Anda di halaman 1dari 7

Clinical Reasoning

Teaching Clinical Reasoning: Case-Based and


Coached
Jerome P. Kassirer, MD

Abstract
Optimal medical care is critically The theory proposes that memory of treatments, and cognitive errors in
dependent on clinicians’ skills to make clinical medicine and clinical reasoning clinical reasoning. The teaching of clinical
the right diagnosis and to recommend strategies is enhanced when errors in reasoning need not and should not be
the most appropriate therapy, and information, judgment, and reasoning delayed until students gain a full
acquiring such reasoning skills is a key are immediately pointed out and understanding of anatomy and
requirement at every level of medical discussed. Rather than using cases pathophysiology. Concepts such as
Downloaded from https://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3f4wPM3XSHuCfq8kTbUPI4BMS5TN0sM74VhzZmIEGeZjd86aS5JhrCw== on 03/23/2019

education. Teaching clinical reasoning is artificially constructed from memory, real hypothesis generation, pattern
grounded in several fundamental cases are greatly preferred because they recognition, context formulation,
principles of educational theory. Adult often reflect the false leads, the diagnostic test interpretation, differential
learning theory posits that learning is polymorphisms of actual clinical material, diagnosis, and diagnostic verification
best accomplished by repeated, and the misleading test results provide both the language and the
deliberate exposure to real cases, that encountered in everyday practice. methods of clinical problem solving.
case examples should be selected for Expertise is attainable even though the
their reflection of multiple aspects of These concepts foster the teaching and precise mechanisms of achieving it are
clinical reasoning, and that the learning of the diagnostic process, the not known.
participation of a coach augments complex trade-offs between the benefits
the value of an educational experience. and risks of diagnostic tests and Acad Med. 2010; 85:1118–1124.

A ll teaching methods are of necessity Clinical cognition encompasses the range


of strategies that clinicians use to
for teaching that have stood the test of
pragmatic and context-dependent. time. One fundamental principle, which
Teaching approaches lack a firm scientific generate, test, and verify diagnoses, to seems almost mundane today, is that
underpinning because of the paucity of assess the benefits and risks of tests and experiences are critical determinants
scientific evidence about optimal treatments, and to judge the prognostic that influence the quality of learning, and
learning. Despite substantial advances in significance of the outcomes of these that the teacher has an obligation to
our understanding of human cognition cognitive achievements. Needless to say, provide optimal experiences. Dewey
during the last few decades, our teaching clinical medicine consists of much more believed that teaching experiences should
methods are still based largely on expert than clinical cognition, including arouse curiosity, enhance personal
opinion. If these assertions are true for meticulous gathering of data, careful initiative, and allow free expression of
elementary teaching, they are even more examination of patients, empathy with learners’ ideas. In explaining the
compelling when applied to a field as the sick, ability to communicate with importance of individual experiences on
complex as clinical reasoning. Given patients, and professional demeanor, the development of expertise, he wrote,
these modest scientific underpinnings, we among many others, but this essay is “What [the student] has learned in the
might just throw up our hands and give restricted to clinical cognition. way of knowledge and skill in one
up any hope of imparting reasoning skills situation becomes an instrument of
to students and residents, yet we know Though we still have much to learn about understanding and dealing effectively
there is much to learn, that many do clinical cognition, several sources can be with the situations which follow.”1
become expert clinical problem solvers, combined to define a reasonable
and that the welfare of patients depends pragmatic approach that can be subjected Modern concepts of “adult learning”
as much on reasoning and problem- to critical evaluation. These sources start supplement these concepts. They hold
solving abilities as it does on the use of with commonsense notions of learning that the role of the teacher is not to
the latest technology. from some of the most venerated and transmit knowledge but to facilitate
respected educators, from modern learning, encourage spontaneity, and
theories of adult learning, from research engage in mutual inquiry.2 Such a
on clinical cognition, and from the strategy requires that the educator be
experience of educators, such as myself, comfortable when others in a group
Dr. Kassirer is distinguished professor, Tufts
University School of Medicine, Boston,
who have been working at it for decades. engage in critical thinking and challenge
Massachusetts, and visiting professor, Stanford the educator’s opinions and convictions.
University, Palo Alto, California.
As in Dewey’s formulation, adult learning
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Kassirer, Insights From Educational Theory theory holds that people learn new
Tufts University School of Medicine, 136 Harrison
Ave., Boston, MA 02111; telephone: (781) 237-1971 Seventy years ago, John Dewey, the great knowledge and skills most effectively
or (617) 306-9788 (cell); e-mail: jpkassirer@aol.com. educator and pragmatist, outlined criteria when they are presented in the context of

1118 Academic Medicine, Vol. 85, No. 7 / July 2010


Clinical Reasoning

the application of new knowledge to real- sources of information, responded repeated exposure to well-selected cases is
life situations.3,4 It proposes that because spontaneously by offering diagnostic or the ideal way to absorb such knowledge
learning cannot be separated from the therapeutic opinions.12 The subjects were and store it in memory.27–32 Over the past
context in which it is used, the best time not asked to explain how or why they decade, efforts to explain how disease
to learn anything is when the material is reached conclusions because such entities (or syndromes) are represented in
immediately useful.2,5 Emotional issues opinions are considered unreliable. memory have been a focus of much
count also: Adults learn best, the theory Instead, the subject’s utterances were analysis. A priori, it seems difficult to
posits, in informal, comfortable, flexible, recorded, transcribed, and then analyzed imagine that there could be any single
and nonthreatening settings. Lastly, for their reasoning content by another such representation, given the polyglot
because experience is the learner’s individual who was familiar with the way that individuals retain such
“textbook,” the core method of adult medical domain.12 In another study, information, the polymorphism of most
education should be the analysis of physicians “working up” simulated diseases, and the complex way diseases
experience.2,5,6 patients were videotaped and debriefed evolve in different patients over time. The
later about what they were considering descriptions are quite varied. Some claim
Gaining expertise is not easy, and it during the encounter, and transcripts of that disease entities are stored in mental
cannot be achieved passively.3,7,8 Some their responses were analyzed.13 The representations of disease attributes
who have studied expertise expressed the assumption of these methods is that called “frames” (in the language of
process this way: “The development of solving problems while speaking is artificial intelligence), in “semantic
genuine expertise requires struggle, probably not greatly dissimilar to doing networks” or as “semantic qualifiers,” in
sacrifice, and honest, often painful self- so without speaking.13–16 “illness scripts,” or in the form of
assessment. There are no shortcuts … scenarios of actual patients previously
and you will need to invest that time Though analysis of transcripts of encountered.29,33–38 Others have
wisely, by engaging in ‘deliberate physicians thinking aloud probes only suggested representations analogous to
practice’—practice that focuses on tasks some aspects of reasoning,9,17 early the “if–then” production rules
beyond your current level of competence studies produced useful insights, of computer programs, or in neuron
and comfort.”8,9 Presumably, expertise including a vocabulary for discussing combinations according to
develops as learners mindfully assemble clinical reasoning concepts, a notion of connectionism theories of brain
simple concepts into more complex the sequence of iterative steps in the function.39,40 All such characterizations
ones.4 Experts just know more, process, and an approach to both probably should be considered not as
remember more, and perceive more than learning and teaching clinical problem definitive descriptions of mental
do novices, but becoming an expert solving. These studies and others suggest processes but, rather, as tentative theories
requires persistence, focus, struggle, and that diagnostic hypotheses are quickly of how the mind works, or as metaphors
rigorous self-assessment.3,10 generated with minimal clinical data and for thought processes. For this reason,
that these hypotheses are then used as a despite recommendations in favor of so
problem representation, a framework for doing,29,31 it remains to be determined
Research on Clinical Reasoning further focused information whether there is value in incorporating
Earlier work on clinical reasoning gathering.12,13,18 –20 Only small numbers these notions into active teaching
centered not on the mental mechanisms of hypotheses appear to be active at any sessions.
and procedures that expert clinicians one time, consistent with the observation
claim that they use in solving problems, that short-term memory has a limited Although considerable uncertainty exists
but on what they are observed to do. capacity.3,21,22 Differential diagnosis is about the structure of knowledge in
Cognitive scientists have long given up envisioned not as a single static list of memory, a substantial body of evidence
on personal theories of mental processes disorders collected when all of a patient’s bears on how people process and apply
because they are known to be facts are revealed, but as an evolving, their knowledge. Reasoning, including
unreliable.11 Nonetheless, observations iterative process involving repeated clinical reasoning, is visualized as a dual-
on what clinicians do and how they hypothesis generation, deletion, and process system, with intuitive (i.e., tacit)
behave can inform both the teaching and refinement.23–25 Modification and and analytical components.17,41–43 (Note
learning of reasoning processes. Much of evolution of hypotheses involves both that though these two components are
the early work in the field was based on probabilistic and causal reasoning modes. described here as discrete entities, in
detailed analysis of thinking-aloud Working diagnoses, that is, hypotheses reality their interactions are almost
transcripts of clinicians solving real used for prognostic or therapeutic certainly far more integrated and
clinical diagnostic and therapeutic recommendations, are evoked only after interdependent.) The intuitive
problems and some on recall of they are assessed for their adequacy in components, thought to be a holdover
physicians viewing videotapes of their explaining all positive, negative, and from our evolutionary origins in our
interactions with simulated patients.12,13 normal clinical findings, and for their primitive past, are instinctual and
In one study, for example, authentic pathophysiologic reliability—namely, a reflexive, require no input from the
clinical material from a patient was made check on the reasonableness of causal analytic system, and respond to domain-
available serially to an experimental linkages between clinical events.12,26 relevant stimuli. They are characterized
subject (a physician) in the same by first impressions, quick pattern
sequence as it became evident to the There is little doubt that clinical recognition, and rapid responses to
doctors caring for the patient, and the knowledge is a fundamental requirement information.28,33,41–45 They seem to be
experimental subject, unaided by external of successful clinical reasoning and that effortless and autonomous, require little

Academic Medicine, Vol. 85, No. 7 / July 2010 1119


Clinical Reasoning

or no awareness or active thought, can be the ordinary.47,48 Finally, strong first Cognitive Errors
influenced by affect and emotions,46 and impressions are often correct, of course, But cold logic as exemplified by the
are activated in conditions of and an override should not be invoked analytic approach, including probabilistic
considerable uncertainty. Some aspects of without a convincing rationale. and causal reasoning, fails to account for
diagnosis, such as hypothesis generation, the fact that humans are human, not
are presumably an intuitive function. Thanks to formal work on quantitative silicon processors. As noted before,
Though intuitive, this heuristic part of clinical approaches, namely, Bayesian humans often jump to conclusions, using
the process is also primed to recognize analysis and decision analysis, there is less intuitive heuristics and reflexive rules of
new situations or patterns in its rapid- mystery in how clinical data can be thumb.59,60 Such conclusions often turn
recall fashion after repeated exposure to combined in diagnostic and therapeutic out to be correct, but when they miss the
the same stimuli or set of events. Stated problem solving than in how information mark in medicine such a miss can be
differently, by repeated practice, what is stored and retrieved in memory. costly in terms of a patient’s welfare.61,62
was once an analytic process can become Though few argue that people reason For decades, cognitive psychologists have
automatized and then can respond according to these formalisms, modeling known, based on laboratory experiments,
autonomously.17,42 Thus, even some clinical decision making by these that people misjudge likelihoods of
decision rules become autonomous42; approaches helps put a rigorous, logical events based on their recall of salient
evidence suggests that “easy cases” are framework on these processes. examples, their vividness, or their
more likely solved by pattern recognition Understanding Bayes’ rule makes resemblance to other examples.26,63,64 In
and more difficult cases by analytic concepts such as sensitivity and addition, they may misjudge the
strategies.27–29 Intuitive components specificity of diagnostic tests likelihood of an outcome based on some
often produce valuable, accurate comprehensible. Bayes’ rule is also a starting point or initial value.63–66
responses, but because of their inherent framework for understanding the Physicians occasionally misjudge the a
characteristics (namely, their quickness evolution of a differential diagnosis based priori likelihood of diseases, suspect rare
and apparent lack of computation), they on any new clinical information whether diseases more often than is appropriate,
can be influenced by the context of the or not the data are derived from overemphasize the significance of a
moment, including emotions,17,46 and are diagnostic tests.26 It embodies the positive test, jump to conclusions with
sometimes prone to error. Such cognitive little information, and judge prematurely
concept of diagnostic “gold standards.”
errors are considered later. that they have a working (or final)
Though few physicians stop to do the
math required of the method, Bayes’ rule diagnosis.67,68 The existence of a cohesive
By contrast, the analytic components are structure of the diagnostic process, as
is the basis of many compiled testing
deliberate, studied problem-solving outlined above, and this laboratory
strategies.49,50 Likewise, formal decision
processes that consciously and mindfully confirmation of cognitive errors, made it
trees are not often constructed and their
consider alternatives and options. They possible 20 years ago to identify and
probabilities and utilities of outcomes not
are thought to require considerable classify cognitive diagnostic errors.61
often specified, yet the principles of this
cognitive work, are slower than the Such errors, many of which lead to life-
intuitive component, and are solidly strategy cement multiple therapeutic
concepts. They include the trade-offs threatening outcomes, have been
based on science, logic, inference, identified in every stage of the diagnostic
causality, probabilistic associations, and between the benefits and risks of tests and
treatments, thresholds for testing and process.27,61,62,69 Despite the early
decision making.42 These components are recognition of cognitive errors, attention
activated when a pattern is not clear, for treating, and decisional toss-ups or close
calls.51–54 to them has been only a recent
example, when a patient’s clinical or endeavor.62,69 –73
laboratory findings do not fit an easily
recognized clinical picture. Parts of the Reasoning based on causality is another
diagnostic process subsumed by these approach to diagnosis that is based not Caveats
components include hypothesis testing, on probabilistic considerations but on All of the foregoing research,
differential diagnosis, diagnostic pathophysiologic concepts.55–58 Causal information, and practical experiences
verification, and maintaining a coherent reasoning involves forming inferences have informed our thinking about how
clinical story that explains all the based on major cause-and-effect relations best to teach clinical reasoning, but
findings. The analytic system creates and between clinical variables or events. before considering the method described
manipulates models of reality in working Because such reasoning often relies on here, first a few caveats. This teaching
memory and maintains a coherent story, the pathophysiologic aspects of proposal encompasses only clinical
thus facilitating diagnostic reasoning and individual disease states, its application is cognition, the apparent mental processes
hypothesis testing.42 The analytic far narrower diagnostically than the other that constitute the diagnostic process.
components are less likely than the strategies. Nonetheless, causal reasoning The method also allows for discussion of
intuitive component to be error-prone, is a powerful analytic tool to explain cognitive aspects of therapeutics,
and they have the special trait of being discrepancies in certain diagnoses. Such including the trade-offs between the risks
capable of being a check and an override reasoning may also be useful in and benefits of treatments, treatment
of the first impressions of rapid unraveling disease polymorphisms, thresholds, and therapeutic toss-ups or
recognition.42 Nonetheless, an individual namely, instances in which a patient’s close calls. Not considered here are
initiates this checking function only when clinical manifestations fail to match critical and often inseparable aspects of
some characteristic of the first impression precisely with the textbook description of patient encounters, namely, personal
strikes the problem solver as being out of a disease state. communication, the importance of

1120 Academic Medicine, Vol. 85, No. 7 / July 2010


Clinical Reasoning

extracting useful and accurate to question, to learn from others, and to


information, the need to meticulously compare notes.77 List 1
review old records, appropriate review of Diagnostic and Therapeutic Concepts
systems, assessment of medical evidence, Selection of examples of Clinical Reasoning
or performance of the physical
Though it is quite clear that repeated, Diagnosis
examination.74 The reader should not
deliberate experience with real clinical • Hypothesis generation
infer that these issues are unimportant
material is an essential component of the • Context formulation
but merely separate from the cognitive
learning process, a random selection of
issues under discussion. One could argue • Hypothesis refinement
cases is not sufficient to teach all the
that teaching clinical reasoning in the age complex elements of reasoning, clinical • Test interpretation
of computer-aided diagnostic aids, or otherwise.7,29 To aim for a broad • Bayesian reasoning
electronic medical records, and massive understanding of reasoning principles, a • Probabilistic, physiologic, and causal
clinical electronic databases is thoughtful selection of examples is reasoning
superfluous. In my judgment, it is more critical. Trainees who bring the cases to • Differential diagnosis
needed than ever: None of these digital the teaching session should be • Assessing for adequacy, coherence, and
modalities can yet substitute for an expert encouraged to select cases that, over time, parsimony
clinician. Lastly, there is plenty of room illustrate all kinds of aspects of the • Working (final) diagnosis
for disagreement over the principles and diagnostic process, as well as those that • Cognitive errors
practice of teaching any subject, and instantiate the judgmental aspects of the Treatment
clinical reasoning is no exception. But trade-off between the risks and benefits • Treatment under conditions of uncertainty
dissension is no reason to avoid of testing and treating (List 1). Such a • Tradeoffs between the risks and benefits of
proposing a method that many besides selection of cases is available.26 Both tests and treatments
myself have found useful both in their recently admitted patients and past cases • Choices based on the relation between the
roles as learners and educators.75,76 have value; the latter have special likelihood of disease and therapeutic risk
usefulness with respect to understanding • Treatment thresholds
how the disease evolved and for • Test-treatment thresholds
Teaching Clinical Reasoning 2010 connecting prior decision making and • Decisional close calls and “toss-ups”
The approach I describe here is applicable patient outcomes. • Therapeutic trial as a diagnostic test
predominantly to case-based teaching • Watchful waiting versus immediate action
conferences, especially with groups of 30 Examples should be selected according to
or fewer students or residents, though it the level of the learners.29 They should
not be synthesized according to clinical reasoning.77 Thus, when possible,
has been widely used with larger clinical material should be organized in
audiences. As a starter, even a someone’s memory of former cases but
instead should be genuine, active cases, to the same chronologic sequence as the
rudimentary exposure to the components events unfolded in real life. Although the
of the clinical reasoning process (List 1) is ensure that the actual uncertainties,
inconsistencies, imperfections, presentation of material will often start
helpful as a framework or roadmap to with a patient’s age, sex, and chief
guide students as they begin to complexity, and ambiguity of clinical
data are encompassed.4,26 To explain how complaint, it also can begin with the
understand the elements of reasoning in problem for which the patient was
particular cases.4,13,26 At the very least, cognitive errors arise and how “near
misses” occur, some examples of referred to a physician or hospital. If a
such an introduction provides students a patient’s chief complaint is nausea and
language for thinking about clinical defective clinical reasoning should be
included among cases that illustrate weakness, for example, and if the learners
problem solving. Given the strong are inexperienced, and if the goal is to
predominance of medical knowledge as a excellent reasoning. The case examples
should be unfamiliar to the learners so elaborate on the causes of these
criterion for learning clinical reasoning, complaints, then it is appropriate to start
some have argued not to introduce they will be forced to confront the clinical
material de novo and thus will not be with these complaints. But for more-
reasoning strategies until after the second advanced learners, if the presenting
year of medical school, at a time when hampered by hindsight (retrospective)
bias.78,79 Needless to say, the more cases symptoms are the same but the principal
students are well grounded in issue is declining kidney function, the
pathophysiology. In fact, beginning experienced in this way, the better.9,17
chief complaints can be bypassed in favor
medical students’ knowledge of medicine of a starting point such as “the patient is
cannot be considered merely tabula rasa, Organization of material
being seen for unexplained progressive
owing to their exposure to medicine in Narratives of cases are time-worn rituals renal insufficiency.”
the media and in their personal lives. that are created to capture clinical
Thus, I believe that exposure to clinical experience. Such narratives should My point is that the individual who
reasoning using carefully selected case contain not just the facts of the patient’s selects the case should be cognizant of the
examples can begin during the first year illness but the judgments that were made teaching goal and should tailor the case
of medical school. Of course, no matter and the actions that were taken as the presentation to achieve the goal. In a
where learners are in their training, some patient’s condition evolved.77 The case classroom setting, learners can be asked
fundamental habits are required. Clinical functions not only as an organized to extract information in an iterative
cognition requires a flexible cast of mind, template for clinical reasoning but as the fashion from the presenter, or the
a power of observation, and a willingness basis for learning clinical medicine and presenter can provide clinical data in

Academic Medicine, Vol. 85, No. 7 / July 2010 1121


Clinical Reasoning

“chunks.” Though the chunks often encouraged at the appropriate time to When summarizing a just-discussed case,
would follow the traditional sequence of seek critical evidence from other however, the information is fresh and the
history, physical examination, and information sources, including available time is ideal for a retrospective analysis
laboratory findings, they need not always electronic databases. But the coach and immediate feedback, including a
be structured this way. Effective problem should be discouraged from retreating discussion of all kinds of errors if there
solving can be illustrated by beginning into his or her special interest when were any.7,29,85 (This approach has been
with a physical examination finding, an befuddled and convert the session into a used effectively for some time by the U.S.
X-ray, or a set of laboratory studies. lecture on his or her research, specialty, Army, which carries out an after-action
or special interest. review of events in training or in
The role of a coach combat.86) If cognitive errors were made,
A coach functions best when he or she is There is no better time to explain the this case “wrap-up” presents an
as unfamiliar about the case as the application of probability theory, opportunity to dissect them and expose
learners and is forced to examine the threshold concepts, the nature of a them. If the learners have been actively
same information prospectively as differential diagnosis, notions of engaged in the problem-solving session,
the learners. The coach fills an essential causality, and disease polymorphisms they will be personally invested in
task, namely, monitoring the learners’ than with real cases in an active teaching understanding how errors occurred.85 In
questions and responses, commenting on session when these issues surface as part wrapping up a case, a coach can also ask
their relevance and accuracy.17 Specific of the discussion, namely, at the time of whether the diagnosis satisfies criteria of
issues about the medical aspects of the greatest interest. Needless to say, the adequacy (Were all findings explained?)
patient as well as the reasoning involved moderator must be well versed in these and coherence (Did physiologic linkages
in diagnosis and treatment can be dealt concepts to be able to impart them make sense?), whether it is a
with simultaneously. The coach asks the adequately. parsimonious explanation of the findings,
participants why they requested what the major clues were that led to the
information and then has them explain correct diagnosis, whether and how a
Avoiding cognitive errors
what they learned when they receive the diagnosis could have been arrived at
answer. The point of this interactive give- Recent essays about clinical reasoning earlier or more efficiently, and whether
and-take during the problem-solving argue that metacognition might be an the therapeutic approaches selected were
session is to provide instant feedback17 by effective strategy for avoiding cognitive rational or not. One might hope that
examining the intermediate reasoning as errors.62,70,72,81–83 Metacognition is a these approaches will reduce future
data are collected and as a diagnosis or method of introspection in which one is errors, but their influence on students’
therapeutic plan is being formulated, supposed to contemplate or reflect on future cognition is only a matter of
rather than holding all discussion until all one’s own thinking. Because many speculation at present. Note, finally, that
the information from the case is cognitive errors are the consequence of this discussion is devoted only to
available.26,33,80 By that time, much of the inappropriate triggering of the intuitive cognitive diagnostic errors, not to those
intermediate reasoning is lost. component of cognition, they are, as involving system-level dysfunctions.
discussed earlier, susceptible to Nothing, however, precludes discussion
The coach must try to engage all correction by analytic reasoning.17,42,47,48 of such systemic errors when they are
participants to be actively involved in the Generally, however, some signal must be discovered as part of the analysis of
problem-solving session even, if perceived that could activate this individual cases.
necessary, by calling on some to checking process. There is little doubt
participate. Adult learning theory stresses that individuals can be forced to rethink
that the teacher must try to make the their instinctive responses, and when they This Approach in Relation to
teaching session intellectually do so, they seem to make fewer errors.84 Other Teaching Modalities
challenging, enjoyable, respectful, and Nonetheless, how much reassessing and Needless to say, this case-based
nonthreatening.2,6 This does not mean revisiting intuitive responses occurs in approach that simulates a real clinical
that the session should always be anxiety- the real world is not known. In theory, encounter is only one of many methods
free, either for the learners or the coach; there would be great value if individuals to teach clinical reasoning, and given its
sometimes such stress actually renders could use critical-thinking skills such as relative inefficiency from a financial
the memory of a case keener. If the coach emotional detachment, neutral standpoint and its requirement for
is as much “in the dark” about the case as examination of beliefs, perspective faculty who are willing to expose their
the learners, he or she might also be switching, and assessment of the current reasoning strategies, why should it be
embarrassed about mistaken facts, wrong context, but how to do so is difficult.42 In preferred? How does it compare to large-
judgments, inappropriate hypotheses, medicine, approaches to teach group lectures, to online interactive
and other errors. The coach must metacognition and thus correct or case exercises, and published
encourage spontaneity: What matters is prevent cognitive errors are not fully clinicopathologic conferences and similar
what is learned, not whether every case tested and so far have produced approaches? It is my view that purposeful
has a final answer. The coach should not inconsistent results.82 Checking through case selection, active student
feel compelled to cover all aspects of the long lists of cognitive errors might be participation, immediate feedback, and
case. Because the coach cannot be another strategy, and though several such thoughtful involvement by a seasoned
expected to be a compendium of medical lists of errors have been created,62,69,70,83 coach promote enhanced learning. I
information on all cases presented at such there is little evidence that their use suggest the method described here not as
a session, participants should be reduces the chance of subsequent errors. the only approach to teaching clinical

1122 Academic Medicine, Vol. 85, No. 7 / July 2010


Clinical Reasoning

reasoning but as one to interdigitate with Higgs J, Jones M, eds. Clinical Reasoning in Kluwer, Lippincott Williams and Wilkins;
any others that share the same goals. the Health Professions. Oxford, UK: 2009.
Butterworth; 1995. 27 Elstein A, Schwarz A. Evidence base of clinical
Whether the human resources required
7 Ericsson KA. Deliberate practice and the diagnosis. Clinical problem solving and
are worth the investment is to some acquisition and maintenance of expert diagnostic decision making: Selective review
extent a local issue, but it is a hypothesis performance in medicine and related of the cognitive literature. BMJ. 2002;324:
worth assessing. domains. Acad Med. 2004;79(10 suppl): 729 –732.
S70 –S80. 28 Eva KW. What every teacher needs to know
Although the practice of medicine is in 8 Ericsson KA, Prietula MJ, Cokely ET. The about clinical reasoning. Med Educ. 2004;39:
itself not a science, it is based on science making of an expert. Harv Bus Rev. July– 98 –106.
August 2007;85:114 –121, 193. Available at: 29 Norman G. Research in clinical reasoning:
and is always striving to become more http://harvardbusiness.org/product/making- Past history and current trends. Med Educ.
scientific.39,77 There are no double-blind of-an-expert/an/R0707J-PDF-ENG. Accessed 2005;39:418 –427.
controlled studies of clinical reasoning, January 15, 2010. 30 Higgs J, Jones M. Clinical reasoning. In:
nor of any of the programs designed to 9 Ericsson KA, Charness N. Expert Higgs J, Jones M, eds. Clinical Reasoning in
teach clinical reasoning. Teaching performance; its structure and acquisition. the Health Professions. Oxford, UK:
Am Psychol. 1994;49:725–747. Butterworth; 1995.
programs are based principally on 10 Remy M, Rikers JP, Paas F. Recent advances 31 Norman GR. The epistemology of clinical
pragmatic considerations, educational in expertise research. Appl Cogn Psychol. reasoning: Perspectives from philosophy,
theory, experience, and (frankly) trial and 2005;19:145–149. psychology, and neuroscience. Acad Med.
error. There is no justification to 11 Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. Telling more than we 2000;75(10 suppl):S127–S133.
apologize for these attributes, because can know: Verbal reports on mental 32 Weber EU, Bockenholt U, Hilton DJ, Wallace
processes. Psychol Rev. 1977;84:231–259. B. Determinants of diagnostic hypothesis
despite these shortcomings, our medical 12 Kassirer JP, Gorry GA. Clinical problem generation: Effects of information, base rates,
schools and training programs do yield solving: A behavioral analysis. Ann Intern and experience. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem
practitioners who excel in clinical Med. 1978;89:245–255. Cogn. 1993;19:1151–1164.
problem solving and who effectively 13 Elstein AS, Shulman LS, Sprafka SA. Medical 33 Bowen JL. Educational strategies to promote
navigate the complexities of diagnosis Problem Solving: An Analysis of Clinical clinical diagnostic reasoning. N Engl J Med.
Reasoning. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard 2006;355:2217–2225.
and treatment. We may not know University Press; 1978. 34 Bordage G. Elaborated knowledge: A key to
precisely how they become expert 14 Anderson JR. Methodologies for studying successful diagnostic thinking. Acad Med.
problem solvers, but over time they do. human knowledge. Behav Brain Sci. 1987;10: 1994;69:883–885.
Our job as educators is to continue to 467–505. 35 Bordage G. Prototypes and semantic
evolve our teaching methods in the hope 15 Kuipers BJ, Kassirer JP. Knowledge qualifiers: From past to present. Med Educ.
acquisition by analysis of verbatim protocols. 2007;41:1117–1121.
that our students become more efficient
In: Kidd AL, ed. Knowledge Acquisition for 36 Custers EJ, Regehr G, Norman GR. Mental
and more accurate problem solvers and Expert Systems: A Practical Handbook. New representations of medical diagnostic
make fewer cognitive errors. York, NY: Plenum Press; 1987. knowledge: A review. Acad Med. 1996;
16 Ericsson KA, Simon HA. Protocol Analysis: 71(10 suppl):S55–S61.
Verbal Reports as Data. Rev ed. Cambridge, 37 Norman G. Building on experience—The
Funding/Support: Supported in part by a grant Mass: MIT Press; 1993. development of clinical reasoning. N Engl
from the Josiah Macy Jr. Foundation. 17 Hogarth RM. Educating Intuition. Chicago, J Med. 2006;355:2251–2252.
Ill: University of Chicago Press; 2001. 38 Schmidt HG, Norman GR, Boshuizen HPA.
Other disclosures: Dr. Kassirer is a coauthor of a 18 Nendaz MR, Bordage G. Promoting A cognitive perspective on medical expertise:
recently published book, Learning Clinical diagnostic problem representation. Med Theory and implications. Acad Med. 1990;65:
Reasoning, which is expected to generate modest Educ. 2002;36:760 –766. 611–621.
royalties. He has no other financial conflicts of 19 Chang RW, Bordage G, Connell KJ. 39 Erneling CE, Johnson DM, eds. The Mind as
interest. Cognition, confidence, and clinical skills. The a Scientific Object. Between Brain and
importance of early problem representation Culture. New York, NY: Oxford University
Ethical approval: Not applicable. in case presentations. Acad Med. 1998; Press; 2005.
73(10 suppl):S109 –S111. 40 Bechtel W, Abrahamson A. Connectionism
20 Elstein AS. Clinical Reasoning in Medicine. and the Mind. Parallel Processing, Dynamics,
References In: In: Higgs J, Jones M, eds. Clinical and Evolution in Networks. 2nd ed. Malden,
1 Dewey J. Experience and Education. New Reasoning in the Health Professions. Oxford, Mass: Blackwell; 2002.
York, NY: Simon and Shuster; 1938. UK: Butterworth; 1995. 41 Dawson NV. Physician judgment in clinical
2 Knowles MS, Holton EF, Swanson RA. The 21 Miller GA. The magical number seven, plus settings: Methodological influences and
Adult Learner. 6th ed. Amsterdam, or minus two: Some limits on our capacity cognitive performance. Clin Chem. 1983;39:
Netherlands: Elsevier; 2005. for processing information. Psychol Rev. 1468 –1480.
3 van Gog T, Ericsson KA, Remy M, Rikers JP, 1994;101:343–352. 42 Stanovich KE. The Robot’s Rebellion. Finding
Paas F. Instructional design for advanced 22 Waldrop MM. The workings of working Meaning in the Age of Darwin. Chicago, Ill:
learners: Establishing connections between memory. Science. 1987;237:1564 –1567. University of Chicago Press; 2004.
the theoretical frameworks of cognitive load 23 Kassirer JP, Kopelman RI. What is a 43 Croskerry P. A universal model of diagnostic
and deliberate practice. Educ Technol Res differential diagnosis? Hosp Pract. August 15, reasoning. Acad Med. 2009;84:1022–1028.
Dev. 2005;53:73–81. 1990;24:19 –28. 44 Coderre S, Mandin H, Harasym PH, Fick
4 van Merrienboer JJG, Sweller J. Cognitive 24 Kassirer JP. Teaching clinical medicine by GH. Diagnostic reasoning strategies and
load theory and complex learning: Recent iterative hypothesis testing. Let’s preach what diagnostic success. Med Educ. 2003;37:695–
developments and future directions. Educ we practice. N Engl J Med. 1983;309:921–923. 703.
Psychol Rev. 2005;17:147–177. 25 Leblanc VR, Brooks LR, Norman GR. 45 Brooks LR, Allen SW, Norman GR. Role of
5 Higgs J, Jones M. Introduction. In: Higgs J, Believing is seeing: The influence of a specific similarity in a medical diagnostic
Jones M, eds. Clinical Reasoning in the diagnostic hypothesis on the interpretation task. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1991;120:278 –287.
Health Professions. Oxford, UK: Butterworth; of clinical features. Acad Med. 2002; 46 Vohs KD, Baumeister RF, Loewenstein G,
1995. 77(10 suppl):S67–S69. eds. Do Emotions Help or Hurt Decision
6 Refshauge K, Higgs J. Teaching clinical 26 Kassirer JP, Wong JB, Kopelman RI. Learning Making? A Hedgefoxian Perspective. New
reasoning in health science curricula. In: Clinical Reasoning. Philadelphia, Pa: Wolters York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation; 2007.

Academic Medicine, Vol. 85, No. 7 / July 2010 1123


Clinical Reasoning

47 Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers R, Penaforte 60 Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A. Judgment 74 Verghese A, Horwitz RI. In praise of the
JC, Cuelho-Filho JM. Breaking down Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. physical examination. BMJ. 2009;339:B5448.
automaticity: Case ambiguity and the shift to New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 75 Kassirer JP. Clinical-problem-solving—A
reflective approaches in clinical reasoning. 1982. new feature in the Journal. N Engl J Med.
Med Educ. 2007;41:1185–1192. 61 Kassirer JP, Kopelman RI. Cognitive 1992;326:60 –61.
48 Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers R. Diagnostic diagnostic errors: Instantiation, classification, 76 Kassirer JP. Images in clinical medicine.
errors and reflective practice in medicine. and consequences. Am J Med. 1989;86:433– N Engl J Med. 1992;326:829 –830.
J Eval Clin Pract. 2007;13:138 –145. 441. 77 Montgomery K. How Doctors Think: Clinical
49 Wells PS, Ginsberg JS, Anderson DR, et al. 62 Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R. Judgment and the Practice of Medicine. New
Use of a clinical model for safe management Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006.
of patients with suspected pulmonary Intern Med. 2005;165:1493–1499.
78 Fischhoff B. Hindsight not equal to foresight:
embolism. Ann Intern Med. 1998;129:997– 63 Patel VL, Kaufman DR, Arocha JF. Emerging
The effect of outcome knowledge on
1005. paradigms of cognition in medical decision-
50 Joseph J, Badrinath P, Basran GS, Sahn SA. Is making. J Biomed Inform. 2002;35:52–75. judgment under uncertainty. 1975. Qual Saf
the pleural fluid transudate or exudate? A 64 Redelmeier DA. The cognitive psychology of Health Care. 2003;12:304 –311.
revisit of the diagnostic criteria. Thorax. missed diagnoses. Ann Intern Med. 2005;142: 79 Wood G. The knew-it-all-along effect. J Exp
2001;56:867–870. 115–120. Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1978;4:345–
51 Bergus GR, Hamm RM. How physicians 65 Hall KH. Reviewing intuitive decision- 353.
make medical decisions and why medical making and uncertainty: The implications for 80 Eva KW, Neville AJ, Norman GR. Exploring
decision making can help. Med Decis medical education. Med Educ. 2002;36:216 – the aetiology of content specificity: Factors
Making. 1995;22:167–179. 224. influencing analogic transfer and problem
52 Pauker SG, Kassirer JP. Therapeutic decision 66 McDonald CJ. Medical heuristics: The silent solving. Acad Med. 1998;73(10 suppl):S1–S5.
making: A cost–benefit analysis. N Engl adjudicators of clinical practice. Ann Intern 81 Dhaliwal G. Clinical-decision-making:
J Med. 1975;293:229 –234. Med. 1996;124:56 –62. Understanding how clinicians make a
53 Pauker SG, Kassirer JP. The threshold 67 Voytovich AE, Rippey RM, Suffredini A. diagnosis. In: Saint S, Drazen JM, Solomon
approach to clinical decision making. N Engl Premature conclusions in diagnostic CG, eds. New England Journal of Medicine:
J Med. 1980;302:1109 –1117. reasoning. J Med Educ. 1985;60:302–307. Clinical Problem Solving. New York, NY:
54 Kassirer JP, Pauker SG. The toss-up. N Engl 68 McSherry D. Avoiding premature closure in McGraw-Hill Professional; 2006:19 –29.
J Med. 1981;305:1467–1469. sequential diagnosis. Artif Intell Med. 1997; 82 Berner ES, Graber ML. Overconfidence as a
55 Patel VL, Arocha JF, Zhang J. Thinking and 10:269 –283. cause of diagnostic error in medicine. Am J
reasoning in medicine. In: Holyoak K, ed. 69 Bordage G. Why did I miss the diagnosis? Med. 2008;121(5 suppl):S2–S23.
Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Some cognitive explanations and educational 83 Croskerry P. Achieving quality in clinical
Reasoning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge implications. Acad Med. 1999;74(10 suppl):
decision making: Cognitive strategies and
University Press; 2004. S138 –S143.
detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med. 2002;9:
56 Woods NN. Science is fundamental: The role 70 Croskerry P. The importance of cognitive
1184 –1204.
of biomedical knowledge in clinical errors in diagnosis and strategies to minimize
reasoning. Med Educ. 2007;41:1173–1177. them. Acad Med. 2003;78:775–780. 84 Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Penaforte JC.
57 Kuipers B, Kassirer JP. Causal reasoning in 71 Graber M, Gordon R, Franklin N. Reducing Effects of reflective practice on the accuracy
medicine: Analysis of a protocol. Cogn Sci. diagnostic errors in medicine: What’s the of medical diagnoses. Med Educ. 2008;42:
1984;8:363–385. goal? Acad Med. 2002;77:981–992. 468 –475.
58 Kuipers BJ. Commonsense reasoning about 72 Graber M. Metacognitive training to reduce 85 Kuhn GJ. Diagnostic errors. Acad Emerg
causality: Deriving behavior from structure. diagnostic errors: Ready for prime time? Acad Med. 2002;9:740 –750.
Artif Intell. 1984;24:169 –203. Med. 2003;78:781. 86 Colvin G. Talent Is Overrated. What Really
59 Tversky A, Kahneman D. The framing of 73 Newman-Toker D, Pronovost PJ. Diagnostic Separates World-Class Performers From
decisions and the psychology of choice. errors—The next frontier for patient safety. Everybody Else. New York, NY: Portfolio;
Science. 1981;211:453–458. JAMA. 2009;301:1060 –1062. 2008.

1124 Academic Medicine, Vol. 85, No. 7 / July 2010

Anda mungkin juga menyukai