Anda di halaman 1dari 7

Sleep Health 4 (2018) 565–571

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Sleep Health
Journal of the National Sleep Foundation

journal homepage: sleephealthjournal.org

Perceived stress and worldview influence sleep quality in Bolivian and


United States university students
Jesse Doolin, MS a,⁎, Jose Enrique Vilches, MS b, Cheryl Cooper, PhD, MSN c,
Christine Gipson, PhD c, William Sorensen, PhD, MSPH b,c
a
University of Texas Health Science Center San Antonio, San Antonio, TX
b
Universidad Autónoma Gabriel Rene Moreno, Santa Cruz, Bolivia
c
University of Texas at Tyler, Tyler, TX

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objective: This study investigated predictors of poor sleep quality among American and Bolivian students.
Received 12 December 2017 Design: A cross-sectional survey was designed and administered to undergraduate university students.
Received in revised form 11 August 2018 Setting and participants: Psychology classes from 2 public universities (Texas, USA, and Bolivia) were conve-
Accepted 15 August 2018 nience sampled (n = 80 American students; 60 Bolivian students).
Measurements: We used a sleep quality index and the Perceived Stress Scale. We added questions
Keywords:
concerning worldview, sleep hygiene, sleep deficiency, health behaviors, and demographics.
Sleep quality
College students
Results: Five variables predicted poor sleep quality: perceived stress, sleep hygiene, sleep deficiency, world-
Bolivia view, and site. Greater stress positively associated with poorer sleep in both cultures. Yet, Bolivian students
Stress reported significantly more stress than American students but experienced significantly better sleep qual-
Culture ity. Worldview, a measure of optimism or pessimism about the world, negatively associated with sleep
quality in both cultures.
Conclusion: Three variables that predict sleep quality (worldview, sleep hygiene, and sleep deficiency) did
not differ significantly between countries. Only perceived stress differed by country but contrary to the pre-
dicted direction. Thus, this work reveals new avenues for future work to the investigation of sleep in differ-
ent cultures. Lastly, we offer recommendations to ameliorate poor sleep in university students.
© 2018 National Sleep Foundation. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction with others is common. In a 2007 article, Worthman and Brown10 re-
ported on the findings from a study that explored sleep among Egyp-
Stress is pervasive in college students. Lack of sleep mediated tian families, with results indicating that age, sex, and co-sleeping
through stress can affect both physical and mental health.1–5 Stress influence sleeping arrangements, thus impacting overall sleep quality.
can delay the onset of sleep or shorten sleeping times. Likewise, Conversely, solitary sleeping is the most common practice in Western
sleep influences the stress response during waking hours. 6 A study societies.11 Therefore, assessing sleep behaviors while also considering
by Buboltz and colleagues7 determined that 73% of college students cultural norms, or beliefs, deserves further investigation.
reported occasional sleep problems and 15% had poor sleep overall. We underscore “sleep behaviors” in light of recent anthropologi-
Relatively little research has been conducted that investigates how cal studies. For instance, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, asserted
specific cultural contexts influence sleep behaviors.8 Worthman and to be high-quality sleep, allows humans to stay relatively active into
Melby9 compared the literature on sleep behavior among adolescents the night,12 albeit a sleep debt response may ensue. 13 And cross-cul-
across cultures in non-Western societies. They found that sleep set- tural analyses from traditional societies have revealed that although 7
tings are generally social, sleeping in solitude is rare, and co-sleeping hours seems to be the human species’ mean for sleep duration, how
that 7 hours is distributed within the 24-hour circadian cycle is rela-
tively flexible and illustrates a significant degree of plasticity.14
Other studies in psychology have investigated coping behaviors
⁎ Corresponding author at: Polytrauma Rehabilitation Center, 7400 Merton Minter
Blvd, San Antonio, TX 78229. Tel.: +1 832 641 8055; fax: +1 903 565 5923. and their influence on stress. Coping as a means for stress reduction
E-mail address: doolin@uthscsa.edu (J. Doolin). can be divided into 2 broad categories: problem-focused coping,

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2018.08.006
2352-7218/© 2018 National Sleep Foundation. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
566 J. Doolin et al. / Sleep Health 4 (2018) 565–571

characterized by cognitions or behaviors aimed at solving the prob- Methods


lem or changing a situation, and emotion-focused coping, character-
ized by behaviors elicited to control the uncomfortable emotional Participants
response caused by the problem. 15 Problem-focused coping, rather
than emotion-focused coping, is viewed as being a more effective ap- We analyzed the responses of 140 undergraduate students en-
proach for reducing stress.16–18 There is evidence that psychological rolled in a psychology program from either a mid-sized, public,
distress propels negative changes in sleep quality, particularly with South-Central US university or a large, public university in Bolivia.
respect to the duration in REM sleep. Problem-focused behaviors re- Their average age was 24.9 years (range 18-45).
sult in less need for REM sleep. 19 This finding is supported from a
study of adult Japanese showing that several emotion-related behav- Measures
iors (smoking, binge-snacking, and excessive TV/gaming consump-
tion) were positively associated with insomnia, whereas problem- Comfort being alone
solving approaches to stress were negatively correlated with Participants marked on a 5-point Likert scale whether they
insomnia20. enjoyed being alone. Scores approaching 5 indicated comfort in a sol-
O'Connor and colleagues21 evaluated stress between Western and itary lifestyle.
Eastern lifestyles, comparing individual-oriented vs society-oriented,
or collectivist, values. Japanese university students demonstrated sig- Health behavior
nificantly more stress than did British university students when mea- This component was composed of 3 health-related behavioral
sured by a Western stress instrument (Perceived Stress Scale [PSS]). questions (physical activity, smoking cigarettes, and consumption
However, the researchers warned that notions of stress emerge of soda) in which the participants engaged. Scores were summed so
from Western paradigms and are therefore subject to Western bias. that very healthy and very unhealthy behaviors were reflected in
Nonetheless, when Korean nursing students were measured on the the extreme ends of the index.
same stress scale, they demonstrated less stress than both British
and Japanese students. 22 This latter study challenges the Western/ Perceived health
Eastern, individualist/collectivist paradigm. This index was a single question asking the participants how they
Still, Hu and colleagues 23 demonstrated that coping behaviors, perceived their own health, offering 4 categorical responses from
both emotional and problem focused, were not specifically associated “Excellent” to “Poor.”
with either individualistic or collectivist cultural orientations. For in-
stance, coping responses that functioned as a buffer for the negative Perceived stress
impact of stress within a Chinese population exacerbated it within a The PSS index has been used in past research and assesses the de-
New Zealand population.24 gree of stress in one's life.27 It is composed of 10 questions, using a 5-
Because of the prevalence of both stress and poor sleep quality point Likert scale. Our results yielded a reliability coefficient of .821,
among US college populations, we explored the extent that culture comparing favorably with other studies, 28–30 ranging from .848 to
plays in students' sleep quality by surveying undergraduate popula- .870. Two of these studies were done in foreign populations. 29,30
tions from countries on 2 different continents, Bolivia and the United
States. Bolivia and the United States can be considered Western coun- Poor sleep quality
tries based on the historical European influence common to both coun- This index was assessed with 4 questions closely following Na-
tries. A few comparative countrywide statistics may indirectly relate to tional Sleep Foundation (NSF) criteria of sleep quality as well as
stress and sleep. For example, life expectancy in Bolivia at birth is 69.5 related to 3 of 7 sleep quality domains captured by The Pittsburgh
years, whereas it is 80.0 years in the United States. The fertility rate in Sleep Quality Index: Sleep Latency, Sleep Disturbances, and Day-
Bolivia is 2.6 children per woman, whereas in the United States, it is 1.9 time Dysfunction 30,31 We refrained from investigating other
children per woman25 (2017 estimates). Yet their economies may be sleep quality domains such as “Use of sleeping medications” and
the most influential engines that drive societal and health issues. The “Subjective sleep quality” included in the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
purchasing power (GDP) per capita in Bolivia is $7500 with a GDP Index to include a sleep hygiene measure described below. Two of
growth rate of 4.2%. In the United States, these figures are $59,500 the questions were: “On an average night, how long does it usually
and 2.2%, respectively.25 Each year, Columbia University ranks coun- take you to fall asleep after you close your eyes?” and “On an aver-
tries based on an index that includes GDP per capita, social support, age night, how many times do you wake up after falling asleep?”
life expectancy, and perceptions about freedom, generosity, and cor- To assess frequency of sleep disturbances, we asked how often
ruption. These 6 factors purportedly express societal happiness. In the participant has trouble falling asleep on a weekly basis. Lastly,
2017, Bolivia ranked 58th in the world and the United States ranked daytime function was assessed by asking how often the partici-
14th on this index.26 At first glance, Bolivian citizens appear to be at pant feels tired or sleepy during the day. 32 A higher score indi-
a disadvantage in well-being or happiness compared to US citizens. cated poor sleep quality. The index demonstrated a reliability
Would this correlate with health- or sleep-related outcomes? coefficient of .575.
Specifically, we searched for predictors of poor sleep quality and
sought to identify differences by country. We aspired to capture cul- Self-efficacy
tural optimism or happiness, calling this construct “worldview”. Our Self-efficacy is a measure of confidence and is championed in
hypothesis is as follows: Perceived stress and worldview will influ- many health models. Participants' self-efficacy was rated based on a
ence sleep quality. From this hypothesis, we predict that: question asking whether they could reduce the stressors in their
life. The response was captured on a 5-point Likert-scale.
• Perceived stress will positively associate with poor sleep quality.
• The group with higher perceived stress will show poorer sleep
Sleep deprivation
quality.
Sleep deprivation was computed by subtracting the number of
• Worldview will negatively associate with poor sleep quality.
hours of sleep a participant reported as indispensable to function
The results of this investigation led us to suggest ideas in improv- properly from the number of self-reported hours he/she gets in an
ing students' sleep quality. average night's sleep. The resulting number is either positive
J. Doolin et al. / Sleep Health 4 (2018) 565–571 567

(indicating a self-reported surplus in sleep) or negative (indicating were female (78.7%; n = 111). In the US sample, 75.9% (n = 60)
that the participant perceives he/she is sleep deprived). were female students and 85.0% (n = 51) were female students in
the Bolivian sample. A significant age difference existed between US
Sleep hygiene and Bolivian students (t = −2.827, df = 135, P = .005), with
This index was created by using 3 multiple-choice questions that Bolivians older by 2½ years (23.9 vs 26.3) on average.
inquired about sleep-promoting routines of the participants. 32 The average amount of time that all the participants had spent in
Choices ranged from “never” to “always” concerning whether the college was 4.6 years (3.9 years US students vs 5.4 years Bolivian stu-
participant (1) goes to bed at the same time every night, (2) keeps dents) (t = −6.018, df = 137, P = .000). Fifty-six participants
the same sleep schedule throughout the week and weekend, and (39.7%) reported to have moved to attend college. Of the US sample,
(3) worries about not getting enough sleep. Two of the questions spe- 37 (46.8%) reported to have moved to attend college, whereas 19
cifically reflect NSF criteria, 33 whereas all 3 also mirror questions (31.7%) Bolivians moved to college, a marginally significant differ-
from the Sleep Hygiene Index (SHI), a reliable sleep hygiene index.34 ence (t = −1.831, df = 137, P = .069; Table 1). (See Fig. 1.)
We chose these 3 questions in lieu of the full SHI both for brevity and Forty-five students reported that they were either married or liv-
to avoid cultural bias. Evaluation of the SHI indicates that it measures ing with a partner (31.9%), and 93 reported to be single (66.0%).
2 factors: sleep-disturbing behavior and environment, and irregular Within the US sample, 26.6% (n = 21) were living with a partner;
sleep wake schedule.35 As such, we excluded behavior and environ- 40.7% (n = 24) of Bolivians reported to be living with a partner. In
ment questions, limiting the cultural assumptions implicit in sleep addition, 26 (18.6%) students within the total sample reported to be
hygienic behaviors and environments. High scores indicated good taking care of another family member, whereas 110 (78.6%) were
sleep hygiene. not. Among the US students, 10 (12.5%) reported to be family
member caregivers, and among the Bolivians, 16 (26.7%) were care-
Worldview givers. This was a significantly different proportion (χ 2 = 5.086; df
Responses from nine original questions using a 5-point Likert = 1, P = .024).
scale were summed to create this index, whereby participants indi-
cated their optimism or pessimism in areas of daily living, whether
they expected things to go their way, or if people around them Health and stress indices
were supportive or selfish and greedy. It was tested for theme consis-
tency using oblique rotation factor analysis, loaded on 2 domains. The health behavior index exhibited a mean of 6.6 (SD = 1.06;
Four items were discarded through factor analysis, leaving 5 vali- range 4-8). The United States, students demonstrated significantly
dated items. One domain expressed positive (or negative) expecta- better health behavior engagement than Bolivian students (6.8 vs
tions of one's future in general, and the second expressed optimistic 6.4; t = −2.331, df = 137, P = .021). We extracted physical activity
or pessimistic beliefs about others or society. Examples of the items from the index to test it separately; physical activity did not reveal
included “I hardly ever expect things to go my way” and “Most people a difference by site, nor did it impact sleep. Not surprisingly, US and
are kind and good and don't want to intentionally hurt others.” The Bolivian students perceived their health equally (2.4 vs 2.3, respec-
worldview index yielded a reliability coefficient of .547. tively; t = −0.609, df = 138, P = .543). The perceived stress index
exhibited a mean of 30.0 overall (SD = 6.21; range 12-48). Bolivian
Procedure students reported significantly more stress than their US counter-
parts (31.3 vs 30.5; t = −2.13, df = 135, P = .035; Table 2). Sex man-
Participants were offered the opportunity to complete the paper/ ifested marginally different stress levels, with female students
pencil questionnaire by designated instructors (instructors demonstrating more stress than men (t = −1.908, df = 134, P =
volunteered their classes ahead of time). Classes were convenience .059). This was not a significant difference.
selected. The participants had to be currently enrolled undergraduate
students in a psychology program. There was no variation in how the
questionnaire was administered between classes or participants. All Table 1
subjects gave informed consent; it took participants approximately Demographics of participants, by country, n = 140
20-25 minutes to complete. Total US Bolivian P
Data analysis included descriptive analysis and bivariate analysis n (%/ave) students students value
between poor sleep quality and variables of interest either through n (%/ave) n (%/ave)
analysis of variance or regression analysis. Lastly, multiple regression Age (average, y) 137 (24.9) 79 (23.8) 58 (26.3) .005⁎⁎
analysis was performed with the significant variables from the bivar- Sex .188
iate component. Significance was set at .05. A sample size of 140 Male 28 (20.1) 19 (24.1) 9 (15.0)
Female 111 (79.9) 60 (75.9) 51 (85.0)
allowed for a margin of error of 8.5% (1/√N), sufficient for exploratory
Civil status .231
research. Married 20 (14.5) 9 (11.4) 11 (18.6)
Permission to conduct this study was approved by the Institu- Living w/ partner 25 (18.1) 12 (15.2) 13 (22.0)
tional Review Board through The University of Texas at Tyler and Single 93 (67.4) 58 (73.4) 35 (59.3)
was approved by the Department of Psychology at the Universidad Time in university 139 (4.6) 79 (3.9) 60 (5.4) .000⁎⁎
(average, y)
Autónoma Gabriel Rene Moreno. Data analysis was completed on Work (h/wk)a,b 129 (16.8) 79 (17.3) 50 (16.0) .703
IBM SPSS (version 20). Caregiver .024⁎⁎
Yes 26 (19.1) 10 (12.7) 16 (28.1)
Results No 110 (80.9) 69 (87.3) 41 (71.9)
Moved to attend university .071⁎
Yes 56 (40.3) 37 (46.8) 19 (31.7)
Demographics No 83 (59.7) 42 (53.2) 41 (68.3)
⁎ .05 b P b .10.
Eighty students (57.1%) were US citizens and 60 students (42.9%) ⁎⁎ P b .05.
were Bolivian. The 3 most prominent ethnicities were Hispanic/La- a
Independent-samples t test.
tino 43.5%, White 39.1%, and Black 8.7%. The majority of participants b
χ2 test.
568 J. Doolin et al. / Sleep Health 4 (2018) 565–571

Fig. 1. Worldview on poor sleep quality, by country.

Sleep indices Worldview

The sleep hygiene index ranged in scores from 3 to 10 and pro- The worldview index ranged from 7 to 23, with an average of 16.8
duced a mean of 7.5 (SD = 1.52). In the US sample, the average for all participants. There was no difference between US students
score was 7.7; the Bolivian sample attained 7.3. This difference was (mean = 16.7) and Bolivian students (mean = 16.9) (t = −.459,
not statistically different (t = −1.267, df = 138, P = .207). The df = 137, P = .647). However, worldview demonstrated a significant
poor sleep quality index ranged from 4 to 19 (mean = 9.5; SD = association with poor sleep quality (Table 3) such that the more pos-
3.57). The US sample averaged 10.4, and Bolivians averaged 8.4; US itive world outlook one had, so too one had better sleep quality. Our
students had significantly poorer sleep quality than their Bolivian third prediction stated that worldview negatively associates with
counterparts (t = −3.560, df = 137, P = .001). In Table 3, stress poor sleep quality; this was not false even after controlling for coun-
shows a significant relationship with sleep quality. Our first predic- try, stress, and other sleep indices (Table 4).
tion stated that as perceived stress increases, sleep quality degrades;
our first prediction was not false. The sleep deprivation index for all Other measures
participants demonstrated a mean of −1.3 (range −7 to 5; SD =
1.74). US students showed an average of −1.1, whereas Bolivians av- Self-efficacy demonstrated a mean of 3.9 overall (SD = .91; range
eraged −1.5; this was not a significant difference (t = −3.560, df = 1-5). US students showed significantly higher self-efficacy compared
136, P = .201). Of note, the average hours actually slept between to their Bolivian counterparts (4.0 vs 3.6, respectively; t = −2.587,
the 2 groups were nearly identical (US students 6.44 hours vs df = 138, P = .011). Comfort-being-alone scores ranged from 1 to 5,
Bolivian students 6.37 hours), but when asked “how many hours of with a mean of 3.4 for all participants (SD = 1.19). The US sample av-
sleep do you need in order to function well,” the US students reported eraged 3.6 and the Bolivians averaged 3.2, another significant differ-
an average of 7.6 hours and the Bolivian students 9.1 hours, still not a ence (t = −2.028, df = 138, P = .045). US students were more
significant difference (t = −1.261, df = 138, P = .212). comfortable with solitude.

Table 2
Selected indices, differences by country⁎

Average Actual range Possible range US students (average) Bolivian students (average) P value

Perceived stress 30.0 12-48 10-50 29.0 31.3 .035⁎⁎


Heath behavior 6.6 4-8 3-8 6.8 6.5 .021⁎⁎
Perceived health 2.4 1-4 1-4 2.4 2.3 .854
Sleep quality (poor) 9.5 4-19 3-19 10.4 8.4 .001⁎⁎
Sleep hygiene 7.5 3-10 3-12 7.7 7.3 .207
Sleep deprivation −1.3 −7 to −5 – −1.1 −1.5 .201
Self-efficacy 3.9 1-5 1-5 4.0 3.6 .011⁎⁎
Comfort being alone 3.4 1-5 1-5 3.6 3.2 .045⁎⁎
Worldview 16.8 7-23 5-25 16.7 16.9 .647
⁎ .05 b P b .10.
⁎⁎ P b .05.
J. Doolin et al. / Sleep Health 4 (2018) 565–571 569

Table 3 predictors of poor sleep quality were perceived stress, sleep hygiene,
Bivariate analysis on poor sleep quality sleep deficiency, and worldview. Among these predictors, only stress
Fa Bb P value differed significantly by country.
Demographic
Regarding stress, perceived sleep deprivation deserves consider-
Age −0.072 .232 ation. This measure was constructed by subtracting the number of
Sex 0.273 .602 hours reported as necessary for proper day-to-day functioning from
Civil status 0.200 .819 actual hours slept. Bolivian and US students reported sleeping nearly
Years in university −0.043 .812
the same number of hours each night. However, when asked, “how
Caregiver 3.047 .083⁎
Move to university 0.159 .691 many hours of sleep do you need in order to function well,” the US
Site 11.147 .001⁎⁎ students reported an average of 7.6 hours and the Bolivian students
Indexes an average of 9.1 hours. Although this difference was not significant,
Worldview −0.490 .000⁎⁎ we think that this might be an important factor. Although neither
Health behavior −0.096 .740
group gets the minimum number of hours recommended by the
Perceived health 1.078 .361
Self-esteem 0.276 .893 NSF (at least 7 hours), the perceived deprivation by Bolivians is
Sleep deprivation −0.501 .004⁎⁎ about 2 times that of US students (2.3 and 1.2 fewer hours, respec-
Sleep hygiene −0.816 .000⁎⁎ tively). The perception that one is not getting enough sleep, whether
Comfort being alone 0.433 .433
real or not, can result in concern or worry, which may increase stress.
Stress 0.045 .000⁎⁎
If this gap produced worry in Bolivian students, it might account for
⁎ .05 b P b .10.
their increased stress as compared with their US counterparts.
⁎⁎ P b .05.
a
Analysis of variance.
Bolivian students were more likely to report being a caregiver for
b
Regression. a family member, a task that would surely increase stress. However,
this variable was removed through multiple regression iteration.
How did Bolivian students sleep better while being more stressed?
Perhaps Bolivians are more resilient36,37 in moments of stress or en-
Variables that showed significant association to poor sleep quality
gage in better coping skills15–18 (note that some emotion-focused be-
through bivariate analysis (Table 3) were placed into a multiple re-
haviors such as smoking or watching television, when performed
gression model to control for each other. The significant variables
before bed, are in direct contrast to NSF sleep hygiene suggestions
emerging from the model, which were predictors of sleep quality,
to promote sleep quality and may partially account for the negative
were site, perceived stress, sleep hygiene, sleep deficiency, and
relationship between emotion-focused coping approaches and sleep
worldview (Table 4). Our second prediction stated that the site
quality). A third explanation could be that stress, like depression or
with the highest perceived stress indicates poorer sleep quality.
anxiety, may be understood as debilitating across cultures but is
This was shown to be untrue; the site with the highest stress had bet-
expressed differently by different cultures. 38
ter sleep quality.
Worthman 13 asks: why do populations from developed coun-
tries, those who can best control sleep debt responses, suffer so
much insomnia? She suggests that although natural threats have
Discussion
mostly disappeared (and with them cues to safety or insecurity),
humans' nighttime arousal ability has not correspondingly disap-
Overall, we found support for 2 out of 3 predictions based on our
peared. Therefore, our biologically wired expectations for cues
hypothesis. First, perceived stress positively associated with poor
may have become emotionally internalized. This paradigm ad-
sleep quality for both sites, which supports our first prediction and
dresses not only the negative aspects of emotion-focused coping
corroborates with previous work. 5 However, we found that stress
but also the internalization of remote threats that one endures
accounted for only 15% of the variance in poor sleep quality in univer-
from a globalized media. Unfortunately, this idea would not ad-
sity students (data not shown), whereas Lund and colleagues 5 found
dress differences between Bolivian and American undergraduate
that stress accounted for 24% of the variance in sleep quality. Sec-
psychology students if we assume that they consume the same
ondly, worldview was negatively associated with poor sleep quality
quantity of media. Nor can we account for any qualitative differ-
for both sites (ie, those who had a negative worldview also had
ences in media between them. Future researchers may want to ex-
poor sleep quality), supporting our third prediction. A central finding
plore this idea.
of our study is that sleep quality was significantly different between
Could a mental construct such as attitude, self-efficacy, or opti-
the 2 sites, with Bolivian students reporting better sleep quality
mism affect sleep? And if so, to what extent? This study's data sup-
than US students while also reporting greater stress. This runs con-
port the third prediction: worldview is significantly associated with
trary to our second prediction. Other than site, the significant
sleep quality. Worldview affected sleep quality no matter which cul-
ture was involved, while also controlling for perceived stress. World-
view did not differ significantly between the sites. Higher worldview
Table 4 scores indicate trust in one's society; with more optimistic social atti-
Multiple regression analysis on poor sleep quality⁎
tudes, one has less stress and has better sleep quality. Other studies
P value B SE T P value support the effects of positive world outlook on decreasing stress
(unadjusted) (adjusted)a and good sleep quality. 39,40 Optimism is a cognitive mood that
Site .000⁎⁎ −2.590 0.531 −4.882 .000⁎⁎ reflects peoples' generalizations of favorable future expectancies. 41
Caregiver .767 – – – – It is associated with several positive health and social outcomes,
Stress .005⁎⁎ 0.141 0.049 2.894 .004⁎⁎
some of which are a sense of well-being in times of difficulty (ie,
Sleep hygiene .013⁎⁎ −0.463 0.183 −2.526 .013⁎⁎
Sleep deprivation .022⁎⁎ −0.342 0.148 −2.314 .022⁎⁎ experiencing civil strife,32 during treatment for a chronic illness, 41,42
Worldview .003⁎⁎ −0.304 0.100 −3.045 .003⁎⁎ or persistence in educational efforts 43,44). Carver and colleagues 41
⁎ .05 b P b .10. hypothesized that an association between optimism and physical
⁎⁎ P b .05. health may be explained by the ability to deflect stress, thus avoiding
a
After 2 iterations. physiological wear and tear on one's body. We claim that optimism is
570 J. Doolin et al. / Sleep Health 4 (2018) 565–571

similar to worldview, which, in our study, protects against both stress 8. Airhihenbuwa C, Iwelunmor J, Ezepue C, Williams N, Jean-Louis G. I sleep, because
we sleep: a synthesis on the role of culture in sleep behavior research. Sleep Med.
and poor sleep quality. 2016:1867–1873. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.07.020.
According to the earlier cited happiness index, 26 Bolivian citizens 9. Worthman CM, Melby MK. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of
were decidedly less happy than US citizens, with ranks of 58 and 14, human sleep. In: Carskadon MA, editor. Adolescent Sleep Patterns: Biological, So-
cial, and Psychological Influences. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2002.
respectively. This could explain our data showing relatively more p. 69–117.
stress among Bolivian students relative to the US group. That is, stress 10. Worthman CM, Brown RA. Companionable sleep: social regulation of sleep and
could be a mediating factor between happiness and sleep quality. cosleeping in Egyptian families. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(1):124–135. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.124.
However, to test that, one would have to look specifically at happi-
11. Musharbash Y. Night, sight, and feeling safe: an exploration of aspects of Warlpiri
ness scores among college students. and western sleep. Aust J Anthropol. 2013;24(1):48–63. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Some limitations of this study exist. First is the nonrandom selec- taja.12021.
12. Samsun DR, Nunn CL. Sleep intensity and the evolution of human cognition. Evol
tion of participants, exposing the study to bias. Also, the self-reported
Anthropol. 2015;24:225–237.
items in the survey tool incur threats of validity by recall bias. In ad- 13. Worthman CM. After Dark: The Evolutionary Ecology of Human Sleep. In:
dition, our results are not easily generalizable to people globally. On Trevathan WR, Smith EO, McKenna JJ, editors. Evol Med Health. Oxford University
the other hand, our participants are well-educated individuals who Press; 2008. p. 291–313.
14. Samson DR, Crittenden AN, Mabulla IA, Mabulla AZP, Nunn CL. Hadza sleep biol-
study cognitive assessments in their discipline; their skills in survey ogy: evidence for flexible sleep-wake patterns in hunter-gatherers. Am J Phys
inspection would incline their responses toward objectivity and cut Anthropol. 2017;162:573–582.
down potential bias. Our sample size was moderate. Despite limita- 15. Folkman S, Lazarus RS. An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample.
J Health Soc Behav. 1980;21(3):219–239.
tions, validity was demonstrated by consistency in results from 16. Billings AG, Moos RH. The role of coping responses and social resources in atten-
other studies. uating the impact of stressful life events. J Behav Med. 1981;4:139–157.
We found that stress and worldview predict poor sleep quality in 17. Pearlin LI, Schooler C. The structure of coping. J Health Soc Behav. 1978;19:2–21.
18. Sigmon ST, Stanton AL, Snyder CR. Gender differences in coping: a further test of
both cultures. But we are challenged to find clear cultural signals be- socialization and role constraint theories. Sex Roles. 1995;33:565–587.
hind a mechanism to explain this, as worldview is similar in both cul- 19. Rotenberg VS, Arshavsky VV. REM sleep, stress and search activity: a short critical
tures. The figure suggests that one may miss detection of culturally review and a new conception. Waking Sleeping. 1979;3(3):235–244.
20. Abe Y, Mishima K, Kaneita Y, Li L, Ohida T, Nishikawa T, Uchiyama M. Stress coping
sensitive predictors of sleep quality in educated populations but behaviors and sleep hygiene practices in a sample of Japanese adults with insom-
may pinpoint them with lower worldview scores, on average. That nia. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2011;9(1):35–45.
is, worldview may surface as a cultural measure in less educated 21. O'Connor DB, Shimizu M. Sense of personal control, stress and coping style: a
cross-cultural study. Stress Health. 2002;18:173–183.
populations.
22. Kang YS, Choi SY, Ryu E. The effectiveness of a stress coping program based on
Our research indicates that, for both cultures, sleep hygiene prac- mindfulness meditation on the stress, anxiety, and depression experienced by
tices are positively related to sleep quality. This implies that interven- nursing students in Korea. Nurse Educ Today. 2009;29:538–543.
tions aimed at improving sleep hygiene might yield favorable results 23. Hu Q, Bernardo A, Lam S, Cheang P. Individualism-collectivism orientations and
coping styles of cyberbullying victims in chinese culture. Curr Psychol. 2016 [serial
in sleep quality. Among the sleep hygiene best practices suggested by online, available from: PsycINFO, Ipswich, MA Accessed April 17, 2018].
the NSF is stress reduction before bed. 33 This may be accomplished 24. Szabo A, English A, Zhijia Z, Jose P, Ward C, Jianhong M. Is the utility of secondary
through meditation or mindfulness techniques. 45,46 Another ap- coping a function of ethnicity or the context of reception? A longitudinal study
across Western and Eastern cultures. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2017;48(8):1230–1246.
proach to improve sleep quality is to address worldview. Worldview 25. Central Intelligence Agency (CIA). https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-
may be modifiable through therapeutic methods such as cognitive world-factbook/, Accessed date: 15 April 2018. [Published 2018].
behavioral therapies (CBTs). CBT challenges and reshapes basic as- 26. Helliwell J, Layard R, Sachs J. World Happiness Report. New York: Sustainable De-
velopment Solutions Network; 2017.
sumptions about the self, others, and the perceived world that result 27. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health
from cognitive distortions. CBT may produce 2-fold results: More Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–396.
positive social beliefs may emerge, and it may reduce stress. Either re- 28. Smith KJ, Rosenberg DL, Timothy HG. An assessment of the psychometric proper-
ties of the Perceived Stress Scale-10 (PSS10) with business and accounting stu-
sult shows promise in increasing sleep quality.
dents. Account Perspect. 2014;13(1):29–59. https://doi.org/10.1111/1911-3838.
12023.
Disclosure 29. Reis RS, Hino AAF, Añez CRR. Perceived Stress Scale reliability and validity study in
Brazil. J Health Psychol. 2010;15(1):107–114.
30. Örücü MÇ, Demir A. Psychometric evaluation of perceived stress scale for Turkish
Mr Doolin has nothing to disclose. university students. Stress Health. 2009;25(1):103–109.
Dr Vilches has nothing to disclose. 31. Ohayon M, Wickwire EM, Hirshkowitz M, Albert SM, Avidan A, Daly FJ, et al. Na-
tional Sleep Foundation's sleep quality recommendations: first report. Sleep
Dr Cooper has nothing to disclose. Health. 2017;3:6–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2016.11.006.
Dr Gipson has nothing to disclose. 32. Gipson C. The effects of a sleep hygiene text-message intervention on sleep in col-
Dr Sorensen has nothing to disclose. lege students. Tyler, Texas: University of Texas at Tyler; 2016http://hdl.handle.
net/10950/361, Accessed date: 10 August 2016 [dissertation].
33. National Sleep Foundation. Sleep Hygiene. https://sleepfoundation.org/sleep-
References topics/sleep-hygiene; 2017, Accessed date: 22 November 2017.
34. Mastin D, Bryson J, Corwyn R. Assessment of sleep hygiene using the sleep hy-
1. Benham G. Sleep: an important factor in stress-health models. Stress Health. 2010; giene index. J Behav Med. 2006;29(3):223–227.
26:204–214. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.1304. 35. Cho S, Kim G, Lee J. Psychometric evaluation of the sleep hygiene index: a sample
2. Grandner M. Sleep, health, and society. Sleep Med Clin. 2017;12(1):1–22 [serial on- of patients with chronic pain. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2013;11(1):213. https://
line, Available from: PsycINFO, Ipswich, MA. Accessed November 30, 2017]. doi.org/10.1186/1477-7525-11-213.
3. Barnes C, Drake C. Prioritizing sleep health: Public health policy recommenda- 36. Ong AD, Bergeman CS, Bisconti TL, Wallace KA. Psychological resilience, positive
tions. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(6):733–737 [serial online, Available from: emotions, and successful adaptation to stress in later life. J Pers Soc Psychol.
PsycINFO, Ipswich, MA. Accessed November 30, 2017]. 2006;91(4):730–749. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.730.
4. Irwin M, Opp M. Sleep health: reciprocal regulation of sleep and innate immunity. 37. Dumont M, Provost MA. Resilience in adolescents: protective role of social sup-
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42(1):129–155 [serial online, Available from: port, coping strategies, self-esteem, and social activities on experience of stress
PsycINFO, Ipswich, MA. Accessed November 30, 2017]. and depression. J Youth Adolesc. 1999;28:343. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
5. Lund HG, Reider BD, Whiting AB, Prichard JR. Sleep patterns and predictors of dis- 1021637011732.
turbed sleep in a large population of college students. J Adolesc Health. 2010;46(2): 38. Love S. Do cultural differences change what depression feels like? Atlantic. https://
124–132. theatlantic.com/health/archive/2017/10/cultural-differences-depression/541893/,
6. Schneider D. Stress and sleep [abstract]. Schweiz Arch Neurol Neurochir Psychiatr. Accessed date: 23 November 2017. [Published October 4, 2017].
1977:47–54 [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/203025. Accessed January 39. Lau EY, Hui CH, Lam J, Cheung S. Sleep and optimism: a longitudinal study of bidi-
23, 2017]. rectional causal relationship and its mediating and moderating variables in a Chi-
7. Buboltz WJ, Brown F, Soper B. Sleep habits and patterns of college students: a pre- nese student sample. Chronobiol Int. 2017;34(3):360–372. https://doi.org/10.
liminary study. J Am Coll Health. 2001;50(3):131–135. 1080/07420528.2016.1276071.
J. Doolin et al. / Sleep Health 4 (2018) 565–571 571

40. Lemola S, Räikkönen K, Gomez V, Allemand M. Optimism and self-esteem are re- Glossary
lated to sleep. results from a large community-based sample. Int J Behav Med.
2012;20(4):567–571. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-012-9272-z. Culture: Shared practices, norms, and values demarcated by a group of people that re-
41. Carver CS, Scheier MF, Segerstrom SC. Optimism. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:879–889. flect a common set of beliefs and attitudes.47
42. Friedman LC, Nelson DV, Baer PE, Lane M, Smith FE, Dworkin RJ. The relationship Insomnia: The presence of 1 of the 3 following sleeping disturbances: (1) difficulty ini-
of dispositional optimism, daily life stress, and domestic environment to coping tiating sleep, (2) difficulty maintaining sleep characterized by frequent awakenings or
methods used by cancer patients. J Behav Med. 1992;15:127. https://doi.org/10. problems returning to sleep after awakening, or (3) early-morning awakening with in-
1007/BF00848321. ability to return to sleep.48
43. Solberg Nes L, Evans DR, Segerstrom SC. Optimism and college retention: meditation Sleep disturbance: Interruption in sleep which produces distress or impairment in
by motivation, performance, and adjustment. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2009;39:1887–1912. social, occupational, academic, behavioral, or other important areas of functioning.
44. Segerstrom SC. Optimism and resources: effects on each other and on health over It must occur at least 3 nights per week, be present for at least 3 consecutive
10 years. J Res Pers. 2007;41:772–786. months, occur despite adequate opportunity for sleep, not be associated with
45. Carmody J, Baer RA. Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of substance abuse or medications, and not be better explained by another sleep-
mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindful- wake disorder.48
ness-based stress reduction program. J Behav Med. 2008;31(1):23–33. Sleep hygiene: An array of habits that are necessary to have good nighttime sleep
46. Shapiro SL, Oman D, Thoresen CE, Plante TG, Linders T. Cultivating mindfulness: quality. 33 Sleep hygiene recommendations include avoiding stimulants before
effects on well-being. J Clin Psychol. 2008;64(7):840–862. bed, limiting daytime naps to 30 minutes, getting adequate exercise, establishing
47. Iwelunmor J, Newsome V, Airhihenbuwa CO. Framing the impact of culture on a regular relaxing bedtime routine, making sure that the sleep environment is
health: a systematic review of the PEN-3 cultural model and its application in pleasant, avoiding foods that can be disruptive before sleep, and ensuring that
public health research and interventions. Ethn Health. 2014;19(1):20–46. lighting is not intrusive.33
https://doi.org/10.1080/13557858.2013.857768. Sleep quality: The NSF lists 4 key determinants of this concept: (1) sleeping at least
48. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Washington, 85% of the time while in bed, (2) falling asleep within 30 minutes of laying down, (3)
Londres: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. waking up no more than once a night, and (4) being awake for 20 minutes or less after
49. McKenzie JF, Pinger PR. An Introduction to Community & Public Health. 8th ed. initially falling asleep.31
Burlington MA: Jones & Bartlett; 2015. Stress: One's response to perceived tension and strain.49
50. Koltko-Rivera ME. The psychology of worldviews. Rev Gen Psychol. 2004;8(1): Worldview: Defined as a set of beliefs about the state of the world, other individuals,
3–58. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.1.3. and their relation to the self.50 This is likened to optimism.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai