graders in our study also had the supportive teachers would perceive greater
strongest sense of peer community. Anderman levels of relatedness was generally supported.
(2003) speculated that decreasing perceptions
of support from peers (a similar but not iden- Perceptions of teacher closeness.
tical concept) may be due to the increasing Consistent with previous research (Buyse et
importance of social comparison in upper el- al, 2008; Danielsen et al., 2010), we found
ementary school (e.g., Dweck, 2002). More- significant between-classroom differences in
over, preadolescence is a time of decreased children's perceptions of teacher closeness.
self-competence for many youths (Jacobs, Danielsen et al. describe this effect as a "sys-
Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002). It tematic agreement" among classmates regard-
may be that children's sense of community is ing their relationships with their teachers.
negatively affected when children frequently Some teachers may easily develop relation-
compare themselves with classmates and view ships with students (Hamre et al., 2008),
peers as sources of competition rather than whereas other teachers struggle to develop
support. close relationships or do not see relationship
Consistent with previous research (Fur- building as a goal.
rer & Skinner, 2003; Gest et al., 2005), girls As expected, children felt closer to
perceived closer relationships with their teach- teachers who were aware of their students'
ers than did boys. Whereas girls tend to be needs, effectively responded to students' so-
more prosocial, boys are more likely to disrupt cial and academic problems, did not rigidly
class (Birch & Ladd, 1998), potentially con- adhere to their teaching plan when students
99
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

were interested in a related topic, granted stu- dren who are consistently exposed to sensi-
dents autonomy, and provided leadership op- tive, caring interactions between teachers and
portunities (Pianta, La Paro, & Hamre, 2008). students may model these qualities in their
It may be that teachers who see students as own interactions with peers (Gest & Rodkin,
individuals with distinct social and academic 2011).
needs are better able to support their students Moderation models, however, indicated
and foster a warm relationship. Because the that although these sensitive and autonomy-
TS/RSP composite score was absent of peer- supportive teacher-child interactions were
to-peer infiuences, we- essentially confirmed important for sense of peer community among
that these teacher-child interactions, which boys, they were not important for girls. These
are intended to be indicators of the attach- findings support our extension of the academic
ment-like relationship between the teacher and risk model (Hamre & Pianta, 2005) in that
child, are indeed associated with children's boys may have more to gain from having
feelings of a close and secure relationship with emotionally supportive teachers, given that
the teacher. boys generally perceive less support from their
The weaker association between class- classmates than do girls (Demaray & Malecki,
room climate (PC/NC composite score) and 2002; Gest et al., 2005). Another possibility is
teacher closeness suggests that observed peer- that emotionally supportive teachers minimize
to-peer interactions may not be important in negative experiences that disproportionately
the development of close teacher-student re- affect boys, such as physical aggression (Zim-
lationships. However, post hoc analyses sug- mer-Gembeck, Geiger, & Crick, 2005). Gen-
gest that a negative climate may have detri- der interactions should be interpreted with
mental effects on teacher closeness, whereas a caution, however, because they did not emerge
positive climate does not have beneficial ef- in the prediction of perceived teacher
fects. A classroom with a negative climate is closeness.
characterized by fighting among students, sar-
castic language from the teacher, and harsh Characteristics of the Child and
discipline. Even students who typically have Perceptions of Relatedness
warm relationships with teachers may struggle We also explored whether aggressive-
to be close to a teacher in this toxic environ- disruptive or less-preferred children had a
ment. In contrast, classrooms with a low pos- greater risk of relational problems, as well as
itive climate simply have fewer positive inter- whether teachers' emotional support protected
actions (Pianta, La Paro, & Hamre, 2008), against negative outcomes. According to sys-
which may not prevent the formation of warm tems theories (e.g., Bronfenbrenner & Morris,
relationships among children who already tend 2006), the implications of a child's character-
to be close to their teachers. istics may differ depending on the child's
context.
Perceptions of peer community. Our
hypothesis that emotionally supportive inter- Perceived teacher closeness. Consis-
actions between teachers and students would tent with our hypothesis and previous research
be associated with children's sense of peer (e.g., Gest et al., 2005), children who were
community was partially confirmed. In the aggressive or disruptive felt less close to their
main-effects model, children whose teachers teachers. Teachers' interactions with children
were sensitive to children's needs and pro- who have emotional and behavioral problems
vided opportunities for children's autonomy tend to be discipline focused, with many fewer
reported a stronger sense of peer community. interactions involving praise for academic or
This finding was consistent with a systems social behavior (Sutheriand, 2000). It is diffi-
perspective, which states that teacher-student cult to imagine a close teacher-child relation-
interactions are expected to infiuence the peer ship developing when interactions tend to be
relational system (Mikami et al, 2011). Chil- reprimands.
100
Relatedness in the Classroom

Contrary to our expectations, low social receive evidence-based classroom practices


preference was not associated with teacher relating to either academic instruction or the
closeness. The peer relationship problems of promotion of positive behavior (Tier 1); more
less-preferred children did not extend into the intensive services are provided for children
teacher-child system. The lack of moderation whose problems persist. Our results suggest
effects in the teacher closeness models sug- that professional development programs that
gests that having an emotionally supportive improve teachers' interactions with students
teacher is universally important for developing may have a place alongside other Tier 1 pro-
a close teacher-child relationship, regardless grams (Pianta, Mashburn, Downer, Hatnre, &
of a child's behavior or social preference. Justice, 2008). For example, the My Teaching-
Partner (MTP) professional development pro-
Sense of peer community. Given that gram (Pianta, Mashburn et al., 2008) targets
rejected and aggressive-disruptive children the dimensions of teacher-child interaction
struggle with their relationships with peers quality that are the foundation of the CLASS
(Gest et al., 2005; Salmivalli & Isaacs, 2005), measure. In addition to providing teachers
we were surprised that the main-effects model with access to online videos that illustrate
showed no differences in peer community teachers succeeding in each dimension, the
based on aggressive-disruptive behavior or MTP program provides coaches who observe
social preference. One possible explanation is teachers' lessons and provide individualized
that children distinguished between their own feedback throughout the year.
personal relationships and their objective per-
Until evidence-based programs such as
ceptions of the peer community. That is, ag-
the MTP program are widely available, school
gressive-disruptive and less-preferred chil-
psychologists can become educated in the
dren may perceive their classrooms to be pos-
CLASS framework and coach teachers using
itive spaces despite their own interpersonal
the same observation-feedback cycles that are
problems. We did find a strong moderation
critical to the success of the MTP program
effect, in that children with high social pref-
(Pianta, Mashburn et al, 2008). For example,
erence did have a greater sense of peer com-
the school psychologist might compliment the
munity, but only in classrooms high in PC/NC.
teacher for performing specific actions that
Implications and Limitations of Study contribute to a positive climate and, with the
teacher, brainstorm additional opportunities
Most school personnel will not be sur- for similar actions. Suggestions should be im-
prised that teachers who have emotionally mediately relevant to the teacher's current
supportive interactions with children have bet- classroom (Pianta, Mashburn et al, 2008); for
ter relationships with their students, but it is example, they may identify specific ways the
notable that teacher-child interactions may teacher can increase support for a particular
contribute to peer relationship quality as well. child. The Classroom Checkup program
These findings have many implications for (Reinke, Lewis-Palmer, & Merrell, 2008) pro-
teachers, school psychologists, and principals, vides similar opportunities for school psychol-
who are increasingly interested in improving ogists to support teachers' positive interac-
teacher-child relationships and fostering a tions with students. In this program, a school
sense of community—often with the goal of psychologist completes a 20-minute observa-
promoting student motivation (Wentzel & tion that quantifies rates of specific behaviors
Wigfield, 2007). (e.g., rates of praise and reprimands). A
First, many schools have adopted tiered unique feature of the Classroom Checkup con-
approaches to fostering academic achievement sultation model is a motivational interviewing
and good behavior, including Response to In- framework in which the observer (psycholo-
tervention (RTI) and Positive Behavioral In- gist) reviews the observation record with the
terventions and Support (PBIS; Sugai & teacher and engages in a dialogue in which the
Horner, 2006). In these models, all students teacher identifies targeted areas for change.
101
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

A second implication is that teachers Second, the ICCs for CLASS dimension
should focus on building relationships with scores were fair to good, but not excellent
aggressive-disruptive children. Making teach- (Fleiss, 1986). Limited reliability is a greater
ers aware of their tendency to have negative concern when there are null findings, as poor
interactions with such children could help reliability can mask true associations. Instead,
teachers be more intentional in their interac- we found significant associations between
tions (Sutherland, 2000), especially if teachers composite CLASS scales and both teacher-
are informed of the importance of close rela- child closeness and sense of peer communi-
tionships for aggressive children's academic ty—in fact, larger effect sizes or more statis-
success (Baker, 2006). Upper elementary tically significant effects may emerge if teach-
school teachers might need special assistance er-child interactions can be measured more
because they may view relationship building reliably.
as less important than preparing students
Third, we cannot be sure whether the
for standardized tests and middle school.
higher scores for teacher closeness and peer
School psychologists should also recognize
community among first graders are solely due
that aggressive children may not be able to
to a greater sense of relatedness among
develop close relationships with the primary
younger children. The interview format used
disciplinarian (i.e., the teacher). Psychologists
for first graders may have increased social
might connect aggressive children with other
adults in the school to ensure that the children desirability compared with third and fifth
benefit from a relationship with a nonparental graders. Furthermore, first graders do not have
adult. the same vocabulary and linguistic abilities of
older students, which may have led first grad-
Our study has several limitations. First, ers to respond in the affirmative when they did
this study was not designed to determine not understand the question. Still, we observed
whether there are causal relationships among
a gradual decline in perceived relatedness after
variables. All of our measures were collected
first grade rather than a precipitous drop, sug-
during the second half of the school year, after
gesting that the grade effects may be attributed
classroom relationships had been established.
to true changes in relationship quality rather
It could be that teachers observed to provide
than response format.
low emotional support at the time of observa-
tion were attempting to work with an excep- An additional limitation is that we do
tionally difficult group of students. Indeed, the not know whether teachers' emotionally sup-
association between teacher-child interac- portive interactions are associated with chil-
tions and children's outcomes is likely recip- dren's perceptions of specific forms of support
rocal (Gest & Rodkin, 2011). Longitudinal from teachers and peers, such as academic
studies and cluster randomized trials are support or clear expectations for behavior
needed to determine whether targeting teach- (e.g., Wentzel et al., 2010); further exploration
ers' emotionally supportive interactions is an of differential effects on multiple dimensions
effective strategy for improving students' of social support is desirable. Finally, our
sense of relatedness. The field is headed in just study highlighted important between-class-
this direction; a recent ttial of a professional room differences in children's sense of relat-
development program grounded in the CLASS edness and the potential impact of teachers'
framework showed that teachers in the inter- emotionally supportive interactions. Within a
vention condition had more emotionally sup- single classroom, however, teachers likely had
portive interactions with their students (Pianta, more supportive interactions with some stu-
Mashburn et al., 2008), as well as higher- dents than with others; as such, our study does
quality peer interactions in their classrooms, not provide a full accounting of the role of
as compared with control classrooms (Mikami warm, autonomy-supportive, and sensitive in-
et al., 2011). teractions for individual students.
102
Relatedness in the Classroom

Conclusion Connor, C. M., Son, S. H., Hindman, A. H., & Morrison,


F. } . (2005). Teacher qualifications, family character-
This study underscores the importance istics, and preschool experience: Complex effects on
first graders' vocabulary and early reading outcomes.
of considering interdependencies among the Journal of School Psychology, 43, 343-375.
different relational systems in the classroom. Danielsen, A. G., Wiium, N., Wilhelmsen, B. U., & Wold,
Several research groups have bridged the his- B. (2010). Perceived support provided by teachers and
classmates and students' self-reported academic initia-
torical gap between studies of teacher-student tive. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 247-267.
relationships and studies of peer relationships, Deci, E. L., Vallerland, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan,
showing the unique roles of each relational R. M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self-
determination perspective. Educational Psychologist,
system in student development. Our results
26, 325-346.
suggest that it may be productive to examine Demaray, M. K., & Malecki, C. K. (2002). Critical levels
direct connections between these two rela- of perceived social support associated with student
tional systems. adjustment. School Psychology Quarterly, 17, 213-
241.
Dweck, C. S. (2002). The development of ability concep-
References tions. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Develop-
ment of achievement motivation (pp. 57-88). San Di-
Anderman, L. H. (2003). Academic and social perceptions ego, CA: Academic Press.
as predictors of change in middle school students' Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2009). Schools, academic
sense of school belonging. The Journal of Experimen- motivation, and stage-environment fit. In R. M. Lemer
tal Education, 72, 5-22. & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psy-
Andridge, R. R. (2011). Quantifying the impact of fixed chology. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
effects modeling of clusters in multiple imputation for Fleiss, J. L. (1986). The design and analysis of clinical
cluster randomized trials. Biometrical Journal, 53, 57- experiments. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
74. Fontaine, R. G., Yang, C , Burks, V. S., Dodge, K. A.,
Asher, S. R., & Coie. J. D. (1990). Peer rejection in Price, J. M., Pettit, G., & Bates, J. E. (2009). Loneli-
childhood. New York, NY: Cambridge University ness as a partial mediator of the relation between low
Press. social preference in childhood and anxious/depressed
Baker, J. A. (2006). Contributions of teacher-child rela- symptoms in adolescence. Development and Psycho-
tionships to positive school adjustment during elemen- pathology, 21, 479-491.
tary school. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 211- Furrer, C , & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as
229. a factor in children's academic engagement and per-
Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Kim, D., Watson, M., & formance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,
Schaps, E. (1995). Schools as communities, poverty 148-162.
levels of student populations, and students' attitudes, Gest, S. D., & Rodkin, P. C. (2011). Teaching practices
motives, and performance: A multilevel analysis.
and elementary classroom peer ecologies. Journal of
American Educational Research Journal, 32, 627-
Applied Developmental Psychology, 32, 288-296.
658.
Gest, S. D., Welsh, J. A., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2005).
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to
Behavioral predictors of changes in social relatedness
belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fun-
and liking school in elementary school. Journal of
damental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
School Psychology, 43, 281-301.
117, 497-529.
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Children's interper- Goodenow, C. (1993). Classroom belonging among early
sonal behaviors and the teacher-child relationship. De- adolescent students: Relationships to motivation and
velopmental Psychology, 34, 934-946. achievement. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 13,
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment. In Attachment and loss 21-43.
(Vol. I). New York, NY: Basic Books. Graham, J. W. (2012). Missing data: Analysis and design.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (2006). The bioecologi- New York, NY: Springer.
cal model of human development. In R. M. Lemer & Hamre, B., Pianta, R., Mashburn, A., & Downer, J.
W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: The- (2007). Building a science of classrooms: Application
oretical models of human development (5th ed.. Vol. of the CLASS framework in over 4,000 U.S. early
1). New York, NY: Wiley. childhood and elementary classrooms. New York, NY:
Buyse, E., Verschueren, K., Doumen, S., Van Damme, J., Foundation for Child Development.
& Maes, F. (2008). Classroom problem behavior and Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Can instructional
teacher-child relationships in kindergarten: The mod- and emotional support in the first-grade classroom
erating role of classroom climate. Journal of School make a difference for children at risk of school failure?
Psychology, 46, 367-391. Child Development, 76, 949-967.
Campbell, S. B., Spieker, S., Burchinal, M., Poe, M. D., & Hamre, B. K., Pianta, R. C , Downer, J. T., & Mashburn,
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2006). A. J. (2008). Teachers' perception of conflict with
Trajectories of aggression from toddlerhood to age 9 young students: Looking beyond problem behaviors.
predict academic and social functioning through Social Development, 17, 115-136.
age 12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Hughes, J. N. (2011). Longitudinal effects of teacher and
47, 791-800. student perceptions of teacher-student relationship

103
School Psychology Review, 2014, Volume 43, No. 1

qualities on academic adjustment. The Elementary Pianta, R. C , & Stuhlman, M. W. (2004). Teacher-child
School Journal, 112, 38-60. relationships and children's success in the first years of
Hughes, J. N., Luo, W., Kwok, O., & Loyd, L. K. (2008). school. School Psychology Review, 33, 444-458.
Teacher-student support, effortful engagement, and Raudenbush, S. W., Martinez, A., Bloom, H., Zhu, P., &
achievement; A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Lin, F. (2008). The reliability of group-level measures
Educational Psychology, 100, 1-14. and the power of group-randomized studies [Working
Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & paper]. Chicago, IL; University of Chicago.
Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children's self-com- Reinke, W. M., Lewis-Palmer, T., & Merrell, K. (2008).
petence and values; Gender and domain differences The classroom check-up; A classwide teacher consul-
across grades one through twelve. Child Development, tation model for increasing praise and decreasing dis-
73, 509-527. ruptive behavior. School Psychology Review, 37, 315-
Jerome, E. M., Hamre, B., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Teach- 332.
er-child relationships from kindergarten to sixth grade; Salmivalli, C , & Isaacs, J. (2005). Prospective relations
Early childhood predictors of teacher-perceived con- among victimization, rejection, friendlessness, and
flict and closeness. Social Development, 18, 915-945. children's self- and peer-perceptions. Child Develop-
Li, K.-H., Meng, X.-L., Raghunathan, T. E., & Rubin, ment, 76, 1161-1171.
D. B. (1991). Significance levels from repeated p-val- SAS Institute (2008). SAS System for Windows (Ver-
ues with multiply-imputed data. Statistica Sinica, 1, sion 9.2) [Software]. Cary. NC; Author.
65-92. Schaps, E., Battistich, V., & Solomon, D. (2004). Com-
Maddox, S. J., & Prinz, R. J. (2003). School bonding in munity in school as key to student growth; Findings
children and adolescents; Conceptualization, assess- from the Child Development Project. In J. Zins, R.
ment, and associated variables. Clinical Child and Weissberg, M. Wang, & H. Walberg (Eds.,), Building
Family Psychology Review, 6, 31-49. academic success on social and emotional learning:
Mashburn, A. J., Pianta, R. C , Hamre, B. K., Downer, What does the research say? (pp. 189-205). New
J. T., Barbarin, O. A., Bryant, D., . . . Howes, C. York, NY; Teachers College Press.
(2008). Measures of classroom quality in prekindergar- Sugai, G., & Horner, R. R. (2006). A promising approach
ten and children's development of academic, language, for expanding and sustaining school-wide positive be-
and social skills. Child Development, 79, 732-749. havior support. School Psychology Review, 35, 245-
Mikami, A. Y., Gregory, A., Allen, J. P., Pianta, R. C , & 259.
Lun, J. (2011). Effects of a teacher professional devel- Sutherland, K. S. (2000). Promoting positive interactions
opment intervention on peer relationships in secondary between teachers and students with emotional/behav-
classrooms. School Psychology Review, 40, 367-385. ioral disorders. Preventing School Failure, 44, 110-
Myers, S. S., & Pianta, R. C. (2008). Developmental 115.
commentary; Individual and contextual influences on Wentzel, K. R., Battle, A., Russell, S. L., & Looney, L. B.
student-teacher relationships and children's early prob- (2010). Social supports from teachers and peers as
lem behaviors. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent predictors of academic and social motivation. Contem-
Psychology, 37, 600-608. porary Educational Psychology, 36, 193-202.
Nye, B., Konstantopoulus, S., & Hedges, L. V. (2004). Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (2007). Motivational
How large are teacher effects? Educational Evaluation interventions that work; Themes and remaining issues.
and Policy Analysis, 26. 237-257. Educational Psychologist. 42, 261-271.
O'Connor, E. (2010). Teacher-child relationships as dy- West, B. T., Weich, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2007).
namic systems. Journal of School Psychology, 48, Linear mixed models: A practical guide using statisti-
187-218. cal software. Boca Raton, FL; Chapman & Hall.
Pianta, R. C. (2001). Student-teacher relationship scale. Wu, J., Hughes, J. N., & Kwok, O. (2010). Teacher-
Lutz, FL; Psychological Assessment Resources. student relationship quality type in elementary grades;
Pianta, R. C , Belsky, J., Vandergrift, N.. Houts, R., & Effects on trajectories for achievement and engage-
Morrison, F. (2008). Classroom effects on children's ment. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 357-387.
achievement trajectories in elementary school. Ameri- Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Geiger, T. C , & Crick, N. R.
can Education Research Journal, 49, 365-397. (2005). Relational and physical aggression, prosocial
Pianta, R. C , La Paro, K. M., & Hamre. B. K. (2008). The behavior, and peer relations; Gender moderation and
classroom assessment scoring system (CLASS). Balti- bidirectional associations. Journal of Early Adoles-
more; Brookes Publishing. cence, 25, 421-452.
Pianta, R. C , Mashburn, A. J., Downer, J. T., Hamre,
B. K., & Justice, L. (2008). Effects of web-mediated
professional development resources on teacher-child Date Received: May 31, 2012
interactions in pre-kindergarten classrooms. Early Date Accepted: November 11, 2013
Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 431-451. Associate Editor; Shannon Suido

104
Relatedness in the Classroom

Rebecca A. Madill, MS, is currently a doctoral candidate in the Departrnent of Human


Development and Family Studies at The Pennsylvania State University. Her research
focuses on identifying and measuring teaching strategies that influence the social and
cognitive development of children during early and middle childhood.

Scott D. Gest, PhD, is an associate professor of human development and family studies
at The Pennsylvania State University. His research focuses on developmental processes
linking children's school-based peer experiences with their academic competence and
problem behaviors from middle childhood through adolescence and explores how teach-
ing practices and intervention efforts may modify these peer experiences in ways that
support better individual adjustment.

Philip C. Rodkin, PhD, is an associate professor of educational psychology at the


University of Illinois. The goal of his work is to understand the socialization and
development of aggressive behavior, particularly the aggressive behavior of children who
are well accepted by their peers, and eventually to devise interventions that use children's
existing social networks to reduce problem behavior in schools.

105
Copyright of School Psychology Review is the property of National Association of School
Psychologists and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai