Anda di halaman 1dari 19

Change Process Research

in Psychotherapy 30
Jeanne C. Watson and Evelyn J. McMullen

analysis is an attempt to find meaning and order


30.1 Change Process Research: in a complex amount of data so that it can be
A Qualitative Approach more easily comprehensible and contribute to
our understanding of the process of psychother-
Change process research dates back to the 1950s apy as well as the roles of the different partici-
when Carl Rogers and his colleagues at the pants and their impact on each other.
University of Chicago began to study the pro- Greenberg and Pinsof (1986) observed in their
cess of change as they listened to audio record- classic text on Process Research, that the word
ings of clients’ sessions. The advent of the new “process” encompasses a range of meanings.
technology had made it possible to review what They noted that: “Dictionary definitions empha-
actually transpired between clients and thera- size three related aspects of process: activity over
pists in order to observe and understand the dif- time, directional change, and movement towards
ferent activities in which they engaged, and completion” (Greenberg and Pinsof 1986, p. 3).
begin to isolate more effective ways of interven- Heppner et al. (1992) captured some of the com-
ing from less effective ones. Change process plexity of process research in their definition, and
research is rooted in a description of processes described it as:
that are central to psychotherapy. As such they […] attempts to characterize what changes occur
draw on the investigators’ understanding of the during counseling. Thus, process research may
phenomenon under study as well as his or her attempt to (1) describe the client, counselor, group,
attempt to make sense of the data that is being family, or interactions, (2) specific change in the
behavior or actions of the client, counselor, group,
examined. Qualitative research is an empirical or family over time, or (3) link one or more of these
undertaking that seeks to illuminate “the how or process variables to client outcome.” (p. 320)
what (i.e., a process) versus why (i.e., etiology of
outcome) aspects of a phenomena” (Hays and The goal of process researchers is to better
Singh 2011, p. 4). This type of process research understand and identify the active ingredients of
different therapeutic approaches so as to develop
more effective treatments. Research on the pro-
J.C. Watson, Ph.D. (*) • E.J. McMullen, Ph.D. cess of change and the identification and devel-
Applied Psychology & Human Development, opment of process outcome links can inform the
OISE, University of Toronto, 252 Bloor St. West,
Room 5-190, Toronto, ON, Canada, M5W 1V6
development of theory as well as test it in order to
e-mail: Jeanne.watson@utoronto.ca; evelyn. improve the effectiveness of treatments
mcmullen@utoronto.ca (Greenberg and Pinsof 1986; Greenberg and

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016 507


K. Olson et al. (eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Health Research for Evidence-Based Practice,
Handbooks in Health, Work, and Disability 4, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2920-7_30
508 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

Watson 2006a; Hill and Lambert 2004; Lambert A number of problems have been identified
and Hill 1994; Orlinsky et al. 1994). Process with outcome studies using quantitative method-
research highlights differences between good and ologies. One problem is that psychotherapy
poor outcome clients leading to more responsive approaches that have not been tested or received
treatment interventions that are more broadly much empirical attention are deemed ineffective
applicable to a range of client groups. in the absence of information. Another problem
Two main streams of inquiry inform process is that of the researcher allegiance effect.
research, one is the study of clients’ and thera- Research has shown that studies that have been
pists’ subjective or covert experience and the sec- conducted by therapists and/or researchers with
ond is the study of clients’ and therapists’ an allegiance to the specific approach being
observable or overt experience and behavior dur- investigated show a higher likelihood that their
ing the change process. Both of these streams use preferred approach will be supported as more
a qualitative approach to study phenomena in effective than the alternative or control treatment
order to describe and understand them and to find (Elliott and Freire 2008; Luborsky et al. 1975).
meaning in the data. By incorporating the study Moreover, it has been suggested that control
of therapists and clients overt behavior in psy- treatments may be negatively impacted if non-
chotherapy we are broadening the usual defini- experts or therapists without adequate training in
tion of qualitative inquiry to include an analysis the approach deliver the therapy (Hollon 1999;
of texts as well as, “[…] the study of a phenom- Lambert 1999; Luborsky et al. 1975, 1999).
enon or research topic in context” as proposed by Treatments that are delivered by therapists with-
Hays and Singh (2011 p. 4). Corbin and Strauss out sufficient training cannot be considered bona
(2009) propose a similar view of qualitative anal- fide or authentic. Therapist allegiance effects call
ysis defining it as: “A process of examining and into question the findings that one treatment is
interpreting data in order to elicit meaning, gain more effective than another.
understanding, and develop empirical knowl- Another problem with RCTs is that the find-
edge” (p. 1). This is one of the primary objectives ings are molar, and thus, the active ingredients of
of process research. how treatments work are not clearly visible. In an
attempt to address this question researchers have
investigated whether the common, or so-called
30.2 Quantitative Versus relational, aspects or the more specific aspects of
Qualitative Approaches treatment (e.g., interventions like thought
to Change records, homework, two-chair work, or transfer-
ence interpretations) lead to change (Beutler
Psychotherapy research consists of two distinct 1995; Greenberg and Rice 1981; Grencavage and
strands to investigate what works: outcome Norcross 1990; Messer and Wampold 2002).
research and change process research. Outcome While this debate is important and has contrib-
research relies on conventional quantitative uted valuable findings to the field, psychotherapy
methods to evaluate outcome, whereas change research continues to identify processes that are
process research is conducted using a variety of generally effective but has not yet adequately
qualitative approaches to study change. Outcome addressed questions raised more that 40 years
research is focused on predicting outcomes and ago related to which treatment for which client,
identifying which type of psychotherapy is most and when (Beutler et al. 2012; Critchfield 2012;
effective and is less concerned with describing Lambert and Barley 2001; Norcross and
processes and procedures. The question of which Wampold 2011; Smith and Grawe 2005). These
psychotherapy is more effective is often explored questions remain pressing, especially if we con-
using Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT), a sider the number of people who do not respond to
method used in pharmacotherapy to test the treatments (Edlund et al. 2002; Mash and Hunsley
effectiveness of different drug treatments. 1993; Wang 2007; Wierzbicki and Pekarik 1993).
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 509

If psychotherapies are to be optimally effective, it data was analyzed using grounded theory meth-
would be helpful to be able to differentiate among odology (Glaser and Strauss 1967). His analyses
client populations as well as treatments methods highlighted the role of clients’ deference (Rennie
to develop ways of working that could be tailored 1994) and clients’ reflexivity (Rennie 2001) in
for specific clients. the change process. Rennie’s analyses (1994)
revealed a general tendency in clients to be defer-
ential to the therapist, which he suggested was
30.3 The Study of Clients’ one way in which the clients protected and fos-
Subjective Experience tered the therapeutic alliance. Some of the rea-
sons clients shared for being deferential were
A number of different methods have been used to being concerned about their therapist’s approach,
study clients’ and therapists’ inner subjective being fearful of criticizing their therapist, want-
experiences of participating in psychotherapy. ing to understand the therapist’s frame of refer-
Qualitative approaches to the study of the change ence, and wishing to meet their perceived
process in psychotherapy use interviews with the expectations of the therapist (Rennie 1994).
participants to gain access to and develop an Findings from his inquiry also showed that cli-
understanding of clients’ inner, subjective experi- ents’ responses to therapists’ operations depended
ence. These interviews can be conducted at any on what they desired and what the felt they could
point in time either immediately after a session, safely disclose, and were very much tied to how
perhaps with the assistance of a video-recording comfortable they felt about approaching inner
of the session to assist with recall, at the end of experiences (Rennie 2001).
therapy or some time thereafter. The information Rennie’s findings underscore the importance
gleaned from these interviews is then subject to of forging an alliance with clients about the goals
qualitative analysis to try to distill common and tasks of psychotherapy. However, Rennie
themes, categories, and views of the experience (1994) observed that the strength of clients’ def-
to inform psychotherapy process research and erence suggests that it is up to the therapist to
illuminate the subjective experience of the par- take the initiative. Therapists and clients need to
ticipants in the psychotherapy encounter. negotiate how they will work together drawing
Examples of this type of work include: on the clients’ goals and current resources and
grounded theory studies conducted by Rennie the therapists’ techniques and expertise.
(1994, 2001), Watson (Watson and Rennie 1994), Metacommunication was suggested as a way to
Bolger (Bolger 1999; Greenberg and Bolger negotiate the therapeutic relationship and the
2001), and Timulák and Lietaer (2001); theme goals and tasks of therapy (Rennie 1998). Rennie
analyses studies conducted by Goldman (1997) suggested that clients need to understand the
and Kagan (2003); content analysis employed by therapists’ perspective of where the therapy is
Bachelor and colleagues (Bachelor 1995; directed as much as therapists need to understand
Bachelor et al. 2007), as well as Lietaer and col- whether or not clients are in agreement with the
leagues (Lietaer 1992; Lietaer and Neirinck process and whether they feel that the therapist
1986; Vanaerschot and Lietaer 2007, 2010). understands their goals. If these are made explicit
In his studies of the clients’ subjective experi- then participants are more likely to establish
ence during psychotherapy, Rennie (1994, 2001) overt agreement on the tasks and goals of therapy
highlighted how many aspects of clients’ inner (Weerasekera et al. 2001).
experiences are not revealed or shared with thera- Furthering the inquiry into clients’ responses
pists. Using the method of Interpersonal Process to specific therapeutic operations or interven-
Recall (IPR; Kagan 1975), Rennie (1994, 2001) tions, Watson and Rennie (1994) studied the sub-
conducted post-therapy interviews with clients to jective experience of eight clients who
better understand their subjective experience of participated in the task of systematic evocative
participating in psychotherapy. The interview unfolding in experiential psychotherapy to
510 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

explore and understand a personal reaction that 1988), misunderstandings in the alliance (Rhodes
they found problematic or perplexing. Using et al. 1994), and clients’ covert processes (Hill
interpersonal process recall (Kagan 1975) and Thompson & Corbett 1993). Hill et al. (1993)
grounded theory (Glaser and Strauss 1967), the conducted a study of clients’ and therapists’
authors developed a model of clients’ cognitive- covert processes in long-term therapy. They
affective processes during the session (Watson found that not only did clients hide their negative
and Rennie 1994). The model of clients’ experi- reactions, thoughts and feelings from their thera-
ence that emerged highlighted the role of the rep- pists, but also that therapists were frequently
resentation of experience in consciousness as an unaware of how clients had been reacting in the
important process that enhances understanding session to what they were saying. This was mod-
and reflection on self and experience to change erated by experience, as more experienced thera-
behavior and ways of being (Watson and Rennie pists in long-term therapy appeared to be
1994). The authors observed that it was clients’ somewhat more adept at recognizing and manag-
curiosity to understand why they acted and felt ing therapeutic work and negative reactions than
the way they did that spurred their engagement in inexperienced therapists (Hill et al. 1992). Their
the task and that, as new information and under- findings also revealed that clients often hid things
standings emerged, they would engage in a pro- from their therapists, about which their therapists
cess of verification to examine and check the were unaware and unable to deduce (Hill et al.
validity of their conceptualizations across a vari- 1993). Reasons clients’ gave for selective sharing
ety of situations (Watson and Rennie 1994). This were feeling too overwhelmed, wanting to avoid,
would often lead to the development of alterna- and fearing being misunderstood (Hill et al.
tive perspectives. As clients’ perspectives 1993). An important goal of meta-communication
changed, they reported experiencing a surge of is to try to foster a therapeutic environment that is
energy and an elevation in their mood that fuelled experienced as non-judgmental, to try to increase
their capacity to imagine solutions and develop clients’ sense of safety and the possibility for
alternative ways of being (Watson and Rennie greater disclosure.
1994). Consistent with Rennie’s earlier work, it Rhodes and colleagues (1994) invited clients
became clear that when clients do not understand to select and describe a major misunderstanding
why therapists are intervening in certain ways event that occurred during therapy. The retro-
they are more reluctant to engage and actively spective reports were then analyzed using a com-
work to regulate the amount and intensity of pain bination of grounded analysis (Rennie et al.
that they experience in a session. The model of 1988) and comprehensive process analysis
the clients’ experience provided a more compre- (Elliott 1989). The reports were categorized as
hensive understanding of systematic evocative resolved when clients felt the issue had been pro-
unfolding as an intervention and suggested ways cessed sufficiently so that they could continue the
that it might be improved so that clients might be work of therapy; and as unresolved when clients
able to engage with the process. The model high- felt the rupture continued to hinder their commu-
lighted alternative paths to change in the resolu- nication with their therapist. Outlining a model
tion of problematic reactions. for the resolved and unresolved pathways,
In addition to using interviews for data collec- Rhodes and colleagues (1994) stated that in
tion, several researchers have administered semi- resolved cases clients tended to have good rela-
structured questionnaires to clients and or tionships with their therapists and were willing to
therapists and then followed up with intensive disclose negative feelings about the misunder-
investigations using various qualitative methods standing. As well, therapists tended to facilitate a
for categorizing meaningful in-session processes mutual repair process maintaining a flexible and
including helpful and non-helpful events (Elliott accepting manner. In the unresolved cases, how-
1985; Elliott et al. 1985; Timulak 2011), clients’ ever, clients tended to have poor relationships
reactions to therapists’ interventions (Hill et al. with their therapist and the therapist was either
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 511

unaware of or unwilling to accept clients’ asser- to resolve difficulties is confronting painful emo-
tion of negative reactions. In cases where misun- tional experiences, then disclosing and talking
derstanding events were not resolved clients about their experiences. Clients sometimes over-
often quit therapy. regulate and avoid painful experiences perpetuat-
Misunderstanding events appeared to occur as ing distress out of fear that the feelings will be too
a result of clients perceiving their therapists as overwhelming or that they may have difficulty
failing to meet their expectations (Rhodes et al. finding the right distance from their emotional
1994). Tinsley et al. (1991) reported that clients experiences in order to be able to explore them
had certain expectations of how they thought effectively (Greenberg and Safran 1987; Leijssen
therapists should behave. In the Rhodes et al. 1990).
(1994 p. 476, 478) study clients reported “some- In a study of pain, Bolger (1999) conducted
thing the therapist did that clients did not like retrospective interviews with adult children of
(e.g., therapist was critical of client choice, thera- alcoholic parent(s) to explore both the phenome-
pist was not paying attention to the client, and non of emotional pain as well as the steps
therapist gave unwanted advice) or something involved in the resolution of painful experiences.
therapists did not do that the client expected or In her qualitative inquiry Bolger (1999) identified
wanted (e.g., therapist missed importance of that clients perceived the essence of pain as a
issue, and therapist failed to remember important feeling of “brokenness” or a “shattering” of the
details).” Also, in both of the resolved and unre- self. Greenberg and Bolger (2001) developed a
solved events clients indicated that they experi- model of the processing of emotional pain. They
enced negative feelings, e.g., anger, frustration, showed that clients’ performances of resolving
resentment, feeling discounted, and feeling dis- painful feelings occurred in stages that included
connected, about their therapists in response to allowing and accepting the feelings, and the
the rupture. reprocessing of emotional experiences within a
Rhodes and colleagues (1994) emphasize that safe therapeutic context. When clients were able
the findings highlight the importance of ensuring to allow and acknowledge their feelings of “bro-
that clients feel safe in therapy and that therapists kenness” they reported experiencing a sense of
should actively encourage clients to assert their release and relief, and adopted a more caring,
feelings and reactions. Their study revealed that self-validating way of being with themselves
when clients did not feel safe they did not share (Greenberg and Bolger 2001).
their dissatisfaction about their therapist’s behav- The work of Rennie, Rhodes, Hill, Watson
ior. The findings suggest that an important part of and Bolger highlights that clients are self-
the change process is to assist clients to bring reflective agents in the therapeutic encounter and
suppressed and unarticulated negative feelings that they as well as their therapists are making
into awareness (Rhodes et al. 1994). Most of the decisions moment-to-moment, consciously and
clients in the resolved misunderstanding cases unconsciously, about what they will and will not
immediately shared their negative feelings with attend to; what they will or will not disclose or
their therapists, whereas in the unresolved cases how they will participate in the process of ther-
clients tended to go underground. The rich data apy at any given moment (Rennie 1994, 2001;
gathered in Rhodes and colleague’s (1994) study Greenberg and Bolger 2001; Watson and Rennie
suggest that “misunderstanding” events are spe- 1994). Thus, therapists need to be attentive to the
cific types of alliance ruptures that can lead to process and inquire into clients’ subjective expe-
drop out and should not be merely viewed as rience and work with them to explicate it in order
weaknesses in the strength of the quality of the to enhance the efficacy of treatment as well as the
alliance. therapeutic alliance.
It is clear that one of the challenges clients To further understand clients’ involvement in
sometimes face in psychotherapy when attempting the development of the therapeutic alliance,
512 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

Bachelor and colleagues (Bachelor 1995; 30.4 The Study of Observable


Bachelor et al. 2007) investigated clients’ or Overt Experience
perceptions of collaboration in psychotherapy. and Behavior
Clients’ phenomenological accounts revealed
that different clients viewed and experienced col- The study of clients’ and therapists’ overt behav-
laboration in various ways (Bachelor 1995; ior in the session and over the course of treatment
Bachelor et al. 2007). Whereas some clients is another area that has been explored to try to
viewed collaboration as a joint effort, other cli- understand the process of change in psychother-
ents viewed themselves or their therapist as the apy. This intensive study of the psychotherapy
primary change agent, playing a significant role process using video and audiotapes as well as
in the process of change (Bachelor et al. 2007). transcripts of psychotherapy sessions allows for
These findings highlight the need for clarification the examination and intensive study of clients’
from the onset of therapy regarding the role and and therapists performances in the session. This
responsibility of participants to avoid possible type of analysis is part of the humanistic client-
misconceptions (Bachelor et al. 2007). The centered tradition that developed with Rogers.
authors reported that irrespective of clients’ per- This method of intensive observation and descrip-
spective of the collaborative process, they consis- tion has led to the development of a number of
tently valued therapists who facilitated emotional “process measures” to describe the type, quality
expression, self-understanding and insight, and and quantity of change. Process measures emerge
who initiated exploration of the clients’ experi- from the intensive study of psychotherapy pro-
ences (Bachelor et al. 2007). cess, their application involves the intensive
Research on events that clients and therapists study of overt behavior in context. They have a
experience as helpful shows that the depth of greater capacity to capture client experience than
experiential self-exploration is central to the would be possible from generating items of clini-
change process in experiential psychotherapies cal and theoretical knowledge alone. Most impor-
and that these processes discriminate between tantly, this work has contributed to the refinement
“very good” and “rather poor” sessions (Dierick of theory as well as the development of therapeu-
and Lietaer 2008; Lietaer and Neirinck 1986; tic interventions and the evolution of client-
Timulak 2007; Vanaerschot and Lietaer 2010). In centered and experiential psychotherapy,
addition, studies by Lietaer and colleagues using including the development of focusing-oriented
content analyses (Lietaer 1992; Vanaerschot psychotherapy and emotion-focused psychother-
and Lietaer 2007, 2010) showed that the thera- apy—the process-experiential approach.
pist stimulating the client to explore more An excellent example of the impact of process
deeply, the client focusing on and exploring his/ research on the development of psychotherapy
her experience more deeply, and the client inten- approaches is provided by the work of Rogers
sively living through his or her experience were and colleagues. Early in the development of
viewed as helpful by therapists and clients. In client-centered psychotherapy, Rogers called for
another investigation of brief person-centered the study of in-session process. Technological
counseling with six clients, Timulák and Lietaer advancements made it possible to record psycho-
(2001) observed that clients’ experiences in therapy sessions so that there would be exact
experiential psychotherapy fall into two records of what occurred in the psychotherapy
domains: strengthening of the therapeutic rela- hour. This enabled Rogers, and his colleague
tionship and clients’ self-empowerment. The Gendlin and their research team at the University
authors found that the therapeutic alliance was of Chicago to examine audio recordings and
strengthened when therapists attended to the observe and identify specific in-session processes
therapeutic relationship, were transparent, and that contributed to change (Rogers 1959b). They
communicated about the goals and tasks in ther- observed that clients who did well in short term
apy (Timulák and Lietaer 2001). psychotherapy engaged in a different process
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 513

than those who did not respond well to treatment. attachment (Karreman and Vingerhoets 2012),
Good outcome clients attended to their inner and psychotherapy research (Hunt 1998; Pascual-
organismic experience and used it as a referent Leone and Greenberg 2007; Pos et al. 2009).
for evaluating their behavior and that of others as Emotional pain is often a catalyst prompting indi-
well as a guide to future actions and ways of viduals to seek psychological help, and the pro-
being. This work led to the development of focus- cessing of emotional pain in psychotherapy has
ing a technique used in client-centered psycho- been a particularly important focus in experien-
therapy and that was later elaborated in the tial therapies (Elliott et al. 2004; Kennedy-Moore
development of focusing-oriented psychotherapy and Watson 1999).
(Rogers 1959a; Gendlin 1969, 1996). The study of in-session change by Rogers and
Focusing describes how clients can become his colleagues started a tradition of change pro-
aware of their inner bodily felt experience, sym- cess research in humanistic and experiential psy-
bolize it in awareness either with images or words chotherapy (Greenberg and Pinsof 1986; Kiesler
and use it to understand the impact of events. 1973; Lambert and Hill 1994). It is change pro-
Once the process was clearly described, it could cess research that has contributed to the ongoing
be taught and shared with clients so that they evolution and development of these approaches.
could engage more productively in psychother- Since Rogers’ and Gendlin’s first studies,
apy (Hinterkopf and Brunswick 1981). research in this area has developed with the goal
Subsequently focusing has been incorporated of understanding and describing clients’ and
into experiential and emotion-focused approaches therapists’ in-session behaviors. Process
and has formed the basis of the work done by the researchers work to identify the little o’s or mini-
focusing organization to become a distinct way outcomes that can be observed during a session
of working with clients (Gendlin 1996). In a and that contribute to change at the end of ses-
review of the literature, Hendricks (2002) sions and over the course of therapy, and they
reported 23 studies supporting that better focus- examine how these build to form more enduring
ing processes positively correlated with success- changes by the end of treatment (Greenberg
ful outcome, and only one study with 1986). As a result of this work, and the numerous
non-significant findings (Loynes 1984). Gendlin process measures developed to identify different
developed the focusing institute in 1972 and types of change, researchers now have a means to
focusing is currently being taught in over 40 link the specific client and therapist processes
countries around the world (The Focusing that are occurring in-session to post session and
Institute 2013). final therapy outcome. Whereas quantitative effi-
It has been suggested that focusing contributes cacy studies provide a way to determine treat-
to the development of clients’ emotional process- ment effectiveness, change process research
ing skills (Hinterkopf and Brunswick 1981). provides a way to link “what” is happening in the
Emotions have been described as brain represen- therapy, and how that relates to good and poor
tations based on internal bodily states (Damasio outcome.
1999) that have an evolutionary adaptive function Rice (1992), a student of Carl Rogers,
(Lang et al. 1997), thus providing people with believed that understanding productive client
essential information as they negotiate interac- process was essential to improving the effective-
tions with their environments. The importance of ness of psychotherapy. Listening intently to psy-
emotional processing has been investigated chotherapy tapes of client-centered therapy and
across research domains including neuroscience analyzing transcripts, Rice and Wagstaff (1967)
(Beitman et al. 2006; Etkin and Wager 2007), classified clients’ style of exploration and
health (Pennebaker and Chung 2007), well-being involvement in therapy moment-by-moment
(Gross and John 2003; Koydemir and Schütz with minimal inferences about meaning or con-
2012; Nyklíček et al. 2011; Saxena et al. 2011), tent. Employing a factor analytic methodology
personality (Newman and McKinney 2002), to locate meaning clusters of process categories
514 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

from an intensive exploration of therapy tran- In a subsequent study, Wiseman and Rice
scripts, Rice and Wagstaff (1967) developed (1989) found evidence for client in-session
classification systems for aspects of client behav- behavior as a function of therapist style. They
ior identified as important client processes in the used psychotherapy transcripts to investigate the
prediction of outcome. sequential nature of therapist-client interactions
Rice and Wagstaff (1967) observed that in suc- in the context of clinical micro-theories of change
cessful cases the client’s voice had two qualities, events. Participants were treated in client-
one of turning inward, exploring inside, and centered therapy and excerpts from transcripts
another, involving intermittent disruption of the that focused on clients’ attempts to resolve prob-
vocal pattern by emotional expression (Rice 1992). lematic reactions. The results indicated that when
In unsuccessful cases, however, the client’s voice therapists were speaking in a softened voice with
tended to either have a strong focus outward, or a an irregular fluctuation pattern, clients were sig-
thin quality with little energy (Rice 1992). They nificantly more likely to shift from an externaliz-
labeled these four Client Vocal Quality categories: ing to focused voice quality, thus attending to
focused, emotional, externalizing, and limited, their own experiences and feelings in an explor-
respectively. They also observed four mutually atory fashion (Wiseman and Rice 1989). Focusing
exclusive verbal categories identifying the level at their investigation on a specific change event,
which the client was engaged with the subject mat- Wiseman and Rice (1989) were able to demon-
ter of the discourse (Rice 1992). They called this strate a context in which the sequential findings
system Expressive Stance and labeled these cate- could be meaningfully interpreted by testing par-
gories: objective analysis, subjective reaction, ticular hypotheses derived from a conceptual
static feeling description, and differentiated explo- model of client change.
ration of feeling. The early work of Rice and colleagues
Following the same methodology of listening revealed that not only are client and therapist pro-
to audiotapes and studying therapy transcripts, cesses, as well as the sequencing of these behav-
Rice and Watstaff (1967) focused on therapist pro- iors important in psychotherapy, but also that
cesses that facilitated these client behaviors. They some client and therapist behaviors are more pro-
identified three therapist process systems: fresh- ductive than others. The study of clients’ and
ness of the therapists’ language, level of the thera- therapists’ overt behavior in psychotherapy at the
pist’s involvement with regard to the clients’ frame micro level, particularly in experiential psycho-
of reference, and therapist vocal quality. Therapist therapies, has demonstrated the importance of
Vocal Quality was classified into seven mutually promoting emotional processing and delineated
exclusive and qualitatively different categories of several models depicting clients’ and therapists’
vocal (Rice and Kerr 1986) with softened, irregu- performances associated with successful thera-
lar and natural, considered to be facilitative of pro- peutic outcomes (Greenberg et al. 2003).
ductive client processing, while restricted, Rogers and Gendlin’s early work was integral
patterned and limited voice qualities tended to to the development of the Experiencing Scale
dampen clients’ productive engagement in the (EXP; Klein et al. 1986), a measure of clients’
therapeutic process (Kerr 1983; Rice and Kerr emotional processing. This scale assesses multi-
1986). The seventh category, definitive voice qual- ple aspects of clients’ emotional processing,
ity, appeared to depend on context. In studies but of particular interest is the immediacy and
investigating therapists’ vocal quality, Rice (1965) quality of the clients’ relationship to their experi-
and Kerr (1983) found that when therapists’ voices ence, and the quality and stage of the exploration
tended to be energetic, controlled, or searching, with regard to their experience. Emotional pro-
clients tended to have better outcomes, and Duncan cessing, as measured by the EXP scale has been
and colleagues (1968) found that a softer voice extensively studied in client-centered,
with a lower pitch was positively associated with process-experiential, and other humanistic thera-
better outcomes in psychotherapy. pies (Goldman 1997; Goldman et al. 2005;
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 515

Hendrix 2002; Pos et al. 2003, 2009; Warwar clients in both therapies demonstrated improve-
1996; Watson and Bedard 2006). ments in perceptual processing over the course of
Overwhelmingly, the results of these studies treatment, becoming more re-evaluative and inte-
indicated that better emotional processing pre- grating new perspectives. In a similar study
dicted better therapy outcomes (Hendricks 2002; examining clients’ self-relevant topics, compar-
Pos et al. 2009; Watson and Bedard 2006), and ing EFT-PE and CBT for depression, Missirlian
that clients with poor emotional processing (2011) extended these findings, showing that cli-
capacities at baseline could improve over the ents’ quality of perceptual processing in conjunc-
course of therapy (Pos et al. 2009; Watson and tion with depth of emotional processing during
Bedard 2006). Studies have shown that when the working phase of therapy proved to be the
controlling for early emotional processing capac- best predictor of outcome for EFT-PE, in com-
ities, clients’ scores at mid or late therapy pre- parison to either alone.
dicted outcome on indices of depression, general Some evidence has been found to support ther-
distress, and self-esteem (Goldman et al. 2005; apists’ capacity to facilitate improvements in cli-
Missirlian 2011; Pos et al. 2003, 2009; Warwar ents’ perceptual processing levels (Macaulay
2003). Pos and colleagues (2003) stated that 70 et al. 2007). Using the York Therapist Process
% of clients’ emotional processing during the Measure (Toukmanian and Armstrong 1998), an
working phase of therapy was not explained by instrument assessing therapists’ level of attun-
clients’ baseline capacities. Thus, both clients ement, tentativeness and meaning exploration
who have the capacity to process their emotional style in relation to clients’ moment-to-moment
experience, as well as those who develop this experience, Macaulay et al. (2007) examined the
capacity during therapy, have a greater likelihood impact of therapists’ behavior on clients’ depth of
of good outcome (Goldman 1997; Goldman et al. emotional processing and level of perceptual pro-
2005; Tomlinson and Hart 1962). cessing in a treatment of EFT-PE for depression.
The research conducted on the EXP scale Results of this investigation were twofold: a
indicates that clients vary in their capacity for greater expression of attunement and tentative-
emotional processing; that clients’ emotional ness was associated with greater complexity in
processing capacity can be improved during the the manner of perceptual processing, i.e., more
course of psychotherapy; that clients’ emotional internal differentiation, re-evaluation, and integra-
processing appears to deepen across therapy; and tion, whereas greater expression of meaning
that clients’ level of investigation, exploration, exploration was associated with a greater depth of
and resolution of their difficulties is deepest dur- emotional processing. These findings confirm that
ing the middle or working stage of therapy (Pos therapists’ style of interacting with their clients in
et al. 2003, 2009; Watson and Bedard 2006). the treatment of depression using EFT-PE, has a
Further clarification is needed to determine which significant impact on improvements in clients’ in-
therapist techniques best facilitate emotional session emotional and perceptual processing.
processing. Researchers propose that promoting aware-
Another productive client process measure is ness and symbolization of experience that leads
that of clients’ perceptual processing. Toukmanian to new understanding and insight is beneficial;
(2004) developed the Levels of Client Perceptual however, greater therapeutic effectiveness is
Processing Scale (LCPP-Revised) consisting of expected when these processes are accompanied
seven mutually exclusive categories of schematic by emotional arousal and expression (Fosha
processing including recognition; elaboration; 2000; Freud 1963; Greenberg 2002; Greenberg
external, analytical, or internal differentiation; and Safran 1987; Perls 1969; Rogers 1951;
reevaluation; and integration. Using the LCPP in Samoilov and Goldfried 2000; Watson 1996). In
a comparative study of process-experiential other words, productive processing of emotion
(EFT-PE) and cognitive-behavioral therapy involves “feeling the feeling” in awareness
(CBT) for depression, Gordon (2007) found that (Greenberg 2008).
516 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

Some support for clients’ emotional arousal as mary emotions as an individual’s very first auto-
an important process in short-term client-centered matic core emotional response to a situation,
and process-experiential therapy was found in such as fear at threat or anger at violation,
two studies demonstrating a significant relation- whereas secondary emotions are responses to
ship between clients’ expressed emotional more primary emotional or cognitive processes
arousal and symptom improvement at the end of such as anger in response to shame, fear of fear,
treatment (Missirlian et al. 2005; Warwar 2003). or anxiety from ruminating or anticipating catas-
It is noteworthy that the mere frequency or degree trophe (Greenberg 2002; Greenberg et al. 2007).
of aroused expressed emotion has not been suffi- Greenberg and colleagues emphasize that activa-
cient in and of itself to lead to successful outcome tion of primary emotions and hot cognitions is
(Greenberg et al. 2007), as the research investi- essential in order to further activate the underly-
gating the association between in-session expres- ing cognitive-affective meaning structure
sion or arousal and outcome has been mixed (scheme) and the personal meaning of these
(Greenberg and Malcolm 2002; Kennedy-Moore experiences in need of exploration (Elliott et al.
and Watson 1999; Nichols and Zax 1977; Warwar 2004; Greenberg et al. 2007; Greenberg and
and Greenberg 2000). Safran 1987; Greenberg and Watson 2006b;
Several researchers have claimed that emo- Whelton 2004; Wiser and Arnow 2001).
tional arousal is not always productive (Greenberg Using a newly developed measure of produc-
2002; Kennedy-Moore and Watson 1999; Wexler tive arousal, the Productive Scale (Greenberg
and Rice 1974). Kennedy-Moore and Watson et al. 2004), Greenberg et al. (2007) found that, as
(1999) stress that what is important is not so much expected, production of more highly aroused
the amount of arousal, but the client’s ability to expressed emotion, rather than arousal alone
engage in therapy so as to process their experi- facilitated therapeutic change and discriminated
ence and to complete the expression. Kennedy- better from poorer outcome cases, even though
Moore and Watson (1999) propose that expression there were no differences between better and
of emotion in and of itself is not necessarily ben- poorer outcome groups on overall degree of
eficial: rather expression is beneficial when it expressed emotional arousal. Highly aroused
facilitates adaptation and leads to the resolution of expressed emotion was shown to be more pro-
distress. Resolution may involve either an increase ductive when characterized by certain emotion-
in the experience of positive feelings and/or the related features: (a) the emotional response was
accessing and processing of negative feelings classified as a primary emotion in order to acti-
(Kennedy-Moore and Watson 1999). vate the underlying cognitive-affective scheme;
Greenberg and colleagues (2007) suggest that (b) the client experienced the emotion in the pres-
merely measuring the degree of arousal of ent; (c) the emotion was fluid, moving forward,
expressed emotion appears to be too general to be and not blocked (or alternately overwhelming);
a valid indicator of productive processing in ther- and (d) the emotion was related to a therapeuti-
apy, and that it seems to be the manner in which cally relevant theme (Greenberg et al. 2007). In
the emotional experience is processed, once acti- addition, the client had to be mindfully aware,
vated, that is important in producing emotional which entailed (e) taking responsibility for his or
change. Warwar (2003) in a study investigating her emotional experience, owning it as his or her
the relationship between emotional processing feeling rather than blaming others for it, and thus
using the experiencing scale, expressed arousal, (f) experienced self as an agent, willing and
and outcome found that the best predictor of motivated to contact the emotion and actively
improvement in depression and general symp- work with it (Greenberg et al. 2007).
toms of distress was the combination of both Watson and colleagues (Kennedy-Moore and
arousal and differentiated experiencing. Watson 1999; Leijssen 1998; Greenberg et al.
Greenberg and colleagues suggest that activa- 2007; Watson 2011; Watson et al. 2007) explain
tion of primary emotions is key (Greenberg et al. that when clients’ emotional experience is either
2007). Greenberg and Paivio (1997) define pri- blocked, or alternately, overwhelming so that
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 517

they are flooded, they may have difficulty main- relational dyad (Greenberg 2007; Pascual-Leone
taining a productive working distance from their 1978; Rice and Greenberg 1984). Task analysis
experience. Thus, there may be an optimal level involves the use video—and audio—tapes of ses-
of arousal (Carryer and Greenberg 2010) for sions, as well as therapy transcripts to intensively
effective therapeutic work to occur. With too lit- analyze and describe clients’ and therapists’ per-
tle emotional arousal clients are limited in their formances in situ (Greenberg 2007; Rice and
capacity to both connect with themselves and Greenberg 1984). Specifically, researchers using
their therapists regarding their experiences, and task analysis are interested in describing success-
with too much emotional arousal clients may be ful performances of both clients and therapists,
unable to adequately reflect on their experiences and identifying the steps that clients perform to
(Kennedy-Moore and Watson 1999; Greenberg resolve specific problem issues in psychotherapy.
et al. 2007; Watson 2011; Watson et al. 2007). This has resulted in a number of performance
Kennedy-Moore and Watson (1999) describe models that could be tested with larger samples
how expression of negative feelings is both a sign to determine whether the steps were component
of distress and a possible means of coping with aspects of resolution. The performance models in
distress; therefore, when a person floods with turn have served as guides for clinicians for how
intense physical, cognitive and affective experi- to intervene at specific junctures in the session to
ence, therapists may need to intervene by either facilitate clients’ emotional processing.
down regulating emotional expression or pro- The task analytic method unfolds in three
cessing the affective material, depending on what phases: discovery, validation, and dynamic
appears to be most beneficial for the client (Briere modeling (Greenberg 2007; Pascual-Leone
1989; Leijssen 1998; McCann and Pearlman et al. 2009). In the discovery phase the researcher
1990; van der Kolk et al. 1996). Without being uses qualitative methods, a mix of expert clini-
able to adequately process their experience the cal experience, intense observation, and an
client has less control over expression and the understanding of the psychological literature, to
action generation process (Kennedy-Moore and develop a model of the resolution of specific
Watson 1999; Greenberg et al. 2007). The func- tasks such as resolving unfinished business with
tion of emotion is to ready the body in response a significant other (Greenberg 2007; A. Pascual-
to environmental stimuli resulting in an action Leone et al. 2009). This is followed by an
tendency: a physiological readiness to respond empirical test of the model using a separate
(Arnold 1960; Frijda 1987, 2005). However, it is sample, and comparing and contrasting cases of
the integration of bodily arousal and conscious resolution and non-resolution of the specific
evaluation of the situation that leads to an effec- task being analyzed (Greenberg 2007; Pascual-
tive response (Beitman et al. 2006; LeDoux Leone et al. 2009).
1996). For example, the exploration of both pri- Rice and Greenberg (1984) developed models
mary, “Is the situation relevant to my well of clients and therapists’ performances in psy-
being?” and, secondary “Of the possible ways of chotherapy to delineate the change processes for
coping with this situation, which would I like to specific change events using task analysis. An
choose at this time?” appraisals are needed event was defined as, “[…] a clinically meaning-
(Lazarus 1991). ful client-therapist interactional sequence that
involved a beginning point, a working-through
process, and an end point” (Greenberg 2007,
30.5 Task Analysis: Modeling p. 16). An event begins with the client’s state-
Change ment of a particular cognitive-affect problem
referred to as a “marker,” followed by a number
Task analysis is an investigative approach that of observable or inferred client and therapist
integrates both theory and rigorous observation, behaviors that lead to resolution of the problem
and analysis of clients, therapists, and/or the (Elliott et al. 2004).
518 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

A number of different performance models than others, and that these serve different pur-
have been developed including models for the poses for people (Gilbert et al. 2004; Greenberg
resolution of problematic reactions (Rice and and Watson 2006b).
Saperia 1984; Watson 1996), self-critical splits Following intensive analysis of psychother-
(Greenberg 1983), decisional conflicts (Clarke apy transcripts, Rice (1974) recognized the
and Greenberg 1986), creation of meaning importance of having clients’ vividly describe
(Clarke 1996), unfinished business with a signifi- external events in order to enhance and facilitate
cant other (Foerster 1990; Greenberg and Foerster in-session change processes. Rice (1973)
1996; Greenberg and Malcolm 2002), self- observed that therapists’ styles influence clients’
interruption (Elliott et al. 2004), and states of in-session processes, and ultimately outcome.
hopelessness (Sicoli 2005). Early studies of task As a result, Rice and Saperia (1984), developed
analyses tested the hypothesized rational- a performance model describing the steps in the
empirical model by relating the presence of key resolution of problematic reactions using
components in the model to good task outcome Systematic Evocative Unfolding. Problematic
(Greenberg 2007; Rice and Greenberg 1984). reactions were identified as moments when cli-
More recently, task analysis has been extended ents viewed their own responses to an event, per-
to include a dynamic modeling phase, to relate a son, or situation as puzzling, confusing,
sequential pattern of processes to outcome unreasonable, or exaggerated. An important step
(Pascual-Leone et al. 2009). In this phase, the in the path to resolution was to help clients bring
sequential structure or patterns identified by the the memory of the scene alive by having clients
models are tested (Pascual-Leone et al. 2009). describe it concretely and vividly (Rice and
The intensive qualitative analysis of clients’ per- Saperia 1984; Watson and Greenberg 1996).
formances is followed with quantitative methods Using vivid description helps to evoke the epi-
and analysis to validate the clinical significance sodic memory and helps clients to pinpoint when
of these processes in relation to client outcomes their reaction occurred and to identify the trigger
at the end of therapy (Pascual-Leone et al. 2009). (Watson and Greenberg 1996).
Process studies investigating two-chair dia- Safran and colleagues (1990, 1994) used task
logue for conflict splits have shown that the analysis to study ruptures in psychotherapy. They
micro-processes involved in resolution are cli- developed a sophisticated model of the resolution
ents’ deeper experiencing of feelings and needs, of alliance ruptures in therapy. After listening
as well as the softening of their internal critical repeatedly to audio-taped therapy sessions, they
voice (Greenberg 1979, 1983; Sicoli and Hallberg identified different themes including: overt
1998). Using theme analysis, Goldman (1997) expressions of negative sentiments; indirect com-
and Kagan (2003) demonstrated the inner critical munication of negative sentiments or hostility;
voices of depressed clients revealed self-oriented disagreements about the goals and tasks of ther-
themes including self-criticism, shutdown, lack apy; compliance; avoidance maneuvers; self-
of direction, and hopelessness (Kagan 2003) and esteem-enhancing operations; and
other-oriented themes of abandonment or rejec- non-responsiveness to interventions. The advan-
tion, isolation, loss, and blame (Kagan 2003). tage of operationalizing ruptures using clearly
The vast majority of subjects in these studies had defined “markers” (Rice and Greenberg 1984) in
both self and other related themes suggesting that this manner was that it enabled researchers to
depression involves both intrapersonal and inter- reliably code categories meeting inter-rater reli-
personal issues (Goldman 1997; Greenberg and ability, and allowed for testing of the model
Watson 2006b; Kagan 2003). In addition, across modalities. The authors conceptualized a
researchers have noted that self-criticism in par- range of possible therapeutic alliance interven-
ticular is not a single process, rather there are dif- tion strategies that appeared important in resolv-
ferent forms, with some being more benevolent ing each aspect of the alliance including the
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 519

therapeutic bond, and collaboration on goals and 30.6 Conclusion


tasks of therapy. An important objective was to
assist therapists in identifying ruptures and to The review of qualitative research approaches
points in the session when greater attention to the that use client interview data as well as transcript
alliance might be necessary. data of clients during psychotherapy shows that
Another group of researchers, Stiles and col- this type of work and methodology has provided
leagues (2005) employed a theory-building case a rich source of information about clients’ and
study research approach using transcripts and therapists’ experience of psychotherapy.
audio recordings to conduct intensive qualitative Qualitative research has illuminated aspects of
analyses of clients’ assimilation of problematic the psychotherapeutic process not visible from
experiences (e.g., Brinegar et al. 2006). This par- clinical trials or from quantitative methods alone.
ticular approach to modeling change tracks and Studies using more qualitative methods have
examines small changes observable in the psy- contributed to the refinement of thinking about
chotherapeutic process related to therapeutic out- the therapeutic alliance and stressed the impor-
come (Stiles 2002; Stiles and Angus 2001). This tance of meta-communication, the need for thera-
work has resulted in a pan-theoretical theory of pists to be more responsive and aware of clients’
change processes and the articulation of an agency in the session, and different therapist
8-stage sequence (Assimilation of Problematic behaviors that clients experience as empathic.
Experiences Sequence, APES; Stiles 2002) that Intense observation of performances in the ses-
describes how problematic experiences can sion has led to the development of numerous
become integrated into the clients’ sense of self. techniques and ways of working with clients to
The qualitative approach of assimilation anal- facilitate clients’ change processes in psycho-
ysis is an intensive, interpretive, iterative proce- therapy. Many of the techniques in experiential
dure for studying client data that involves four and emotion-focused psychotherapy have devel-
steps: scrutinizing therapy data and creating a oped from intense observation and qualitative
summary catalogue; identifying problematic and analysis of therapists’ and clients’ performances
dominant voices representing distinct cognitive- to develop performance models and increase
affective positions within the self; selecting pas- understanding of how to facilitate clients’ emo-
sages relevant to these voices, describing the tional processing during the session. The assump-
relationship of the dominate voices with the other tive framework of humanistic and experiential
voices, and determining whether the voices were psychotherapies fits well with that of qualitative
or were not assimilated during the therapy; and research methodologies as both emphasize cli-
then rating units based on the APES stages (e.g., ents’ reflexivity, autonomy, and capacity for
Brinegar et al. 2006). growth. Going forward it will be important to
The APES rating system provided a tool to continue to investigate psychotherapy process
follow the developmental sequence of therapeu- and outcome using a variety of methodologies in
tic progress. Osatuke and Stiles (2011 p. 215) an attempt to capture as holistically as possible
argue that, “numbers and words are compatible all the aspects of this complex phenomenon.
for building scientific knowledge and can be used
in mutually complementary ways rather than
being pitted against each other.” Osatuke and References
Stiles (2011) suggest that the numeric properties
of the APES make it suitable for use as a metric Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality
(Psychological aspects, Vol. 1). Oxford, England:
of therapeutic progress, particularly because of
Columbia University Press.
its capacity to identify symptomatic change Bachelor, A. (1995). Clients’ perception of the therapeutic
sequentially and within the broader context of alliance: a qualitative analysis. Journal of Counseling
psychological change. Psychology, 42(3), 323–337. doi:10.1037/0022-
0167.42.3.323.
520 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

Bachelor, A., Laverdière, O., Gamache, D., & Bordeleau, Edlund, M. J., Wang, P. S., Berglund, P. A., Katz, S. J.,
V. (2007). Clients’ collaboration in therapy: Self- Lin, E., & Kessler, R. C. (2002). Dropping out of men-
perceptions and relationships with client psychologi- tal health treatment: Patterns and predictors among
cal functioning, interpersonal relations, and epidemiological survey respondents in the United
motivation. Psychotherapy, 44(2), 175–192. States and Ontario. American Journal of Psychiatry,
doi:10.1037/0033-3204.44.2.175. 159, 845–851. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.845.
Beitman, B. D., Viamontes, G. I., Soth, A. M., & Nittler, Elliott, R. (1985). Helpful and nonhelpful events in brief
J. (2006). Toward a neural circuitry of engagement, counseling interviews: An empirical taxonomy.
self-awareness, and pattern search. Psychiatric Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32(3), 307–322.
Annals, 36(4), 272–280. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.32.3.307.
Beutler, L. E. (1995). Common factors and specific Elliott, R. (1989). Comprehensive process analysis:
effects. Clinical Psychologist, 2(1), 79–82. Understanding the change process in significant ther-
Beutler, L. E., Forrester, B., Gallagher-Thompson, D., apy events. In J. M. Parker & R. B. Addison (Eds.),
Thompson, L., & Tomlins, J. B. (2012). Common, Entering the circle: Hermeneutic investigation in psy-
specific, and treatment fit variables in psychotherapy chology (pp. 165–184). Albany, NY: State University
outcome. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 22(3), of New York Press.
255–281. doi:10.1037/a0029695. Elliott, R., & Freire, B. (2008). Person-Centred/experien-
Bolger, E. A. (1999). Grounded theory analysis of emo- tial therapies are highly effective: Summary of the
tional pain. Psychotherapy Research, 9(3), 342–362. 2008 meta-analysis. Person-Centred Quarterly, 1–3.
doi:10.1093/ptr/9.3.342. Elliott, R., James, E., Reimschuessel, C., Cislo, D., &
Briere, J. (1989). Therapy for adults molested as children: Sack, N. (1985). Significant events and the analysis of
Beyond survival. New York, NY: Springer. immediate therapeutic impacts. Psychotherapy:
Brinegar, M. G., Salvi, L. M., Stiles, W. B., & Greenberg, Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 22(3), 620–630.
L. S. (2006). Building a meaning bridge: Therapeutic doi:10.1037/h0085548.
progress from problem formulation to understanding. Elliott, R., Watson, J. C., Greenberg, L., & Goldman, R.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53(2), 165–180. (2004). Learning emotion focused psychotherapy: The
doi:10.1037/0022-0167.53.2.165. process-experiential approach to change. Washington,
Carryer, J. R., & Greenberg, L. S. (2010). Optimal levels DC: APA Books.
of emotional arousal in experiential therapy of depres- Etkin, A., & Wager, T. D. (2007). Functional neuroimag-
sion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, ing of anxiety: A meta-analysis of emotional process-
78(2), 190–199. doi:10.1037/a0018401. ing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific
Clarke, K. M. (1996). Change processes in a creation of phobia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(10),
meaning event. Journal of Consulting and Clinical 1476–1488. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504.
Psychology, 64(3), 465–470. doi:10.1037/0022- Foerster, F. S. (1990). Refinement and verification of a
006X.64.3.465. model of the resolution of unfinished business
Clarke, K. M., & Greenberg, L. S. (1986). Differential (Unpublished master’s thesis). York University, York,
effects of the gestalt two-chair intervention and prob- ON, Canada.
lem solving in resolving decisional conflict. Journal of Fosha, D. (2000). The transforming power of affect: A
Counseling Psychology, 33(1), 11–15. model for accelerated change. New York, NY: Basic
doi:10.1037/0022-0167.33.1.11. Books.
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2009). Basics of qualitative Freud, S. (1963). General psychological theory. Oxford,
research: Techniques and procedures for developing England: Crowell-Collier.
grounded theory (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure, and
Critchfield, K. L. (2012). Tailoring common treatment action tendency. Cognition and Emotion, 1(2), 115–
principles to fit individual personalities. Journal of 143. doi:10.1080/02699938708408043.
Personality Disorders, 26(1), 108–125. doi:10.1521/ Frijda, N. H. (2005). Emotion experience. Cognition and
pedi.2012.26.1.108. Emotion, 19(4), 473–497.
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: doi:10.1080/02699930441000346.
Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Gendlin, E. T. (1969). Focusing. Psychotherapy: Theory,
New York: Harcourt Brace. Research & Practice, 6(1), 4–15. doi:10.1037/
Dierick, P., & Lietaer, G. (2008). Client perception of h0088716.
therapeutic factors in group psychotherapy and growth Gendlin, E. T. (1996). Focusing-oriented psychotherapy:
groups: An empirically-based hierarchical model. A manual of the experiential method. New York, NY:
International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 58(2), Guilford Press.
203–230. doi:10.1521/ijgp.2008.58.2.203. Gilbert, P., Clarke, M., Hempel, S., Miles, J. N. V., &
Duncan, S., Rice, L. N., & Butler, J. M. (1968). Therapists’ Irons, C. (2004). Criticizing and reassuring oneself:
paralanguage in peak and poor psychotherapy hours. An exploration of forms, styles and reasons in female
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73(6), 566–570. students. British Journal of Clinical Psychology,
doi:10.1037/h0026597. 43(1), 31–50. doi:10.1348/014466504772812959.
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 521

Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1987). Emotion in psy-
grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. chotherapy: Affect, cognition, and the process of
Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company. change. New York: Guilford Press.
Goldman, R. (1997). Thematic depth of experiencing and Greenberg, L. S., & Watson, J. C. (2006a). Change pro-
outcome in experiential psychotherapy (Doctoral dis- cess research. In J. C. Norcross, L. E. Beutler, & R. F.
sertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Levant (Eds.), Evidence-based practice in mental
Theses database. (UMI No. 1998-95008-022) health: Debate and dialogue on the fundamental ques-
Goldman, R. N., Greenberg, L. S., & Pos, A. E. (2005). tions (pp. 81–89). Washington, DC: American
Depth of emotional experience and outcome. Psychological Association.
Psychotherapy Research, 15(3), 248–260. doi:10.108 Greenberg, L. S., & Watson, J. C. (2006b). Emotion-focused
0/10503300512331385188. therapy for depression. Washington, DC: American
Gordon, L. B. (2007). Client perceptual processing in Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/11286-000.
cognitive behavioural therapy and process- Greenberg, L., Elliott, R., & Lietaer, G. (2003).
experiential therapy for depression (Doctoral disserta- Humanistic-experiential psychotherapy. In G. Stricker
tion). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and & T. Widiger (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (Clinical
Theses database. (UMI No. 2007-99240-045) Psychology, Vol. 8, pp. 301–326). Hoboken, NJ:
Greenberg, L. S. (1979). Resolving splits: Use of the two Wiley.
chair technique. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Greenberg, L. S., Auszra, L., & Herrmann, I. R. (2004).
Practice, 16(3), 316–324. Productivity scale. Unpublished instrument,
Greenberg, L. S. (1983). Toward a task analysis of conflict Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto,
resolution in gestalt therapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, ON, Canada.
Research & Practice, 20(2), 190–201. doi:10.1037/ Greenberg, L. S., Auszra, L., & Herrmann, I. R. (2007).
h0088490. The relationship among emotional productivity, emo-
Greenberg, L. S. (1986). Change process research. tional arousal and outcome in experiential therapy of
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(1), depression. Psychotherapy Research, 17(4), 482–493.
4–9. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.54.1.4. doi:10.1080/10503300600977800.
Greenberg, L. S. (2002). Emotion-focused therapy: Grencavage, L. M., & Norcross, J. C. (1990). Where are
Coaching clients to work through their feelings. doi: the commonalties among the therapeutic common fac-
10.1037/10447-000. tors? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
Greenberg, L. S. (2007). A guide to conducting a task 5, 372–378. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.21.5.372.
analysis of psychotherapeutic change. Psychotherapy Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in
Research, 17(1), 15–30. two emotion regulation processes: Implications for
doi:10.1080/10503300600720390. affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of
Greenberg, L. S. (2008). Emotion and cognition in psy- Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.
chotherapy: The transforming power of affect. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.
Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, Hays, D. G., & Singh, A. A. (2011). Qualitative inquiry in
49(1), 49–59. doi:10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.49. clinical and education settings. New York, NY:
Greenberg, L. S., & Bolger, L. (2001). An emotion- Guilford Press.
focused approach to the over-regulation of emotion Hendricks, M. N. (2002). Focusing-oriented/experiential
and emotional pain. Journal of Clinical Psychology: psychotherapy. In D. J. Cain & J. Seeman (Eds.),
In-Session, 57, 197–211. Humanistic psychotherapies: Handbook of research and
Greenberg, L. S., & Foerster, F. S. (1996). Task analysis practice (pp. 221–251). Washington, DC: APA Press.
exemplified: The process of resolving unfinished busi- Heppner, P. P., Kivlighan, D. M., & Wampold, B. E.
ness. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, (1992). Research design in counseling. Belmont, CA:
64(3), 439–446. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.439. Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing.
Greenberg, L. S., & Malcolm, W. (2002). Resolving Hill, C. E., & Lambert, M. J. (2004). Methodological
unfinished business: Relating process to outcome. issues in studying psychotherapy processes and out-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), comes. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Handbook of psycho-
406–416. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.2.406. therapy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 84–136).
Greenberg, L. S., & Paivio, S. C. (1997). Working with the New York: Wiley.
emotions in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press. Hill, C. E., Helms, J. E., Spiegel, S. B., & Tichenor, V.
Greenberg, L. S., & Pinsof, W. M. (Eds.). (1986). The psy- (1988). Development of a system for categorizing cli-
chotherapeutic process: A research handbook. ent reactions to therapist interventions. Journal of
New York: Guilford Press. Counseling Psychology, 35(1), 27–36. doi:10.1037/0022-
Greenberg, L. S., & Rice, L. N. (1981). The specific 0167.35.1.27.
effects of a gestalt intervention. Psychotherapy: Hill, C. E., Thompson, B. J., & Corbett, M. M. (1992).
Theory, Research & Practice, 18(1), 31–37. The impact of therapist ability to perceive displayed
doi:10.1037/h0085958. and hidden client reactions on immediate outcome in
522 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

first sessions of brief therapy. Psychotherapy Research, S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and
2(2), 143–155. doi:10.1080/10503309212331332914. behavior change (4th ed., pp. 72–113). New York:
Hill, C. E., Thompson, B. J., Cogar, M. C., & Denman, Wiley.
D. W. (1993). Beneath the surface of long-term therapy: Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997).
Therapist and client report of their own and each other’s Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In
covert processes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.),
40(3), 278–287. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.40.3.278. Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational
Hinterkopf, E., & Brunswick, L. (1981). Teaching mental processes (pp. 97–135). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
patients to use client-centered and experiential thera- Erlbaum.
peutic skills with each other. Psychotherapy: Theory, Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York:
Research & Practice, 18(3), 394–402. doi:10.1037/ Oxford University Press.
h0088392. LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysteri-
Hollon, S. D. (1999). Allegiance effects in treatment ous underpinning of emotional life. New York: Simon
research: A commentary. Clinical Psychology: & Schuster.
Science and Practice, 6, 107–112. Leijssen, M. (1990). On focusing and the necessary condi-
Hunt, M. G. (1998). The only way out is through: Emotional tions of therapeutic personality change. In G. Liétaer,
processing and recovery after a depressing life event. J. Rombauts, & R. Van Balen (Eds.), Client-centered
Behavior Therapy Research, 36(4), 361–384. and experiential psychotherapy in the nineties (pp. 225–
Kagan, N. (1975). Influencing human interaction: Eleven 250). Leuven, Belgium: Leuven University Press.
years with IPR. Canadian Counselor, 9(2), 74–97. Leijssen, M. (1998). Focusing microprocesses. In L. S.
Kagan, F. (2003). Differentiating depression: A qualita- Greenberg, J. C. Watson, & G. O. Lietaer (Eds.),
tive analysis of the depression themes within 36 case Handbook of experiential psychotherapy (pp. 121–
studies (Unpublished honor’s thesis). York University, 154). New York: Guilford Press.
Toronto, ON, Canada Lietaer, G. (1992). Helping and hindering processes in
Karreman, A., & Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2012). client-centered/experiential psychotherapy: A content
Attachment and well-being: The mediating role of analysis of client and therapist postsession percep-
emotion regulation and resilience. Personality and tions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Individual Differences, 53(7), 821–826. doi:10.1016/j. Lietaer, G., & Neirinck, M. (1986). Client and therapist
paid.2012.06.014. perceptions of helping processes in client-centered/
Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (1999). Expressing experiential psychotherapy. Person-Centered Review,
emotion: Myths, realities and therapeutic strategies. 1(4), 436–455.
New York: Guilford Press. Loynes, J. L. (1984). Effects of experiential focusing on
Kerr, G. P. (1983). Therapist vocal quality and psycho- anger experiences of separated or divorced men
therapeutic effectiveness (Unpublished doctoral dis- (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Michigan State
sertation). York University, Toronto. University, East Lansing, MI.
Kiesler, D. J. (1973). The process of psychotherapy: Luborsky, L., Singer, B., & Luborsky, L. (1975).
Empirical foundations and systems of analysis. Comparative studies of psychotherapies. Is it true that
Oxford, England: Aldine. “everyone has won and all must have prizes”? Archives
Klein, M. H., Mathieu-Coughlan, P. L., & Kiesler, D. J. of General Psychiatry, 32, 995–1008.
(1986). The experiencing scales. In L. S. Greenberg & Luborsky, L., Diguer, L., Seligman, D. A., Rosenthal, R.,
W. M. Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: Krause, E. D., Johnson, S., & Schweizer, E. (1999).
A research handbook (pp. 21–71). New York: Guilford The researcher’s own therapy allegiances: A “wild
Press. card” in comparisons of treatment efficacy. Clinical
Koydemir, S., & Schütz, A. (2012). Emotional intelli- Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 95–106.
gence predicts components of subjective well-being Macaulay, H., Toukmanian, S. G., & Gordon, K. M.
beyond personality: A two-country study using self- (2007). Attunement as the core of therapist expressed
and informant reports. The Journal of Positive empathy. Canadian Journal of Counselling, 41, 244–
Psychology, 7(2), 107–118. doi:10.1080/17439760.20 254, 4.
11.647050. Mash, E. J., & Hunsley, J. (1993). Assessment consider-
Lambert, M. (1999). Are differential treatment effects ations in the identification of failing psychotherapy:
inflated by researcher therapy allegiance? Could Bringing the negatives out of the darkroom.
Clever Hans count? Clinical Psychology: Science and Psychological Assessment, 5(3), 292–301.
Practice, 6, 127–130. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.5.3.292.
Lambert, M. J., & Barley, D. E. (2001). Research sum- McCann, I. L., & Pearlman, L. A. (1990). Psychological
mary on the therapeutic relationship and psychother- trauma and the adult survivor: Theory, therapy, and
apy outcome. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, transformation. Philadelphia, PA: Brunner/Mazel.
Practice, Training, 38(4), 357–361. Messer, S. B., & Wampold, B. E. (2002). Let’s face facts:
doi:10.1037/0033-3204.38.4.357. Common factors are more potent than specific therapy
Lambert, M. J., & Hill, C. E. (1994). Assessing psycho- ingredients. Clinical Psychology: Science and
therapy outcomes and processes. In A. E. Bergin & Practice, 9(1), 21–25. doi:10.1093/clipsy/9.1.21.
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 523

Missirlian, T. M. (2011). A comparative study of the Psychology, 77(6), 1055–1066. doi:10.1037/


nature of change processes in emotion focused and a0017059.
cognitive-behavioural psychotherapies for depression Rennie, D. L. (1994). Clients’ deference in psychother-
(Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest apy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41(4), 427–
Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI No. 437. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.41.4.427.
2012-99060-136) Rennie, D. L. (1998). Person-centred counselling: An
Missirlian, T. M., Toukmanian, S. G., Warwar, S. H., & experiential approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Greenberg, L. S. (2005). Emotional arousal, client per- Rennie, D. L. (2001). The client as a self-aware agent in
ceptual processing, and the working alliance in experi- counselling and psychotherapy. Counselling &
ential psychotherapy for depression. Journal of Psychotherapy Research, 1(2), 82–89. doi:10.1080/14
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(5), 861–871. 733140112331385118.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.5.861. Rennie, D. L., Phillips, J. R., & Quartaro, G. K. (1988).
Newman, L. S., & McKinney, L. C. (2002). Repressive Grounded theory: A promising approach to conceptu-
coping and threat avoidance: An idiographic stroop alization in psychology? Canadian Psychology/
study. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Psychologie Canadienne, 29(2), 139–150.
28(3), 409–422. doi:10.1177/0146167202286011. doi:10.1037/h0079765.
Nichols, M. P., & Zax, M. (1977). Catharsis in psycho- Rhodes, R. H., Hill, C. E., Thompson, B. J., & Elliott, R.
therapy. New York, NY: Gardner Press. (1994). Client retrospective recall of resolved and
Norcross, J. C., & Wampold, B. E. (2011). What works unresolved misunderstanding events. Journal of
for whom: Tailoring psychotherapy to the person. Counseling Psychology, 41(4), 473–483.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 127–132. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.41.4.473.
Nyklíček, I., Vingerhoets, A., & Zeelenberg, M. (2011). Rice, L. N. (1965). Therapist’s style of participation and
Emotion regulation and well-being. New York, NY: case outcome. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-6953-8. 29(2), 155–160. doi:10.1037/h0021926.
Orlinsky, D. E., Grawe, K., & Parks, B. K. (1994). Process Rice, L. N. (1973). Client behavior as a function of thera-
and outcome in psychotherapy-Noch einmal. In A. E. pist style and client resources. Journal of Counseling
Bergin & S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psycho- Psychology, 20(4), 306–311. doi:10.1037/h0034805.
therapy and behavior change (pp. 270–376). Rice, L. N. (1974). The evocation function of the thera-
New York: Wiley. pist. In D. Wexler & L. N. Rice (Eds.), Innovations in
Osatuke, K., & Stiles, W. B. (2011). Numbers in assimila- client-centered therapy (pp. 289–311). New York:
tion research. Theory & Psychology, 21(2), 200–219. Wiley Interscience.
doi:10.1177/0959354310391352. Rice, L. N. (1992). From naturalistic observation of psy-
Pascual-Leone, J. (1978). Compounds, confounds, and chotherapy process to micro theories of change. In
models in developmental information processing: A S. G. Toukmanian & D. L. Rennie (Eds.),
reply to trabasso and foellinger. Journal of Psychotherapy process research: Paradigmatic and
Experimental Child Psychology, 26(1), 18–40. narrative approaches (Vol. 143, pp. 1–21). Thousand
Pascual-Leone, A., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007). Emotional Oaks, CA: Sage.
processing in experiential therapy: Why “the only way Rice, L. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (Eds.). (1984). Patterns of
out is through.”. Journal of Consulting and Clinical change: Intensive analysis of psychotherapy process.
Psychology, 75(6), 875–887. New York: Guilford.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.75.6.875. Rice, L. N., & Kerr, G. (1986). Measures of client and
Pascual-Leone, A., Greenberg, L. S., & Pascual-Leone, therapist vocal quality. In L. Greenberg & W. Pinsoff
J. (2009). Developments in task analysis: New meth- (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research
ods to study change. Psychotherapy Research, 19(4– handbook (pp. 73–105). New York: Guilford Press.
5), 527–542. doi:10.1080/10503300902897797. Rice, L. N., & Saperia, E. (1984). Task analysis and the
Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2007). Expressive resolution of problematic reactions. In L. N. Rice &
writing, emotional upheavals, and health. In H. S. L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Patterns of change: Intensive
Friedman & R. C. Silver (Eds.), Foundations of health analysis of psychotherapy process (pp. 29–66).
psychology (pp. 263–284). New York: Oxford New York: Guilford.
University Press. Rice, L. N., & Wagstagg, A. K. (1967). Client voice qual-
Perls, F. S. (1969). Gestalt therapy verbatim. Moab, UT: ity and expressive style as indexes of productive psy-
Real People Press. chotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31(6),
Pos, A. E., Greenberg, L. S., Goldman, R. N., & Korman, 557–563. doi:10.1037/h0025164.
L. M. (2003). Emotional processing during experien- Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy; its current
tial treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting and practice, implications, and theory. Oxford, England:
Clinical Psychology, 71(6), 1007–1016. Houghton Mifflin.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.71.6.1007. Rogers, C. R. (1959a). A tentative scale for the measure-
Pos, A. E., Greenberg, L. S., & Warwar, S. H. (2009). ment of process in psychotherapy. In E. A. Rubinstein
Testing a model of change in the experiential treat- & M. B. Parloff (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy
ment of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical (pp. 96–107). doi: 10.1037/10036-006.
524 J.C. Watson and E.J. McMullen

Rogers, C. R. (1959b). A theory of therapy, personality, episodes in brief person-centred counselling.


and interpersonal relationships, as developed in the Counselling & Psychotherapy Research, 1(1), 62–73.
client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), doi:10.1080/14733140112331385268.
Psychology: The study of a science (Vol. 3, pp. 184– Tinsley, D. J., Holt, M. S., Hinson, J. A., & Tinsley, H. E.
256). New York: McGraw-Hill. (1991). A construct validation study of the expecta-
Safran, J. D., Crocker, P., McMain, S., & Murray, P. tions about counseling-brief form: Factorial validity.
(1990). Therapeutic alliance rupture as a therapy event Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and
for empirical investigation. Psychotherapy: Theory, Development, 24(3), 101–110.
Research, Practice, Training, 27(2), 154–165. Tomlinson, T. M., & Hart, J. T. (1962). A validation study
doi:10.1037/0033-3204.27.2.154. of the process scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
Safran, J. D., Muran, J. C., & Samstag, L. W. (1994). 26(1), 74–78. doi:10.1037/h0041062.
Resolving therapeutic alliance ruptures: A task ana- Toukmanian, S. G. (2004). Levels of client perceptual pro-
lytic investigation. In A. O. Horvath & L. S. Greenberg cessing (LCPP-R): A measure for psychotherapy process
(Eds.), The working alliance: Theory, research, and research, revised. Unpublished measure, Department of
practice (pp. 225–255). New York: Basic Books. Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON.
Samoilov, A., & Goldfried, M. R. (2000). Role of emotion Toukmanian, S. G., & Armstrong, S. M. (1998). York
in cognitive-behavior therapy. Clinical Psychology: therapist process measure. Unpublished measure,
Science and Practice, 7(4), 373–385. doi:10.1093/ York University, Toronto, ON.
clipsy/7.4.373. van der Kolk, B. A., McFarlane, A. C., & Weisaeth, L.
Saxena, P., Dubey, A., & Pandey, R. (2011). Role of emo- (1996). Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming
tion regulation difficulties in predicting mental health experience on mind, body, and society. New York:
and well-being. Journal of Projective Psychology & Guilford Press.
Mental Health, 18(2), 147–155. Vanaerschot, G., & Lietaer, G. (2007). Therapeutic ingre-
Sicoli, L. A. (2005). Development and verification of a dients in helping session episodes with observer-rated
model of resolving hopelessness in process- low and high empathic attunements: A content analy-
experiential therapy of depression (Doctoral disserta- sis of client and therapist postsession perceptions in
tion). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and three cases. Psychotherapy Research, 17(3), 338–352.
Theses database. (UMI No. 2006-99014-142). doi:10.1080/10503300600650910.
Sicoli, L. A., & Hallberg, E. T. (1998). An analysis of cli- Vanaerschot, G., & Lietaer, G. (2010). Client and therapist
ent performance in the two-chair method. Canadian postsession perceptions of therapeutic ingredients in
Journal of Counselling, 32(2), 151–162. helping episodes: A study based on three cases.
Smith, E. C., & Grawe, K. (2005). Which therapeutic Person-Centered and Experiential Psychotherapies,
mechanisms work when? A step towards the formula- 9(3), 205–219.
tion of empirically validated guidelines for therapists’ Wang, J. (2007). Mental health treatment dropout and its
session-to-session decisions. Clinical Psychology & correlates in a general population sample. Medical
Psychotherapy, 12(2), 112–123. doi:10.1002/cpp.427. Care, 45, 224–229.
Stiles, W. B. (2002). Assimilation of problematic experi- Warwar, S. (1996). The relationship between level of
ences. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relation- experiencing and session outcome in client-centered
ships that work: Therapist contributions and and process-experiential therapies. (Master’s thesis).
responsiveness to patients (pp. 357–365). New York: Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses
Oxford University Press. database. (UMI No. MM10333)
Stiles, W. B. (2005). Extending the assimilation of prob- Warwar, S. (2003). Relating emotional arousal to out-
lematic experiences scale: Commentary on the special come in the treatment of depression (Doctoral disser-
issue. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 18(2), tation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and
85–93. doi:10.1080/09515070500136868. Theses database. (UMI No. 2005-99014-377)
Stiles, W. B., & Angus, L. (2001). Qualitative research on Warwar, S., & Greenberg, L. S. (2000). Advances in theo-
clients’ assimilation of problematic experiences in psy- ries of change and counseling. In S. D. Brown & R. W.
chotherapy. Psychologische Beitrage, 43(3), 112–127. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (3rd
The Focusing Institute (2013). http://www.focusing.org. ed., pp. 571–600). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Accessed 16 May 2013. Watson, J. C. (1996). The relationship between vivid
Timulak, L. (2007). Identifying core categories of client- description, emotional arousal, and in-session resolu-
identified impact of helpful events in psychotherapy: tion of problematic reactions. Journal of Consulting
A qualitative meta-analysis. Psychotherapy Research, and Clinical Psychology, 64(3), 459–464.
17(3), 310–320. doi:10.1080/10503300600608116. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.459.
Timulak, L. (2011). Review of case studies in emotion- Watson, J. C. (2011). The process of growth and transfor-
focused treatment of depression: A comparison of mation: Extending the process model. Person-
good and poor outcome. Counselling & Psychotherapy Centered & Experiential Psychotherapies, 10(1),
Research, 11(1), 78–80. 11–27. doi:10.1080/14779757.2011.564760.
Timulák, L., & Lietaer, G. (2001). Moments of empower- Watson, J. C., & Bedard, D. L. (2006). Clients’ emotional
ment: A qualitative analysis of positively experienced processing in psychotherapy: A comparison between
30 Change Process Research in Psychotherapy 525

cognitive-behavioral and process-experiential thera- Psychotherapy Research, 11(2), 221–233.


pies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, doi:10.1093/ptr/11.2.221.
74(1), 152–159. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.1.152. Wexler, D. A., & Rice, L. N. (Eds.). (1974). Innovations
Watson, J. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (1996). Pathways to in client-centered therapy. Oxford, England: Wiley.
change in the psychotherapy of depression: Relating Whelton, W. J. (2004). Emotional processes in psycho-
process to session change and outcome. Psychotherapy: therapy: Evidence across therapeutic modalities.
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 33(2), 262–274. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 11(1), 58–71.
doi:10.1037/0033-3204.33.2.262. doi:10.1002/cpp.392.
Watson, J. C., & Rennie, D. L. (1994). Qualitative analy- Wierzbicki, M., & Pekarik, G. (1993). A meta-analysis of
sis of clients’ subjective experience of significant psychotherapy dropout. Professional Psychology:
moments during the exploration of problematic reac- Research and Practice, 24(2), 190–195.
tions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41(4), 500– doi:10.1037/0735-7028.24.2.190.
509. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.41.4.500. Wiseman, H., & Rice, L. N. (1989). Sequential analyses
Watson, J. C., Goldman, R. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007). of therapist-client interaction during change events: A
Case studies in emotion-focused treatment of depres- task-focused approach. Journal of Consulting and
sion: A comparison of good and poor outcome. Clinical Psychology, 57(2), 281–286.
Washington, DC, US: American Psychological doi:10.1037/0022-006X.57.2.281.
Association. doi:10.1037/11586-000. Wiser, S., & Arnow, B. (2001). Emotional experiencing:
Weerasekera, P., Linder, B., Greenberg, L., & Watson, To facilitate or regulate? Journal of Clinical
J. (2001). The working alliance in client-centered and Psychology, 57(2), 157–168.
process-experiential therapy of depression.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai