Anda di halaman 1dari 7

Accepted: 27 December 2016

DOI: 10.1111/jmp.12251

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Pattern of novel object exploration in cynomolgus monkey


Macaca fascicularis

Hong Zou1,2 | Ming Liu2 | Yi Luan2 | Qinglian Xie2 | Zhiheng Cheng2 | 


Guoping Zhao1,2,* | Meilei Jin2,* | Ning Guo3,4,* | Gang Jason Jin4,* | Lei Yu5,*,#

1
Department of Microbiology and Microbial
Engineering, School of Life Sciences, Fudan Abstract
University, Shanghai, China Background: Primates exhibit substantial capacity for behavioral innovation, expand-
2
Shanghai Institutes for Biological
ing the diversity of their behavioral repertoires, and benefiting both individual survival
Sciences, Chinese Academy of Sciences,
Shanghai, China and species development in evolution. Novel object exploration is an integral part of
3
Shanghai University of Traditional Chinese behavioral innovation. Thus, qualitative and quantitative analysis of novel object ex-
Medicine, Shanghai, China
ploration helps to better understand behavioral innovation.
4
ShanghaiBio Corporation, Shanghai, China
5
Methods: To study the pattern of novel object exploration, two different sized balls
Department of Genetics and Center
of Alcohol Studies, Rutgers University, were sequentially introduced to singly caged cynomolgus monkeys. Two aspects of
Piscataway, NJ, USA monkeys’ behaviors were analyzed: the types of motor activities in toy playing and
#
Sent inquiries to whether there is an orderly sequence of such motor activities during novelty
Lei Yu, Department of Genetics and Center exploration.
of Alcohol Studies, Rutgers University,
Piscataway, NJ, USA. Results: Four types of behavioral activities (oral contact, gross and fine forelimb motor,
Email: yu@biology.rutgers.edu and hind limb motor) followed a pattern: first forelimb gross motor and oral contact,
Funding information followed by forelimb fine motor and hind limb activities. Oral contact appeared to be
ShanghaiBio Corporation. an important behavior in monkeys’ repertoire of novelty exploratory behaviors, both
as an early appearing activity, and showing a consistent pattern of high cumulative
time for two different novel objects.
Conclusions: These results provide a profile of novel object exploratory behaviors in
cynomolgus monkeys, contributing to a better understanding of this aspect of behav-
ioral innovation.

KEYWORDS
cynomolgus monkey, non-human primate, novel object exploration

1 |  INTRODUCTION their respective social ranking.3 Novel object exploration is an inte-
gral part of behavioral innovation, likely to aid in food source inves-
Compared to non-­primates, primates exhibit substantial capacity for tigation, danger assessment, stress response, and adaptation to new
behavioral innovation,1,2 thus expanding the diversity of their be- environments. Successful patterns of novelty exploration may play an
havioral repertoires, to the benefit of individual survival as well as evolutionary role through social learning and transmission in primate
species development in biological evolution. Behavioral innovation populations.4
is the development of novel behavioral patterns, such as in forag- Non-­human primates have been used to study various behaviors,
ing and mate-­seeking, and reflects individual primates’ positions in including novelty exploration behavior. When exploring novel objects,
two issues are relevant: types of behavioral activities displayed and
*Co-corresponding authors. the sequence of such activities.

J Med Primatol. 2017;46:19–24. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jmp © 2017 John Wiley & Sons A/S. |  19
Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
|
20       ZOU et al.

When encountering a novel object, mammals may be attracted to the underlying biological functions,19 the understanding of which
it. This tendency toward novelty has been well demonstrated in differ- would benefit from a better description of these behaviors. Thirdly,
ent species of mammals, including rats,5 gerbils,6 marmosets,7 and ba- the outcome from both qualitative and quantitative analyses of spe-
boons.8,9 In terms of the types of behavioral activities displayed by the cific types of behaviors can serve as a frame of reference, with which
mammals, visual observation, forelimb contact/exploration, and oral the results from further studies can be compared and contrasted for
8-10
contact have all been reported. However, it is not clear whether investigating different types of behaviors. With this rationale in mind,
monkeys use other body parts, such as hind limbs or buttocks, as part we report here the findings from the study.
of their behavioral repertoire during novel object exploration.
In studies of motor skill learning, rodents do not display a strong
2 | MATERIALS AND METHODS
tendency for a patterned sequence of activities, as they often per-
form motor movements in a seemingly random fashion, without any
2.1 | Animals and study site
discernable order.11,12 Non-­human primates, on the other hand, are
known to acquire an orderly sequence of discrete motor movements, This study used 17 male cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis)
13,14
as shown in hand grasping activities. However, it is not well un- between 3 and 4 years old, with average body weight of 3.4±0.1 kg
derstood whether an orderly sequence of motor activities takes place (mean±SEM). All animals were supplied by Hainan Jingang Laboratory
in novelty exploration. Animal Co. Ltd., an AAALAC-­
accredited facility located in Hainan
Previous studies have used several methods to analyze reaction Province in southern China. The local climate in Hainan Province
to novel objects in non-­human primates. To compare the novelty-­ is similar to that of the natural habitat of cynomolgus monkeys in
seeking trait of stump-­tailed macaques (Macaca arctoides) and spider the neighboring southeastern Asia area. All experimental protocols
monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), these two species were kept in groups were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee
separately, and the risk-­taking index (facing a hazardous object) and (IACUC) at Hainan Jingang Laboratory Animal Co. Ltd., adhered to the
the curiosity index (facing a nonhazardous object) were assessed by guidelines of the Committee for Research and Ethical Issues of IASP,
categorizing behaviors in four levels depending on the distance be- and were consistent with the National Institutes of Health Guide for
tween the subjects and the objects and the frequency of the objects the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Two months before the
being manipulated.15,16 By changing environmental stressors, free-­ study, monkeys were transferred from group housing to individual
choice novelty-­seeking behavior was assessed in vervet by coding the cages (80 × 60 × 70 cm, LxWxH). There were two rows of cages fac-
novelty-­seeking score according to latency and times of the animal ap- ing each other in the laboratory, with solid dividers between neighbor-
proaching the novel objects.17 Baboons kept in new cages individually ing cages; thus, the monkeys could not see their immediate neighbors,
were exposed to a certain type of novel object, and the frequency and but had visual contact with the monkeys in the opposite row in the
duration of object contact were investigated, to assess their reaction room. Animals also could reach out of the cage and had limb contact
to changes in spatial and shape variation of novel objects.8 Baboons in with the neighboring monkeys. The room was well ventilated. Room
social groups were exposed to daily changes of novel objects in their lighting was supplied mainly by natural lighting, with some fluorescent
home cages, and their reactions were analyzed for both exploratory lighting to facilitate nighttime video recording of behavior. Monkeys
behaviors (looking at and sniffing objects) and manipulative behav- were served three meals a day with regular monkey chow, plus sea-
ior (touching, grasping, and transporting objects).9 Rhesus monkeys sonal fruits in the afternoon. Drinking water was provided ad libitum.
of different ages were kept in individual home cages, and the types Meal service, room cleaning, and animal care were performed by the
of behaviors directed toward simple toys were studied.18 In terms of technicians at Hainan Jingang Laboratory Animal Co. Ltd., with the
the method for analyzing novelty exploration in non-­human primates, supervision of certified veterinarians.
these studies focused on rating animal behavior either by the extent
of their approaching/manipulating novel objects,15-17 or by the fre-
2.2 | Behavioral monitoring
quency and duration of novel object contact,8 with some reference to
the specific types of exploratory behavior.9,18 The behavioral experiments were conducted in the laboratories at
In this study, we focused on the specific types of exploratory be- Hainan Jingang Laboratory Animal Co. Ltd, from 2009 to 2010. A be-
haviors, and on whether there is an orderly sequence of such motor havioral monitoring system was used, which consisted of digital cam-
activities during novelty exploration, using a paradigm of two different eras, digital video recorders, and computers running video monitoring
sized balls as novel objects and introducing them sequentially to sin- software. Digital cameras were mounted both in front of and on top
gly caged cynomolgus monkeys. Our rationale is based on the follow- of monkey cages, so that both frontal view and top view of monkey
ing considerations. Firstly, novelty exploration involves different, and behaviors were recorded.
sometimes complex, behaviors. By separating and categorizing spe-
cific types of behaviors, it helps pave the way for systematic and quan-
2.3 | Basketball and tennis ball playing
titative analyses. Secondly, various behaviors occur at different times
during an animal’s object exploration, and some behaviors may even Monkey’s basketball-­playing behavior was recorded when a toy bas-
overlap. The timing and the sequence of these behaviors may reflect ketball (175 mm in diameter, surface made of rubbery plastic material
ZOU et al. |
      21

1. Oral contact: when a monkey contacts the ball with any part
of its mouth, including lips and teeth.
2. Forelimb (fine motor): various fine motor activities using the mon-
key’s forelimbs, such as peeling off the label on the ball, or attempt-
ing to pull out the air valve of the ball.
3. Forelimb (gross motor): various gross motor activities using the
monkey’s forelimbs, such as holding up the ball, rolling the ball
against the cage wall, patting the ball, or pushing the ball away.
4. Hind limb: using hind limbs, such as touching/holding/pushing the
ball, or attempting to stand on the ball.
5. Overall ball playing: cumulative time when a monkey touches the
ball with any part of its body (except its tail). This behavior was
scored independently; thus, it was not an arithmetic sum of the
other behavior scores.

2.4 | Statistics
For the time of the first appearance of various behaviors, data are
expressed as mean±SEM. To compare the first-­appearance time of
various behaviors upon basketball introduction, one-­way ANOVA
with Tukey’s test was used for multiple comparisons. To compare the
first-­appearance time of various behaviors upon tennis ball introduc-
tion, because of unequal variance (significantly different), Kruskal-­
Wallis test was used. Dunn’s test was used for multiple comparison
test. To compare the mean duration times for oral contact, forelimb
gross motor contact, forelimb fine motor contact, and hind limb con-
F I G U R E   1   Sequence of behaviors upon novel object introduction.
tact, one-­way ANOVA with Tukey’s test was used for basketball and
Time of the first appearance for each behavior is plotted for the
Kruskal-­Wallis with Dunn’s test was used for tennis ball. T test was
basketball (A) or the tennis ball (B). Data are shown as mean±SEM
(n=17 for the basketball; n=16 for the tennis ball). *, significant used to compare cumulative play time (for each of the four play types)
difference between the two behaviors (P<.05) between basketball and tennis ball.

with yellow/purple stripes and black-­line marking, fully inflated with


3 | RESULTS
air) was placed in a monkey’s cage. Each toy basketball was used only
for one monkey.
3.1 | Behaviors during basketball and tennis ball
Seventeen hours after the basketball was introduced, a tennis ball
playing
(regular sports grade, 67-­mm in diameter, with green velvet surface)
was placed in a monkey’s cage as a new toy. Each tennis ball was used When a toy basketball was introduced into a monkey’s cage, various
only for one monkey. ball-­
playing behaviors were observed. It was observed that mon-
For both basketball and tennis ball playing, the following proce- keys tended to use their forelimb first for ball manipulation (gross
dure was used for behavioral analysis. Video recording was analyzed of- motor), followed by oral contact (Figure 1A). Later on, monkeys dis-
fline. To determine the first-­appearance time of various behaviors, the played forelimb fine motor and hind limb activities, with similar first-­
first 30 minutes of video footage upon novel object introduction was appearance time values (Figure 1A). Statistical analysis by one-­way
examined, and the time of the first appearance of each behavior was ANOVA showed significant differences among the time of the first
noted. If a particular behavior was not observed in an animal during appearance for these four behaviors (F3,67=4.357, P<.05). Multiple
the 30-­minute video, the cutoff time of 30 minutes was assigned as comparison analysis with Tukey’s test showed that the time of the
the value. To determine the cumulative time of object exploration, first appearance of both the forelimb fine motor and the hind limb
1 hour of video footage was divided into two consecutive 30-­minute activities was significantly later than that of forelimb gross motor ac-
blocks after the ball was introduced. For the first 30-­minute block, be- tivity (for forelimb fine motor, q=4.206, P<.05; for hind limb, q=4.162,
haviors were analyzed for the first 2 minutes for every 5-­minute time P<.05).
segment. For the second 30-­minute block, behaviors were analyzed When a tennis ball was introduced to the cage as another novel
for the first 2 minutes for every 10-­minute time segment. The follow- object, monkeys displayed the same four behaviors, with a similar
ing criteria and behavioral categories were used: sequence of appearance: first was forelimb gross motor, followed by
|
22       ZOU et al.

(23.9±11.0 seconds). The overall basketball-­playing activities were


measured separately, showing 408.6±48.7 seconds. For tennis ball
(Figure 2B), Kruskal-­Wallis test showed a significant difference for cu-
mulative time of these behaviors (P<.05). Monkeys spent significantly
more time (P<.05) for forelimb gross motor contact (342.8±39.1 sec-
onds) than forelimb fine motor contact (56.7±14.3 seconds) or hind
limb contact (106.2±26.4 seconds). They also spent significantly more
time (P<.05) for oral contact (157.9±20.7 seconds) than forelimb fine
motor contact. The overall tennis ball-­playing activities were meas-
ured separately, showing 479.7±59.5 seconds. Comparing cumula-
tive play time (for each of the four play types) between basketball
and tennis ball, monkeys spent significantly more time when playing
tennis ball on oral contact (P=.0134), forelimb gross motor contact
(P<.0001), and hind limb (P=.0093) than playing basketball. However,
there is no significant difference for overall ball-­playing time between
object types (P=.36).

4 | DISCUSSION

In the present study, we performed quantitative analysis of novel


object-­playing behaviors in cynomolgus monkey Macaca fascicularis,
as a way to characterize monkeys’ behavioral pattern in exploring
novel objects. Novelty exploration opportunities for monkeys were
created by first introducing a toy basketball, and later a tennis ball, to
singly caged monkeys. By analyzing the video recording, a number of
characteristics were noted:
When a monkey encountered novel objects, four types of be-
F I G U R E   2   Mean cumulative time of behaviors for basketball and havioral activities were observed: oral contact, forelimb motor
tennis ball playing. The cumulative time of object exploration for each
(both gross and fine), and hind limb motor. The process of novel
behavior was determined from video footage for each animal for the
object exploration appeared to follow a sequence: forelimb gross
basketball (A) or the tennis ball (B). Data are shown as the mean±SEM
(n=17 for the basketball; n=16 for the tennis ball). *, significant motor activities and oral contact appeared first, followed by fore-
difference between the two behaviors (P<.05) limb fine motor and hind limb activities. This sequence of behavioral
activities during novelty exploration was evident when monkeys
first encountered the basketball (Figure 1A). When the tennis ball
oral contact, and then forelimb fine motor and hind limb activities was subsequently introduced, monkeys followed a similar pattern
(Figure 1B). Kruskal-­Wallis test showed a significant difference among (Figure 1B). It is worth noting that the first appearance of all four
the time of the first appearance for these four behaviors (P<.05). behaviors occurred earlier for the tennis ball than for the basketball.
Multiple comparison analysis by Dunn’s test showed that the time of It is possible that the small size of a tennis ball compared with the
first appearance of both the forelimb fine motor and the hind limb ac- larger size of a toy basketball represented a less threatening situ-
tivities was significantly later than either that of forelimb gross motor ation for monkeys, resulting in monkeys’ prompt physical engage-
activity (P<.05) or that of oral contact (P<.05). ment with the tennis ball. Also, as the monkeys had already played
with the first novel object, the toy basketball, a sense of familiarity
with a novel object may have contributed to the shorter lapsed time
3.2 | Cumulative time for individual behaviors during
between novel object introduction and a monkey’s physical contact
basketball and tennis ball playing
with the novel object.
To compare individual behaviors during basketball and tennis ball play- Studies of non-­human primates have shown that discrete motor
ing, the cumulative time for each of the four behaviors was measured. movements are often sequenced in an orderly fashion; that is, cer-
For basketball (Figure 2A), statistical analysis by one-­way ANOVA tain motor actions take place first, followed by subsequent motor ac-
showed significant difference (F3,67=8.505, P<.05). Monkeys spent sig- tions, demonstrating the stability of a sequential pattern. Thus, in hand
nificantly more time (P<.05) for oral contact (89.9±16.0 seconds) and grasping activities, motor movements are shown to follow a specific
forelimb fine motor contact (88.8±16.3 seconds), compared to fore- sequence.13,14 This pattern of sequential order in non-­human primates
limb gross motor contact (18.9±9.1 seconds) and hind limb contact parallels that in humans—infants follow a sequence of coordinated
ZOU et al. |
      23

visual and motor activities when reaching for objects.20 Our work con- (Figure 1A). In other words, oral contact is not significantly different
firms this general activity pattern in cynomolgus monkeys. from forelimb and hind limb (Figure 1A). One possibility may be related
It should be noted that an orderly motor sequence does not to the size of the novel object. The tennis ball is relatively small, similar
appear to be present in rodents, as rodents often perform motor to certain fruits (such as apples and pears) that monkeys are used to
movements in a seemingly random fashion, without any discernable handle and eat. Therefore, when they explore a small object such as
11,12,21,22
order. In primates, correct order of motor sequence appears a tennis ball, monkeys may intuitively initiate oral contact, but quickly
to be the norm.14,23,24 Specifically, the orderly appearance of several abandon the effort once they realize the tennis ball is non-­edible. For
submovements becomes stable over time, and these primitives even- a large object such as a basketball, the beginning of oral contact explo-
tually are integrated into a whole, more complex, skilled movement ration was delayed. As a result, the time of first oral contact with the
for a purpose.25-27 Human infants have also been shown to display basketball is similar to that of the forelimb (fine motor) and hind limb
the purposeful order of motor activities and to integrate these sub- behaviors. Therefore, for basketball playing, there is no statistically
movements into a whole movement to achieve a simple purpose.20 significant difference between the time of first oral contact with those
These findings indicate that in both non-­human primates and human of the forelimb (fine motor) and hind limb movements. And forelimb
infants, the correct ordering of submovements and the integration into gross motor, which was also an early appearance behavior, showed a
a whole movement are to achieve a simple purpose, such as getting rather low level of cumulative time for basketball (Figure 2A). Thus, the
an object. Therefore, it can be considered that an orderly sequence different patterns of statistical significance between a large object (the
of submovements is a necessity to achieving an actionable purpose. basketball) and a small object (the tennis ball) may reside in the size of
What we have shown in this study is that, when encountering a the object, which reflect the monkey’s innate interest in exploring a
large playing object such as a basketball, monkeys employ discrete potential edible subject and the amount of exploration it may take to
motor behaviors. These behaviors are not only discernable from one explore that object.
another, but also they appear to follow an orderly sequence. In other It should be pointed out that, because the current study was not
words, distinct motor behaviors are used by non-­human primates in an repeated with a reverse order of novel object introduction (the ten-
orderly nature and in a purposeful manner. nis ball first, and the basketball second), the interpretations based on
Oral contact appears to be an important behavior in monkeys’ object size are tentative until such time that the reverse presentation
repertoire of novelty exploratory behaviors. It appeared early when can be completed.
a monkey encountered a ball. In fact, there was no significant differ- In conclusion, we showed that in novelty exploration, monkeys
ence between the first appearance for oral contact and forelimb gross displayed an orderly sequence of activities, beginning with forelimb
motor (Figure 1A,B), indicating that a monkey initiates physical explo- gross motor and oral contact, followed by forelimb fine motor and hind
ration of a novel object with forelimb gross motor and oral contact be- limb activities. Furthermore, oral contact appeared to be an import-
haviors. Additionally, oral contact showed a consistent pattern of high ant behavior in monkeys’ repertoire of novelty exploratory behaviors,
cumulative time, for both basketball and tennis ball (Figure 2), whereas both as an early appearing activity, and showing a consistent pattern
forelimb gross motor, which was also an early activity, showed a rather of high cumulative time for two different novel objects. These results
low level of cumulative time for basketball (Figure 2A). Taken together, provide a profile of object exploratory behaviors in cynomolgus mon-
these results suggest that oral contact may serve as a primary sensory keys, contributing to a better understanding of this aspect of behav-
input in monkeys’ novelty exploration. ioral innovation.
This is consistent with the pattern of novelty exploration in
human and other non-­human primates. When human infants see a
AC KNOW L ED G M ENTS
novel object, their first physical action toward it is to engage in oral
contact—attempting to touch with lips, bite, chew, even swallow.28-30 We thank Hainan Jingang Laboratory Animal Co. Ltd. for providing
In gray mouse lemurs, extensive mouth use was observed when the technical support. This work was partially supported by funding from
animals retrieve food items.31 As lips contain the densest sensory ShanghaiBio Corporation.
nerve terminals for body surface of human and other primates, oral
contact would be very informative in terms of providing sensory sig-
REFERENCES
nals for a novel object. With such initial characterization of an object,
subsequent contact with hands and feet would provide additional 1. Kummer H, Goodall J. Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates.
Philos Trans R Soc B. 1985;308:203–214.
information.
2. Lee P. Adaptations to environmental change: an evolutionary per-
It is worth noting that the pattern of behavior differences differs spective. In: Box HO, ed. Primate Responses to Environmental Change.
between that for basketball playing and that for tennis ball playing, London: Chapman & Hall; 1991:39–56.
in that in tennis ball playing, both the first-­appearance time of fore- 3. Reader SM, Laland KN. Primate innovation: sex, age and social rank
limb (gross motor) and oral contact show significant difference with differences. Int J Primatol. 2001;22:787–805.
4. Laland KN. A theoretical investigation of the role of social transmis-
forelimb (fine motor) and hind limb (Figure 1B), whereas in basket-
sion in evolution. Ethol Sociobiol. 1992;13:87–113.
ball playing, only the first-­appearance time of forelimb (gross motor) 5. Berlyne DE. Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-
shows significant difference with fine forelimb motor and hind limb Hill Book Company; 1960.
|
24       ZOU et al.

6. Cheal M. Stimulus-­elicited investigation in the Mongolian gerbil 21. Hermer-Vazquez L, Hermer-Vazquez R, Chapin JK. The reach-
(Meriones unguiculatus). J Biol Psychol. 1978;20:26–32. to-­grasp-­food task for rats: a rare case of modularity in animal behav-
7. Menzel EW, Menzel CR. Cognitive, developmental and social aspects ior? Behav Brain Res. 2007;177:322–328.
of responsiveness to novel objects in a family group of marmosets 22. Hermer-Vazquez R, Hermer-Vazquez L, Srinivasan S, Chapin JK.
(Saguinus fuscicollis). Behaviour. 1979;70:251–279. Beta-­ and gamma-­frequency coupling between olfactory and motor
8. Gouteux S, Vauclair J, Thinus-Blanc C. Reaction to spatial nov- brain regions prior to skilled, olfactory-­driven reaching. Exp Brain Res.
elty and exploratory strategies in baboons. Anim Learn Behav. 2007;180:217–235.
1999;27:323–332. 23. Castiello U. Grasping a fruit: selection for action. J Exp Psychol Hum
9. Joubert A, Vauclair J. Reaction to novel objects in a troop Percept Perform. 1996;22:582–603.
of Guinea baboons: approach and manipulation. Behaviour. 24. Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Gorny BP. Skilled reaching in rats and hu-
1986;96:92–104. mans: evidence for parallel development or homology. Behav Brain
10. Fragaszy DM, Mason WA. Response to novelty in Saimiri and Res. 1992;47:59–70.
Callicebus: influence of social context. Primates. 1978;19:311–331. 25. Barone P, Joseph JP. Role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in orga-
11. Gharbawie OA, Whishaw IQ. Parallel stages of learning and recovery of nizing visually guided behavior. Brain Behav Evol. 1989;33:132–135.
skilled reaching after motor cortex stroke: “oppositions” organize normal 26. Ninokura Y, Mushiake H, Tanji J. Integration of temporal order
and compensatory movements. Behav Brain Res. 2006;175:249–262. and object information in the monkey lateral prefrontal cortex. J
12. Hermer-Vazquez L, Moshtagh N. Rats’ learning of a new motor skill: Neurophysiol. 2004;91:555–560.
insight into the evolution of motor sequence learning. Behav Process. 27. Shima K, Mushiake H, Saito N, Tanji J. Role for cells in the presupple-
2009;81:50–59. mentary motor area in updating motor plans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
13. Graziano M. The organization of behavioral repertoire in motor cor- 1996;93:8694–8698.
tex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2006;29:105–134. 28. Juberg DR, Alfano K, Coughlin RJ, Thompson KM. An observational
14. Rizzolatti G, Luppino G. The cortical motor system. Neuron. study of object mouthing behavior by young children. Pediatrics.
2001;31:889–902. 2001;107:135–142.
15. Santillan-Doherty AM, Cortes-Sotres J, Arenas-Rosas RV, et  al. 29. Tulve NS, Suggs JC, McCurdy T, Cohen Hubal EA, Moya J. Frequency
Novelty-­seeking temperament in captive stumptail macaques of mouthing behavior in young children. J Expo Anal Environ Epidemiol.
(Macaca arctoides) and spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). J Comp 2002;12:259–264.
Psychol. 2010;124:211–218. 30. Xue J, Zartarian V, Tulve N, et al. A meta-­analysis of children’s object-­
16. Santillan-Doherty AM, Munoz-Delgado J, Arenas R, Marquez A, to-­mouth frequency data for estimating non-­dietary ingestion expo-
Cortes J. Reliability of a method to measure novelty-­seeking in non- sure. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol. 2010;20:536–545.
human primates. Am J Primatol. 2006;68:1098–1113. 31. Scheumann M, Joly-Radko M, Leliveld L, Zimmermann E. Does body
17. Fairbanks LA, Bailey JN, Breidenthal SE, Laudenslager ML, Kaplan JR, posture influence hand preference in an ancestral primate model?
Jorgensen MJ. Environmental stress alters genetic regulation of nov- BMC Evol Biol. 2011;11:52.
elty seeking in vervet monkeys. Genes Brain Behav. 2011;10:683–688.
18. Line SW, Morgan KN, Markowitz H. Simple toys do not alter the be-
havior of aged rhesus monkeys. Zoo Biol. 1991;10:473–484. How to cite this article: Zou H, Liu M, Luan Y, et al. Pattern of
19. Ott I, Schleidt M, Kien J. Temporal organisation of action in baboons:
novel object exploration in cynomolgus monkey Macaca
comparisons with the temporal segmentation in chimpanzee and
human behaviour. Brain Behav Evol. 1994;44:101–107. fascicularis. J Med Primatol. 2017;46:19–24. https://doi.
20. McCarty ME, Clifton RK, Ashmead DH, Lee P, Goubet N. How infants org/10.1111/jmp.12251
use vision for grasping objects. Child Dev. 2001;72:973–987.
Copyright of Journal of Medical Primatology is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai