Anda di halaman 1dari 10

Applied Neuropsychology Copyright 2001 by

2001, Vol. 8, No. 1, 41–50 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Longitudinal Assessment of Neuropsychological Functioning,


Psychiatric Status, Functional Disability and Employment Status in
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome LONGITUDINAL ASSESSMENT IN CHRONIC FATIGUE
TIERSKY
SYNDROME
ET AL.

Lana A. Tiersky
School of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, New Jersey, USA,
Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Medicine and Dentistry
of New Jersey, New Jersey Medical School, Newark, New Jersey, USA, and Chronic
Fatigue Syndrome Center, Newark, New Jersey, USA
John DeLuca
Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Medicine and Dentistry
of New Jersey, New Jersey Medical School, Newark, New Jersey, USA, Chronic Fatigue
Syndrome Center, Newark, New Jersey, USA, and Kessler Medical Research, Rehabilitation
and Education Corporation, West Orange, New Jersey, USA
Nancy Hill
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Center, Newark, New Jersey, USA
Sunil K. Dhar
Department of Mathematical Sciences and the Center for Applied Mathematics and
Statistics, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, New Jersey, USA
Susan K. Johnson
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina-Charlotte, Charlotte, North
Carolina, USA
Gudrun Lange
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Center, Newark, New Jersey, USA, and Departments of
Radiology and Psychiatry, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, New Jersey
Medical School, Newark, New Jersey, USA
Gabrielle Rappolt
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Center, Newark, New Jersey, USA
Benjamin H. Natelson
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Center, Newark, New Jersey, USA, and Department of
Neurosciences, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, New Jersey Medical
School, Newark, New Jersey, USA

The longitudinal course of subjective and objective neuropsychological functioning, psycho-


logical functioning, disability level, and employment status in chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS)
was examined. The relations among several key outcomes at follow-up, as well as the baseline

This research was supported by Grants U0I–AI32247 and R01–MH52810 from the National Institutes of Health.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Lana A. Tiersky, School of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, 1000 River Road, Mail Stop
T-WH1-01, Teaneck, NJ 07666, USA.

41
TIERSKY ET AL.

characteristics that predict change (e.g., improvement), were also evaluated. The study sample
consisted of 35 individuals who met the 1988 and 1994 CFS case definition criteria of the Cen-
ters for Disease Control (CDC) at intake. Participants were evaluated a mean of 41.9 (SEM =
1.7) months following their initial visit (range = 24–63 months). Results indicated that objec-
tive and subjective attention abilities, mood, level of fatigue, and disability improve over time in
individuals with CFS. Moreover, improvements in these areas were found to be interrelated at
follow-up. Finally, psychiatric status, age, and between-test duration were significant predic-
tors of outcome. Overall, the prognosis for CFS appears to be poor, as the majority of partici-
pants remained functionally impaired over time and were unemployed at follow-up, despite the
noted improvements.

Key words: chronic fatigue, neuropsychological assessment, follow-up

Although numerous cross-sectional studies docu- task (Cope, Pernet, Kendall, & David, 1995). In the
ment the extent and nature of cognitive impairment, second study, Vercoulen et al. (1996) found that sub-
psychological distress, disability, and unemployment jective concentration difficulties significantly de-
in chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS), it remains unclear creased over time in individuals who rated themselves
how these factors change over time (Tiersky, Johnson, as recovered from CFS or generally improved. Clearly,
Lange, Natelson, & DeLuca, 1997; Wessely, Hotopf, & little is known about the longitudinal course of subjec-
Sharpe, 1998). Few longitudinal studies have been tive and objective cognitive impairment in CFS.
completed, and even fewer have used CFS samples de- When examining the course of an illness, it is impor-
fined by recognized clinical criteria (Joyce, Hotopf, & tant to look at factors that affect prognosis. Although no
Wessely, 1997). Thus, the purpose of this investigation study has investigated the effect of baseline level of ob-
was to evaluate these variables over time in a group of jective neuropsychological impairment on final out-
CFS patients diagnosed according to case definition come, two studies have investigated how initial level of
criteria. subjective cognitive impairment affects prognosis
A key feature of CFS is subjective and objective (Ray, Jefferies, & Weir, 1997; Vercoulen et al., 1996).
cognitive impairment. It is estimated that between 74% The findings of these studies are mixed. One study re-
and 95% of CFS patients complain of some type of cog- ported that higher scores on a measure of cognitive
nitive deficit (Komaroff & Buchwald, 1991; Vercoulen problems at baseline predicted higher levels of fatigue
et al., 1998). In addition, studies of objective at follow-up (Ray et al., 1997). Vercoulen et al. (1996),
neuropsychological functioning in CFS consistently however, did not find that baseline level of cognitive
document impairment in information-processing difficulty affected prognosis. In addition, it remains an
speed, divided auditory attention, and memory (for re- open question whether objective neuropsychological
views, see DiPino & Kane, 1996; Moss-Morris, Petrie, abilities affect outcome. It is possible that objective
Large, & Kydd, 1996; Tiersky et al., 1997). neuropsychological abilities contribute to overall prog-
Given its centrality, it is important to examine the nosis in CFS, because neuropsychological impairment
longitudinal course of cognitive functioning in CFS, as was found to be related to functional disability in a
it likely affects the overall prognosis of the illness. cross-sectional study (Christodoulou et al., 1998).
Only two longitudinal studies have examined change in Psychiatric comorbidity is common in CFS
subjective or objective cognitive abilities in individuals (Wessely et al., 1998), but its status over time has not
diagnosed with CFS or chronic fatigue. One study in- been extensively investigated. One study that examined
vestigated cognitive functioning in 14 patients with the natural course of CFS found that self-reported
chronic fatigue over the course of an average of 6 symptoms of depression and general psychological
months. In this study, chronic fatigue was defined as well-being significantly improve over time in some
scoring above a certain cutoff point on a measure of fa- CFS patients (Vercoulen et al., 1996). Although few
tigue and demonstrating fatigue for at least 6 months. studies have examined the course of psychiatric distur-
Although limited by the small sample size and short du- bance in CFS, several have attempted to relate psychi-
ration of follow-up, this study found that the patients atric status at baseline to health status at follow-up. The
significantly improved on a block design task, a majority of studies have found that psychological status
supraspan learning task, and a verbal paired associates at initial evaluation is not related to improvement in

42
LONGITUDINAL ASSESSMENT IN CHRONIC FATIGUE SYNDROME

CFS symptoms, fatigue, or self-rated functional im- tus, and employment status in CFS over the course of
pairment at outcome (Hill, Tiersky, Scavalla, Lavietes, several years. Change in CFS severity and fatigue was
& Natelson, 1999; Ray et al. 1997; Vercoulen et al., also examined to provide descriptive information per-
1996; Wilson et al., 1994). Bombardier and Buchwald taining to severity of illness in the sample. In addition
(1995), however, found that having a dysthymic disor- to examining change over time, the baseline character-
der at initial evaluation predicted general improvement istics that predict improvement in objective
but accounted for only 2% of the variance. Moreover, neuropsychological functioning and disability, as well
studies that have looked at the relation between general as employment status at follow-up, were evaluated.
improvement and psychiatric status at follow-up have Finally, the relations among key outcomes were studied
found that improvement in health status is related to im- to determine if long-term performances were interre-
provement in psychiatric functioning (Hill et al., 1999; lated. Thus, we also investigated the relations among
Vercoulen et al., 1996; Wilson et al., 1994). Thus, we neuropsychological functioning, disability level, fa-
know little about the longitudinal course of psychiatric tigue, and mood at follow-up.
symptoms in CFS other than that as health status im-
proves, psychiatric status does as well.
Functional disability is also often observed in CFS, Method
but only a few authors have examined the course of dis-
ability over time (Ray et al., 1997; Vercoulen et al., Participants
1996; Wilson et al., 1994). In general these studies
show that although functional disability tends to signif- At study entry, 47 individuals who were participants
icantly improve, many patients remain disabled at fol- in the New Jersey Fatigue Research Center and who
low-up. For instance, Wilson et al. found that at completed a comprehensive neuropsychological and
follow-up 32% of participants were unable to perform psychological evaluation were invited to participate in
any significant physical activity and Vercoulen et al. the longitudinal investigation. In addition to meeting
found that at follow-up only 3% of the total sample was the 1988 and 1994 CFS case definition, participants
performing at the level of healthy participants on a also had to meet the following inclusion and exclusion
measure of functional ability. In addition, studies that criteria at study entry: (a) onset of CFS within the 4
have examined the factors that predict disability in CFS years prior to the initial evaluation, (b) presence of
conclude that illness duration, levels of baseline fatigue symptoms of at least moderate severity at intake, (c) no
(Ray et al., 1997), as well as baseline scores on mea- history of psychiatric disorder in the 5 years prior to in-
sures of illness beliefs such as “disease conviction” are take, (d) no substance abuse history, and (e) no loss of
related to functional impairment at follow-up (Wilson consciousness greater than 5 min.
et al., 1994). Thus, it appears that individuals with CFS All 47 individuals who participated in the initial
remain disabled over time and that illness beliefs, ill- baseline evaluation (Time 1) were later contacted to
ness duration, and initial fatigue level play some role in participate in the follow-up psychological and
this outcome. neuropsychological evaluations (Time 2). The follow-
Although unemployment is common in CFS ing procedures were utilized to contact patients at Time
(Wessely et al., 1998), only two studies have examined 2. First, all participants who participated at Time 1 were
employment status over time in individuals with CFS. contacted by phone to schedule an appointment for the
Both Hill et al. (1999) and Vercoulen et al. (1996) Time 2 evaluation. Participants who were unable to re-
found no significant change in employment status from turn in person at Time 2, but who were willing to partic-
baseline to follow-up in individuals with CFS. More- ipate in the study, were sent questionnaire measures to
over, little is known about what predicts employment complete. Individuals who were unwilling to partici-
status at follow-up. Specifically, Bombardier and pate at Time 2, as well as those who could not be
Buchwald (1995) found that demographic, clinical, or reached by phone, were sent a follow-up letter to re-
psychiatric variables were not predictive of return to quest participation. Those individuals who could not be
work. Thus, it appears that employment status does not contacted or who refused to participate were consid-
change in CFS over time. Still, the baseline factors that ered lost to follow-up or nonresponders.
contribute to this outcome are not yet identified. Of those participants contacted, 35 (74.5%) partici-
The purpose of this investigation was to examine pated at Time 2. These participants were designated as
change in subjective and objective neuropsychological responders. Two responders (4%) only completed
functioning, psychological functioning, disability sta- questionnaire measures that allowed us to rate CFS se-

43
TIERSKY ET AL.

verity because they lived geographically too distant to tom is a very severe problem). Information pertaining to
complete the in-person evaluations. Twelve (25.5%) of psychiatric status was obtained using the Quick Diag-
the 47 individuals declined to participate or could not nostic Interview Schedule for the Diagnostic and Statis-
be contacted and were designated nonresponders. Par- tical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., rev.
ticipants were reevaluated a mean of 41.9 (SEM = 1.7) [DSM–III–R]; American Psychiatric Association,
months following their initial visit (range = 24–63 1987), a structured computerized diagnostic interview
months). that was administered by trained personnel (Marcus,
At Time 1, the mean age of the responders was 35.5 Robins, & Bucholz, 1990). A psychiatric disorder was
(SEM = 1.6) years, the mean level of education was considered concurrent if the onset of the disorder oc-
14.8 (SEM = .40) years, and the mean duration of ill- curred following the onset of CFS. Thus, participants
ness was 25.9 (SEM = 2.5) months. At Time 2, the who were diagnosed with a concurrent Axis I disor-
mean age of the responders was 38.9 (SEM = 1.5) years, der(s) at Time 1 were noted to have the disorder concur-
and the mean educational level, which did not signifi- rent with CFS. In addition, lifetime history of psychiat-
cantly change over time (p > .05), was 14.8 (SEM = .40) ric illness was also evaluated. Axis II status was
years. Some of the individuals who participated in this determined based on the participants’ responses on the
investigation were also included in an earlier investiga- Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire–Revised, which
tion of longitudinal functioning in CFS (Hill et al., is a self-report questionnaire (Hyler & Rieder, 1987).
1999). At Time 1, participants also completed a
neuropsychological evaluation that included the fol-
lowing measures: The California Verbal Learning Test
Procedure (CVLT; Delis, Dramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987); the
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT;
Initial baseline evaluation (Time 1). At Time 1, Gronwall, 1977); the Rey–Osterreith Complex Figure
all participants underwent a comprehensive psycholog- Test, Immediate Recall (ROCF–I) and Delayed Recall
ical and physical assessment. During the physical as- (ROCF–D) subtests (Corwin & Bylsma, 1993); the
sessment, a medical history was obtained by a physi- Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS–R)
cian’s assistant or nurse practitioner trained in the Digit Span Forward and WAIS–R Digit Span Back-
diagnosis of CFS. At this time, the severity of each indi- ward subtests (Wechsler, 1981); the Meta-Memory
vidual’s CFS was rated by the clinician using a 6-point Questionnaire (Mateer, Sohlberg, & Crinean, 1987);
scale. The CFS severity category rankings are listed in the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward,
Table 1. CFS severity was rated based on level of reduc- Medelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961); and the
tion in activity and number and severity of minor symp- State–Trait Anxiety Inventory–Form X (Spielberger,
toms rated from 0 (symptom is no problem) to 5 (symp- Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1983). All measures were admin-
istered according to published procedures.
Table 1. CFS Severity Rating Scale In addition, at Time 1 participants completed the Fa-
tigue Rating Scale (Krupp, LaRocca, Muir-Nash, &
Rating Definition of Numeric Rating Steinberg, 1989), which is a self-report measure. A
1 ≥ 50% reduction in activity and 7 symptoms rated as self-report measure of magnitude of disability due to
at least substantial (severe CFS) illness, the CFS Disability Scale (CDS), was also com-
2 ≥ 50% reduction in activity and 4–6 symptoms rated pleted. The CDS is a modification of the Karnofsky
as at least substantial Performance Index (Wilson et al., 1994) in which the
3 < 50% reduction in activity and 4 or more symptoms areas of mild to moderate disability are expanded.
rated as at least substantial (at least 3 symptoms Moreover, it uses a scoring system whereby each dis-
rated as very severe)
ability rating is defined within a 10-point band. The
4 < 50% reduction in activity and 4 or more symptoms
rated as at least substantial (no more than 2 CDS has been utilized in other studies examining func-
symptoms may be rated as very severe) tional ability in CFS (Natelson et al., 1995). Employ-
5 ≥ 50% reduction in activity (must be at least ment status (i.e., employed full time or part time or
substantial) and 4 or more symptoms rated less unemployed or retired due to health) was also deter-
than substantial mined during the initial evaluation.
6 Substantial reduction in activity and 4 or more
symptoms rated less than substantial
Follow-up evaluation (Time 2). All participants
Note: CFS = chronic fatigue syndrome. who returned in person at Time 2 underwent both a

44
LONGITUDINAL ASSESSMENT IN CHRONIC FATIGUE SYNDROME

physical assessment and a neuropsychological evalua- least 1 point from Time 1 to Time 2 on the measure of cli-
tion. At this time, a follow-up psychiatric interview was nician-rated CFS severity were placed in the improved
completed, and a follow-up medical history was also category, and those that did not demonstrate any positive
obtained. The procedures for administration of the Time change in symptoms were categorized as not improved.
2 assessments were the same as those followed at Time The following were used as predictor variables in all
1 except that lifetime history of disease or of the regression analyses: duration of illness, illness se-
psychopathology was not reassessed during the medical verity, duration of time lapsed between baseline and fol-
history and psychiatric interview. A trained physician’s low-up evaluation (“between test duration”), presence
assistant or nurse practitioner rated CFS severity during of a concurrent psychiatric diagnosis, age, gender, and
the follow-up evaluation. education at Time 1. To predict improvement in level of
neuropsychological functioning, also included in the
model was disability status at Time 1. To predict im-
Analyses provement in disability status, level of
neuropsychological impairment at Time 1 was also in-
Due to the non-normal distributions of the data, cluded. Finally, to predict employment status at Time 2,
Wilcoxon matched-pair sign-rank tests were used to ex- level of neuropsychological impairment and disability
amine the significance of change over time for the con- status at Time 1 were included in the model. A backward
tinuous variables. McNemar’s binomial probability test stepwise elimination method was used to complete all of
was utilized to examine change over time for the dichot- the logistic regression analyses. Specifically, all of the
omous variables. When normality assumptions for two predictor variables were included in the initial model.
independent samples were not satisfied, Mann–Whitney Then, the variable whose coefficient was the least signif-
U tests were utilized to compare group differences icant, including that of the constant, was removed from
within Time 1. A proportion test (the Z test) was used to the model one step at a time.
examine improvement in CFS severity. Because the To examine overall neuropsychological function-
sample size was large (n > 30), the Central Limit Theo- ing, a neuropsychological impairment index was cre-
rem was invoked to validate the use of the Z test. ated that was similar to that utilized in other studies of
Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were com- cognitive functioning in CFS (Christodoulou et al.,
puted to determine the level of association among the 1998). This index was created by summing the number
variables because they were non-normal in distribution of tests a participant failed. A test was considered failed
as well. if the individual performed 2 or more SD below the
Logistic regression analyses were completed to pre- mean of a healthy control group on a given test. The
dict improvement in disability level and data on the healthy comparison group have been pub-
neuropsychological functioning, as well as employ- lished elsewhere (DeLuca, Johnson, Beldowicz, &
ment status at Time 2. Improvement in disability was Natelson, 1995). The measures included in the index
defined as a 10-point decrease in disability level as were the following: CVLT total score (CVLT–T),
measured by the CDS. The improved group, therefore, long-delay free recall (CVLT–LDF), and short-delay
consisted of participants whose CDS scores improved free recall (CVLT–SDF); PASAT total score; ROCF–I
by at least 10 points at Time 2. Those participants who and ROCF–D; and WAIS–R Digit Span Forward and
did not demonstrate such an improvement were consid- WAIS–R Digit Span Backward subtests.
ered not improved. Ten points was chosen as the crite-
ria for improvement because each 10-point band of
scores on the CDS defines a different disability cate- Results
gory. Improvement in neuropsychological functioning
was defined as a 1-point decrease on the impairment in- Comparison Between Responders and
dex from Time 1 to Time 3. Thus, participants who de- Nonresponders on Time 1 Measures
creased by 1 point on the impairment index from Time
1 to Time 2 were placed in the improved category, and To examine for the possibility of a sampling bias,
those that did not demonstrate such a decrease in im- the Time 1 performance of the responders (n = 35) was
pairment were considered not improved. compared to those who did not respond
Improvement in CFS severity was defined as a (nonresponders, n = 12). There were no significant dif-
1-point increase in CFS category (i.e., a lessening of ferences in age, education, illness severity, illness dura-
symptoms). Thus, those participants who improved by at tion, employment status, gender, level of depression, or

45
TIERSKY ET AL.

concurrent psychiatric status between responders and Table 2. Performance of Responders on Psychological,
Illness-Related, and Disability Measures at Time 1 and Time 2
nonresponders at Time 1 (all p >. 05). However, the
nonresponders demonstrated lower levels of anxiety Time 1 Time 2
(responders: Mdn = 38, range = 21–78; nonresponders:
Mdn = 27, range = 20–74; p = .02) and less disability at Mdn Range Mdn Range p
Time 1 than the responders (responders: Mdn = 30, BDI 13 4–30 10 0–33 .02
range = 20–60; nonresponders: Mdn = 45; range = STAI 38 21–78 39 23–67 .66
20–70; p = .03) The median disability level of the Fatigue 60 49–63 57 17–63 .01
nonresponder group corresponded to a disability rank- CDS 30 20–60 40 20–100 .01
ing identified by the following characteristics: not con-
Note: BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; STAI = State–Trait
fined to the house, unable to perform strenuous duties, Anxiety Inventory; Fatigue = Fatigue Rating Scale; CDS = CFS
and able to perform light-duty desk work 3 to 4 hr a day Disability Scale.
but requires rest periods. In contrast, the median dis-
ability level of the responder group corresponded to a
disability ranking identified by the following character- Table 3. Performance of Responders on Neuropsychological
Measures at Time 1 and Time 2
istics: usually confined to house, unable to perform any
strenuous tasks, and able to perform desk work 2 to 3 hr Time 1 Time 2
a day but requires rest periods. Finally, there were no
significant differences between the groups in level of Mdn Range Mdn Range p
neuropsychological functioning at Time 1 (all p >. 05). CVLT–T 52 30–76 54 29–75 .89
CVLT–SDFR 11 4–16 11 3–16 .76
CVLT–LDFR 10 4–16 12 2–16 .31
Change From Time 1 to Time 2 ROCF–I 34 4–64 35 0–66 .40
ROCF–D 35 3–64 36 3–63 .47
W–DSF 9 6–14 9 5–12 .84
CFS severity and fatigue. At Time 1, all partici- W–DSB 9 3–13 7 3–12 .26
pants met both the 1988 and 1994 CDC case definition PASAT 128 60–177 137 75–189 .01
criteria for CFS, and 97% of the participants demon- MMQ 73 38–136 67 32–111 .01
strated severe CFS (see Table 1 for a definition of severe Note: CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; CVLT–T = total
CFS). At Time 2, only 1 participant no longer met either score; CVLT–LDF = long-delay free recall; CVLT–SDF =
the 1988 or 1994 CDC criteria. Moreover, clini- short-delay free recall; PASAT = Paced Auditory Serial Addition
cian-rated CFS severity did not significantly improve Test total score; ROCF–I = Rey–Osterreith Complex Figure Test,
immediate recall; ROCF–D = delayed recall; W–DSF = Wechsler
from Time 1 to Time 2 (p = .40). Specifically, 57% of Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised Digit Span Forward–total Digits;
the CFS participants demonstrated improvement over W–DSB = Digit Span Backward–total Digits; MMQ =
time, whereas 43% did not. At follow-up, all of the par- Meta-Memory Questionnaire total score.
ticipants designated as not improved continued to dem-
onstrate severe CFS. Moreover, 45% of the improved
participants continued to rate some of their symptoms as
at least a substantial problem. Finally, although no Concurrent psychiatric status. No significant
change in CFS severity was observed, the participants change in the proportion of concurrent Axis I or Axis II
did demonstrate an improvement in level of fatigue over (all p >. 05) disorders in the sample was observed over
time (p = .01; see Table 2). time. Specifically, at both Time 1 and Time 2, 34% of
the participants met the criteria for a concurrent Axis I
Neuropsychological status. As demonstrated in disorder. In addition, no change in Axis II performance
Table 3, significant improvement from Time 1 to Time was observed with a prevalence of 45% during both
2 was noted on some of the neuropsychological vari- time periods. Despite the lack of change in the propor-
ables. Notably, a significant positive change in perfor- tion of concurrent Axis I and Axis II disorders in the
mance was observed on the PASAT, a measure of com- sample over time, a significant improvement was noted
plex information processing (p = .01) as well as on the on a measure of mood, the BDI (p = .02; see Table 2).
Meta-Memory Test, a self-report measure of attentional
and memory functioning (p = .01). No other significant Disability and employment status. As illus-
changes were found. trated in Table 2, initial level of disability significantly

46
LONGITUDINAL ASSESSMENT IN CHRONIC FATIGUE SYNDROME

diminished at follow-up (p = .01). At Time 2, the me- Table 5. Correlations Among Fatigue, CFS Severity, Disability,
Mood, and Objective and Subjective Neuropsychological
dian level of functioning on the disability measure cor- Functioning at Time 2
responded to a disability ranking characterized by: not
confined to the house, unable to perform strenuous du- Fatigue CDS BDI Neuro MMQ
ties, able to perform light-duty desk work 3 to 4 hr a day
but requires rest periods. The sample did not change sig- Fatigue
CDS –.78*
nificantly from Time 1 to Time 2 in employment status
BDI .60* –.57*
(p >. 05). Specifically, 68% of the participants were un-
Neuro .44** –.66* .53*
employed at both time periods.
MMQ .69* –.84* .55* .59*

Note: Fatigue = Krupp Fatigue Rating Scale; CDS = CFS


Predicting Time 2 Performance From Time 1 Disability rating scale; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; Neuro =
level of neuropsychological impairment (total number of tests
Performance failed); MMQ = Meta-Memory Questionnaire total score.
*p < .01. **p < .02.
Predicting neuropsychological performance. As
illustrated in Table 4, between-test duration was the
at baseline to contribute to lower odds of improvement
only variable that significantly predicted improvement
in disability status.
in neuropsychological functioning when the baseline
log odds were set to zero, c2(1, N = 25) = 8.16, p = .01.
Predicting employment status. Concurrent psy-
This model demonstrates that the longer the duration of
chiatric status and age at Time 1 significantly predicted
time lapsed between baseline and follow-up evalua-
employment at Time 2, when the baseline log odds were
tions, the lower the odds of improvement in
set to zero (a = 0), c2(2, N = 35) = 13.50, p = .01 (see Ta-
neuropsychological functioning.
ble 4). This model demonstrates that being older at base-
line lowers the odds of being employed at Time 2 and
that having a concurrent psychiatric diagnosis at intake
Predicting disability. Concurrent psychiatric
contributes to higher odds in favor of being employed at
history at Time 1 and age at Time 1 predicted improve-
Time 2.
ment in level of disability when the baseline log odds
were set to zero, c2(2, N = 32) = 9.63, p = .01 (see Table
4). As Table 4 illustrates, the positive coefficient for the
variable measuring psychiatric status indicates that hav- Interrelated Variables at Time 2
ing a concurrent psychiatric diagnosis at Time 1 con-
tributes to higher odds in favor of improvement in dis- The relations at Time 2 among the variables that sig-
ability. The negative coefficient for the variable nificantly changed over time were also examined. As is
measuring age reflects that there is a trend for older age illustrated in Table 5, improvement in level of fatigue,
mood, disability level, neuropsychological function-
ing, and subjective attention and memory abilities were
all interrelated.
Table 4. Logistic Regression Models for Neuropsychological
Improvement, Improvement in Disability, and Time 2 Employment
Status
Discussion
Wald
Predictors Beta SE c2(1) p
This study was designed to examine long-term
Neuropsychological change in neuropsychological functioning, psychiatric
Improvement status, disability, and employment status in individuals
Between-Test Duration –.03 0.01 6.57 .01 with CFS. Disability level, depression, fatigue, subjec-
Disability Improvement tive attention and memory, and complex information
Age –.03 0.01 3.40 .07
processing were found to improve. Moreover, when
Psychiatric Status 2.61 0.93 7.40 .01
compared to standard normative values, CFS partici-
Time 2 Employment Status
pants improved to the extent that at Time 2 they were
Age –.05 0.02 8.95 .01
Psychiatric Status 1.93 0.83 5.44 .02
functioning in the normal to near-normal range in two
of these areas. Specifically, on the PASAT, participants

47
TIERSKY ET AL.

performed in the average range at Time 2 (Brittain, La sufferers with no such history is that the etiology of
Marche, Reeder, Roth, & Thomas, 1991). Likewise, the their CFS symptoms may differ. Psychiatric disorders
level of depression demonstrated by the sample at Time such as depression have a variable course in which
2 fell in the mild to minimal range (Beck et al., 1961). symptoms fluctuate, and often a natural remission in
Thus, these findings are consistent with prior studies symptoms occurs (American Psychiatric Association,
(for a review, see Joyce et al., 1997). 1987). Thus, it is possible that the more favorable
Despite the observed improvement, the majority of course observed in participants with CFS and a psychi-
the participants continued to demonstrate CFS symp- atric history reflects an underlying psychiatric etiology.
toms, were unemployed (68%), and remained function- Participants with CFS with no psychiatric history, how-
ally impaired at Time 2. Only a single individual was ever, may have another cause for their symptoms. For
found to no longer meet the 1988 or 1994 CFS criteria instance, it is possible that participants with CFS and no
at follow-up. Moreover, the median CDS score at Time psychiatric history have a neurologic cause to the ill-
2 indicated that participants were not able to perform ness, the consequence of which is a chronic and intrac-
strenuous duties and were only able to perform table course. Two studies provide some support for this
light-duty desk work for 3 to 4 hr a day. The CDS score notion (DeLuca, Johnson, Ellis, & Natelson, 1997;
indicated that even this amount of work required rest Lange et al., 1999). Specifically, Lange et al. (1999)
periods. This level of disability and illness suggests a found that CFS sufferers with no psychiatric history
poor overall prognosis in CFS. demonstrated a greater number of MRI abnormalities
It is possible that the participants in this investiga- than CFS patients with a concurrent psychiatric history.
tion were more functionally disabled at Time 2 than In addition, DeLuca et al. (1997) found that CFS suffer-
participants included in other studies. For instance, we ers with no psychiatric history demonstrated greater
found higher unemployment rates in our study at Time neuropsychological impairment than those with a posi-
2 as compared to other authors (i.e., Bombardier & tive psychiatric history. Future research should investi-
Buchwald, 1995; Vercoulen et al., 1996). In addition, gate the possible differences in illness course in CFS
Wilson et al. (1994) reported higher levels of function- sufferers with differing psychiatric histories or illness
ing at follow-up in their investigation, which used the etiologies.
clinician-rated Karnofsky Performance Index. They Moreover, the fact that some individuals with CFS
found CFS participants to be functioning at a level be- develop psychiatric disorders and others do not sug-
tween “cares for self, unable to carry on normal activity gests differences in coping style and adaptation. Thus,
or to do active work” and “normal activity with effort.” it is also possible that the factors that maintain the ill-
This comparison between studies must be interpreted ness are different in CFS participants with a psychiatric
cautiously, however, as the measures used to evaluate history as compared to those with no psychiatric his-
outcome differed (i.e., a self-report measure of disabil- tory. For example, participants with CFS and no psy-
ity was used in our investigation, and a clinician rating chiatric history may accommodate to their illness more
of disability was utilized in the study by Wilson et al.). readily and thus not develop an emotional disorder.
This discrepancy highlights the need for a future inves- CFS patients with no concurrent psychiatric disorders
tigation to examine long-term disability level in indi- may also be more behaviorally disengaged as evi-
viduals with CFS using both self-report as well as denced by the fact that they are more likely to be unem-
objective criteria, such as the Karnofsky Performance ployed than those with a psychiatric history. Illness
Index. accommodation and behavioral disengagement have
Based on the results of this investigation, it also ap- both been found to predict poor outcome in some par-
pears that having a concurrent psychiatric disorder at ticipants with CFS (Ray et al., 1997). A future investi-
intake (or Time 1) is positively related to long-term out- gation should examine changes over time in these
come. Patients diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder cognitive variables in CFS participants with differing
concurrent with their illness onset demonstrated greater psychiatric histories.
odds of improvement in level of disability than those In contrast to the findings of this investigation, other
with no history of a concurrent psychiatric disorder. authors have not found that psychiatric status at intake
Participants with a concurrent psychiatric diagnosis at predicts employment status or improvement in func-
intake were also more likely to be employed at fol- tioning at follow-up (Bombardier & Buchwald, 1995;
low-up than those without such a history. Hill et al., 1999; Ray et al. 1997; Vercoulen, 1996; Wil-
One reason patients with a concurrent psychiatric son et al., 1994). These discrepant findings may be due
history may demonstrate better outcomes from CFS to methodological differences, such as the use of differ-

48
LONGITUDINAL ASSESSMENT IN CHRONIC FATIGUE SYNDROME

ent outcome measures or the use of different measures that treatment targeted toward improving one or a few
to evaluate psychiatric distress. For example, of the areas may result in improvement in the other do-
Vercoulen et al. (1996) and Wilson et al. (1994) used mains. For instance, cognitive behavioral therapy
self-reported change as a final outcome variable. Other (CBT), which has been found to improve overall level
primary outcomes have included self-reported fatigue of disability and mood in CFS patients (Deale, Chalder,
and functional impairment (Ray et al., 1997; Vercoulen Marks, & Wessely, 1997; Sharpe et al., 1996), may also
et al., 1996); clinician-rated functional impairment result in an improvement in level of
(Wilson et al., 1994); change in immunological param- neuropsychological impairment. A topic for future re-
eters (Wilson et al., 1994); clinician-rated improve- search is whether CBT can improve cognitive function-
ment (Hill et al., 1999); and return to work (Bombardier ing in CFS.
& Buchwald, 1995). Measures of psychiatric status at
baseline have included a variety of measures such as
the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (Ray et al.,
References
1997), the neuroticism subtest of the Eysenck Personal-
ity Inventory, and the Diagnostic Interview Schedule American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statisti-
for the DSM–III–R (American Psychiatric Association, cal manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington,
1987; Bombardier & Buchwald, 1995; Hill et al., DC: Author.
1999). Thus, due to the use of differing measures, it is Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J.
difficult to directly compare the studies. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Demographic factors also appear to be related to
Bombardier, C. H., & Buchwald, D. (1995). Outcome and prognosis
long-term outcome. In this study it was found that older of patients with chronic fatigue vs. chronic fatigue syndrome.
patients at intake had a higher risk of unemployment at Archives of Internal Medicine, 155, 2105–2110.
Time 2, a finding that has been observed by other au- Brittain, J. L., La Marche, J. A., Reeder, K. P., Roth, D. L., & Boll, T.
thors (for a review, see Joyce et al., 1997). There was J. (1991). Effects of age and IQ on Paced Auditory Serial Addi-
also a trend for older patients to have lower odds of im- tion Task (PASAT) performance. The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 5, 163–175.
provement on the disability measure.
Christodoulou, C., DeLuca, J., Lange, G., Johnson, S. K., Sisto, S.
One limitation of our investigation is that the dura- A., Korn, L., & Natelson, B. H. (1998). Relation between
tion between baseline and follow-up testing was not neuropsychological impairment and functional disability in pa-
constant for all participants. This limitation was ad- tients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Neurology,
dressed by including between-test duration as a vari- Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 64, 431–434.
able in the regression analyses. This allowed us to Cope, H., Pernet, A., Kendall, B., & David, A. (1995). Cognitive
functioning and magnetic resonance imaging in chronic fa-
examine how elapsed time may have affected our three tigue. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 86–94.
main outcomes: neuropsychological impairment, dis- Corwin, J., & Bylsma, F. W. (1993). Translations of excerpts from
ability, and employment. We found that between-test Andre Rey’s “Psychological examination of traumatic
duration only impacted neuropsychological function- encephalopathy” and P. A. Osterreith’s “The Complex Figure
ing. Specifically, the longer the duration between base- Copy Test.” The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 3–15.
line and follow-up testing, the lower the odds of Deale, A., Chalder, T., Marks, I., & Wessely, S. (1997). Cognitive be-
havior therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: A randomized con-
detecting improvement. Given this finding, it is possi- trolled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 408–414.
ble that our repeated measures analyses did not capture Delis, D. C., Dramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (1987). Cali-
significant changes on some of the neuropsychological fornia Verbal Learning Test: Adult version. San Antonio, TX:
measures. Psychological Corporation.
Finally, the findings of our investigation have two DeLuca, J., Johnson, S. K., Beldowicz, D., & Natelson, B. H. (1995).
Neuropsychological impairments in chronic fatigue syndrome,
main clinical implications. Given that participants
multiple sclerosis, and depression. Journal of Neurology, Neu-
demonstrate some improvement in neuropsychological rosurgery, and Psychiatry, 58, 38–43.
functioning, periodic reevaluation appears warranted DeLuca, J., Johnson, S. K., Ellis, S. P., & Natelson, B. H. (1997).
so that level of functioning can be most accurately esti- Cognitive functioning is impaired in patients with chronic fa-
mated. Reevaluation can also provide information tigue syndrome devoid of psychiatric disease. Journal of Neu-
about whether interventions such as psychotherapy or rology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 62, 151–155.
DiPino, K. R., & Kane, L. R. (1996). Neurocognitive functioning in
vocational rehabilitation are indicated. In addition, as is
chronic fatigue syndrome. Neuropsychology Review, 6, 47–60.
consistent with the findings of other authors, we noted Gronwall, D. (1977). Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task: A mea-
that many areas of functioning improved concurrently sure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual Motor Skills, 44,
(Ray et al., 1997). These related improvements suggest 367–373.

49
TIERSKY ET AL.

Hill, N. F., Tiersky, L. A., Scavalla, V. R., Lavietes, M., & Natelson, Prognosis in chronic fatigue syndrome: A prospective study on
B. H. (1999). Natural history of severe chronic fatigue syn- the natural course. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
drome. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 80, Psychiatry, 60, 489–494.
1–5. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised
Hyler, S. E., & Rieder, R. O. (1987). PDQ–R. New York: New York manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.
State Psychiatric Institute. Wessely, S., Hotopf, M., & Sharpe, M. (1998). Chronic fatigue and
Joyce, J., Hotopf, M., & Wessely, S. (1997). The prognosis of its syndromes. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome: A systematic re- Wilson, A., Hickie, I., Lloyd, A., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Boughton, C.,
view. Quarterly Journal of Medicine, 90, 223–233. Dwyer, J., & Wakefield, D. (1994). Longitudinal study of out-
Komaroff, A. L., & Buchwald, D. (1991). Symptoms and signs in come of chronic fatigue syndrome. British Medical Journal,
chronic fatigue syndrome. Reviews of Infectious Diseases, 13, 308, 756–759.
S8–S11.
Krupp, L. B., LaRocca, N. G., Muir-Nash, J., & Steinberg, A. D.
(1989). The fatigue severity scale: Application to patients with
multiple sclerosis and systemic lupus erythematosus. Archives
of Neurology, 46, 1121–1123.
Lange, G., DeLuca, J., Maldjian, J. A., Lee, H.-J., Tiersky, L. A., & Original submission November 15, 1999
Natelson, B. H. (1999). Brain MRI abnormalities exist in a sub- Accepted June 19, 2000
set of patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Neu-
rological Sciences, 171, 3–7.
Marcus, S., Robins, L. N., & Bucholz, K. (1990). Quick Diagnostic
Interview Schedule III–R, Version 1. St Louis, MO: Washing- Appendix
ton University School of Medicine. Management of Missing Values
Mateer, C. A., Sohlberg, M. M., & Crinean, J. (1987). Focus on clini-
cal research: Perceptions of memory function in individuals When creating the impairment index, the following
with closed-head injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilita- methods were used to statistically manage missing val-
tion, 2, 74–84.
Moss-Morris, R., Petrie, K. J., Large, R. G., & Kydd, R. R. (1996).
ues. The test variable PASAT total score had nine miss-
Neuropsychological deficits in chronic fatigue syndrome: Arti- ing observations at Time 1 because the participants
fact or reality? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psy- were not administered the measure. Values for these
chiatry, 60, 474–477. nine missing data points were computed using an opti-
Natelson, B. H., Johnson, S. K., DeLuca, J. Sisto, S., Ellis, S. P., Hill, mal regression model that included the remaining
N., & Bergen, M. T. (1995). Reducing heterogeneity in chronic seven cognitive measures (i.e., CVLT–T, CVLT–LDF,
fatigue syndrome: A comparison with depression and multiple
sclerosis. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 21, 1204–1210.
CVLT–SDF, ROCF–I, ROCF–D, WAIS–R Digit Span
Ray, C., Jefferies, S., & Weir, W. R. C. (1997). Coping and other Forward and Digit Span Backward subtests). Spe-
predictors of outcome in chronic fatigue syndrome: A 1-year cifically, to generate the scores for the nine missing val-
follow-up. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 43, ues, all seven of the cognitive variables were entered in
405–415. the multiple linear regression model, and a stepwise
Sharpe, M., Hawton, K., Simkin, S., Surawy, C., Hackmann, A.,
backward elimination procedure was performed. The
Klimes, I., Peto, T., Warrell, D., & Seagroatt, V. (1996). Cog-
nitive behavior therapy for the chronic fatigue syndrome: A final model, which was selected on the basis of having
randomized controlled trial. British Medical Journal, 312, the smallest sum of square error (SSE), included the fol-
22–26. lowing predictor variables: ROCF–I, CVLT–SDF, and
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R., & Lushene, R. (1983). The ROCF–D (SSE = 25.55). An analysis of variance
State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) test manual. Palo Alto, showed that this regression model was significant with
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
p = .00. Although the model coefficients corresponding
Tiersky, L. A., Johnson, S. K., Lange, G., Natelson, B. H., &
DeLuca, J. (1997). Neuropsychology of chronic fatigue syn- to ROCF–I and CVL–SDF were not significant (p = .21
drome: A critical review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental and .22, respectively), they were kept in the final model
Neuropsychology, 19, 560–586. because the objective of getting the model with the
Vercoulen, J. H. M. M., Bazelmans, E., Swanink, C. M. A., Galama, smallest SSE, which yields the best prediction, was met
J. M. D., Fennis, J. F. M, Van der Meer, J. W. M., & with their inclusion. Thus, the final model used to pre-
Bleijenberg, G. (1998). Evaluating neuropsychological impair-
dict PASAT total score for the nine missing vales was
ment in chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Ex-
perimental Neuropsychology, 20, 144–156. the following: PASAT (score) = 76.68 + 1.67 ×
Vercoulen, J. H. M. M., Swanink, C. M. A., Ferris, J. F. M., Galama, ROCF–D (score) + 1.90 × CVL–SDF (score) – .96 ×
J. M. D., Van der Meer, J. W. M., & Bleijenberg, G. (1996). ROCF–I (score).

50

Anda mungkin juga menyukai