Anda di halaman 1dari 10

New insights into central cardiovascular control during exercise in humans: a

central command update

J. W Williamson, P. J Fadel and J. H Mitchell

Exp Physiol 2006;91;51-58; originally published online Oct 20, 2005;

DOI: 10.1113/expphysiol.2005.032037

This information is current as of December 13, 2007

This is the final published version of this article; it is available at:


http://ep.physoc.org/cgi/content/full/91/1/51

This version of the article may not be posted on a public access website for 12 months
after publication, unless the article is open access.

Experimental Physiology is a publication of The Physiological Society. It has been published


continuously since 1908. To subscribe to Experimental Physiology go to
http://ep.physoc.org/subscriptions. Experimental Physiology articles are free 12 months after
publication. No part of this article may be reproduced without the permission of Blackwell
Publishing:
JournalsRights@oxon.blackwellpublishing.com

Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007


Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58 51

Experimental Physiology – Neural Control of the Circulation During Exercise

New insights into central cardiovascular control during


exercise in humans: a central command update
J. W. Williamson1 , P. J. Fadel2 and J. H. Mitchell3
1
Department of Kinesiology, Health Promotion & Recreation University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA
2
Department of Medical Pharmacology and Physiology, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
3
Department of Internal Medicine, The Harry S. Moss Heart Center, University of Texas South-western Medical Center, Dallas, TX, USA

The autonomic adjustments to exercise are mediated by central signals from the higher brain
(central command) and by a peripheral reflex arising from working skeletal muscle (exercise
pressor reflex), with further modulation provided by the arterial baroreflex. Although it is clear
that central command, the exercise pressor reflex and the arterial baroreflex are all requisite
for eliciting appropriate cardiovascular adjustments to exercise, this review will be limited
primarily to discussion of central command. Central modulation of the cardiovascular system via
descending signals from higher brain centres has been well recognized for over a century, yet the
specific regions of the human brain involved in this exercise-related response have remained
speculative. Brain mapping studies during exercise as well as non-exercise conditions have
provided information towards establishing the cerebral cortical structures in the human brain
specifically involved in cardiovascular control. The purpose of this review is to provide an update
of current concepts on central command in humans, with a particular emphasis on the regions
of the brain identified to alter autonomic outflow and result in cardiovascular adjustments.
(Received 19 August 2005; accepted after revision 5 October 2005; first published online 20 October 2005)
Corresponding author J. W. Williamson: Univeristy of North Texas, College of Education, PO Box 311337, Denton,
TX 76203-1337, USA. Email: jwilliamson@coe.unt.edu

Neural control of the circulation during exercise is a thalamus, hypothalamus and mesencephalon prior to
complex phenomenon involving many sites within the reaching medullary regions of cardiovascular integration.
central nervous system from sacral portions of the spinal Animal investigations have employed stimulation of both
cord to higher regions of the cerebral cortex. The concept diencephalic and mescenphalic regions to represent the
of descending signals from higher brain centres capable descending central command signals and have provided
of influencing cardiovascular responses during exercise valuable information as to the role of central command
(Johansson, 1895; Zuntz & Geppert, 1886), originally in cardiovascular regulation (Waldrop et al. 1996). What
termed ‘cortical irradiation’ (Krogh & Lindhard, 1913) remains of interest are the specific cerebral cortical regions
and later termed ‘central command’ (Goodwin et al. of the human brain activated by central command during
1972), has been well studied and is widely accepted exercise. By mapping the human brain during exercise, it
throughout the scientific community. Most would concur is possible to gain further insight as to the specific regions
that the magnitude of central command during exercise involved in central command or, more specifically, the
can be largely dictated by an individual’s perception of neural networks that are altered by changes in perception
effort during actual or even attempted physical exertion, of effort and result in cardiovascular adjustments.
independent of the actual workload or force production
(Mitchell, 1990). While increases in an individual’s rating
of perceived exertion or effort sense during exercise
Defining and re-defining central command
are coupled with elevated cardiovascular responses, the
specific region(s) within the higher brain responsible Before one can begin to discuss the functional importance
for generating the neural signals resulting in autonomic of central command and the location of the brain regions
adjustments has remained elusive. It is postulated that participating in a central command response, one must
these signals can affect neural activity within the first consider how the term central command is being


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society DOI: 10.1113/expphysiol.2005.032037
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
52 J. W. Williamson and others Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58

used. The concept of central command during exercise terminology in reference to a parallel activation of both
has been classically defined as ‘a feedforward mechanism cardiovascular and motor regions during exercise remains
involving parallel activation of motor and cardiovascular accurate, since the activation of motor and cardiovascular
centres’. The primary focus of central command-related centres in the higher brain is requisite for physical activity.
investigations has been the modulation of motor effort However, an important finding from recent research is
and the resulting alterations in cardiovascular responses. that a network of structures exist that are involved in a
We would contend that the cerebral cortical regions centrally mediated cardiovascular activation, which do
involved in a ‘central cardiovascular command’ do not require a parallel motor activation to exert their
not always require the parallel activation of ‘central influence (Nowak et al. 1999, 2005; Williamson et al.
motor command’ systems to exert their influence. This 1999, 2001; 2002). Therefore, central command during
contention is based on findings that indicate that the exercise may actually involve the simultaneous activation
magnitude of a central command-mediated cardiovascular of two separate networks, one for central motor control
response during exercise can be independent of force and one for central cardiovascular control. While these two
production (e.g. imagined exercise) and dictated more by networks may interact as components of central command
an individual’s perception of effort (see Fig. 1; Nowak et al. during exercise, they can function independently of one
1999, 2005; Williamson et al. 1999, 2001, 2002). Thus, another. In this regard, we will focus our discussion
central command implies an ‘effort-induced modulation specifically on those brain regions believed to be involved
of autonomic function’. Nevertheless, the classically used in central cardiovascular regulation.

Figure 1. Heart rate, mean blood


pressure and perceived exertion
responses during actual and imagined
handgrip exercise
Subjects with high hypnotizability exhibited
significant increases in heart rate, mean
blood pressure and perceived exertion
during imagined handgrip (right-hand side)
compared to subjects with low
hypnotizability. Importantly, muscle
electromyographic recordings demonstrated
that no measurable increases in force were
produced during imagined handgrip. The
data are presented as means ± S.D. and
significant differences between groups are
shown as ∗ P < 0.05. (Reproduced with
permission from Williamson et al. 2002.)


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58 Central cardiovascular control 53

A second issue involving the classic definition of Thus, it is the sense of effort that appears to be driving the
central command is the term ‘feedforward’ mechanism. central command response. Therefore, we propose that
Feedforward typically implies a control system operating ‘central command’ can function as a feedback system,
without continuous negative feedback. This feedforward responding to an individual’s sense of effort, to elicit
characterization may be largely based on the immediate proportional changes in cardiovascular responses, which
cardiovascular response to the onset (or even anticipation) does not necessarily require a parallel motor activation to
of exercise, such that there would not be sufficient time exert its influence.
for feedback from the periphery (i.e. working skeletal
muscle afferent signals) to reach the cardiovascular centres
in the brainstem (Johansson, 1895; Zuntz & Geppert, Importance of central command in cardiovascular
1886). However, there is evidence that the effects of control during exercise
central command on cardiovascular responses are closely
related to the intensity or perceived effort of the exercise The cardiovascular and haemodynamic adjustments
(Asmussen et al. 1965; Gandevia et al. 1993; Leonard et al. to exercise are primarily mediated by alterations
1985), even at the onset of exercise (Smith et al. 1997). in parasympathetic and sympathetic neural activity
Studies employing partial and complete neuromuscular (Mitchell, 1990). These exercise-induced changes in
blockade have reported greater cardiovascular responses autonomic neural outflow, which are designed to meet the
resulting from a greater level of central command as metabolic demands of the exercising muscle, are mediated
indexed by an individual’s perceived effort (Asmussen via multiple neural mechanisms working in concert. It is
et al. 1965; Leonard et al. 1985; Gallagher et al. 2001; well accepted that central command (Waldrop et al. 1996),
Gandevia et al. 1993). From these findings, a strong the exercise pressor reflex (McCloskey & Mitchell, 1972;
case could be made that an individual’s perception or Mitchell et al. 1983; Kaufman & Forster, 1996) and the
sense of effort can serve as a means of ‘feedback’ to arterial baroreflex (Raven et al. 1997; Fadel et al. 2004) are
establish the magnitude of the central command response. all involved in mediating the characteristic cardiovascular
Although feedback from the working muscles during and haemodynamic adjustments to exercise (Fig. 2).
exercise may certainly modulate the central command The importance of central command in initiating the
response, the aforementioned studies show that muscle autonomic adjustments to exercise is further emphasized
afferent feedback is not a requisite component of the by the key role central command plays in the resetting
central command response. A key point for consideration of the arterial baroreflex during exercise (Gallagher et al.
in human studies is that an increase in motor unit 2001; Querry et al. 2001; Ogoh et al. 2002; Raven et al.
recruitment or force production, resulting in an increased 2002). By using an innovative tendon vibration protocol
afferent feedback, is typically coupled with increased (Goodwin et al. 1972) to alter central command input,
ratings of perceived exertion or effort. More importantly, Ogoh et al. (2002) recently demonstrated that the resetting
changes in perception of effort, even without alterations in of the carotid baroreflex stimulus–response curve during
muscle afferent input during constant exercise, can modify exercise could be reduced to lower pressures or increased to
cardiovascular responses (Williamson et al. 2001, 2002). higher pressures by selectively decreasing or augmenting

Figure 2. A schematic illustration of the


mechanisms of neural cardiovascular
control during exercise
Neural signals originating from the brain
(central command), the aorta and carotid
arteries (arterial baroreceptor reflex) and
skeletal muscle (exercise pressor reflex) are
known to modulate sympathetic and
parasympathetic nerve activity during
exercise. The alterations in autonomic
outflow mediate changes in heart rate and
contractility, as well as the diameter of
resistance and capacitance vessels within
various tissue beds (e.g. splanchnic region).
As a result, changes in heart rate (HR),
stroke volume (SV) and systemic vascular
resistance (SVR) mediate alterations in mean
arterial pressure (MAP) appropriate for the
intensity and modality of the exercise.


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
54 J. W. Williamson and others Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58

central command activation, respectively. These findings Functional anatomy


confirm and extend previous studies in both animals
Based on our ‘proposed’ definition of central command, a
(McIlveen et al. 2001) and humans (Gallagher et al. 2001;
network of higher brain structures (or a single structure)
Querry et al. 2001) demonstrating that central command
capable of interpreting an individual’s sense of effort and
is actively involved in baroreflex resetting during exercise.
making appropriate autonomic adjustments should be
Aside from its critical role in the actual resetting of
considered within any anatomical framework. Primary
the baroreflex stimulus–response curve, central command
regions of the cerebral cortex with the capacity for
input appears also to be responsible for the relocation
modulation of autonomic function have been carefully
of the operating point (prestimulus blood pressure) away
reviewed, and it was concluded that both the insular
from the centring point (point at which there is an equal
cortex and the infralimbic cortex, more specifically the
depressor and pressor response to a given change in
medial prefrontal cortex, were well suited for this role
blood pressure) and closer to the threshold of the cardiac
(Cechetto & Saper, 1990; Verberne & Owens, 1998).
baroreflex stimulus–response curve (see Fig. 3; Potts et al.
Efferent pathways from the insular cortex to well-
1993; Gallagher et al. 2001; Ogoh et al. 2002). This effect
recognized sites of cardiovascular control, including
of central command on the operating point appears to be
the lateral hypothalamus, rostral ventrolateral medulla
mediated via vagal withdrawal associated with increases
and nucleus of the solitary tract, have also been well
in exercise intensity (Ogoh et al. 2005). It has been shown
documented (see Fig. 4; Yasui et al. 1991). Changes in
to occur in order to allow the arterial baroreflex to adapt
heart rate and blood pressure have been reported in
to and potentially modify the increases in blood pressure
response to activation of the insular cortex in both rats
induced by activation of the exercise pressor reflex (Sheriff
(Ruggiero et al. 1987) and humans (Oppenheimer et al.
et al. 1990; Potts & Mitchell, 1998). Thus, the relocation
1992). Saper (1982) noted a convergence of autonomic
of the operating point by central command may be
and limbic connections within the rat insula (Fig. 4).
very important in preventing excessive increases in blood
There also exist reciprocal connections between the
pressure during exercise, since it places the baroreflex in a
insular cortex and the infralimbic cortex (medial prefontal
more optimal position to counter hypertensive stimuli.
region), suggesting a potential for interaction between
these regions. The medial prefrontal cortex has multiple
limbic sensory inputs and appears to have a significant
role in ‘stress-related modulation’ of sympathetic outflow
(Verberne & Owens, 1998). Taken together, the insular
cortex and medial prefrontal cortex may function in
concert or independently to interpret sensory input and
elicit appropriate autonomic adjustments. It would seem
plausible to predict that these same brain regions that alter
cardiovascular responses during non-exercise conditions
could also be activated during exercise conditions.
Therefore, given the importance of the insular and
medial prefrontal regions in overall cortical modulation
of autonomic function, human studies have focused on
assessing their possible roles in central neural regulation
of autonomic function during exercise.

Figure 3. A schematic illustration of a baroreflex Neuroimaging studies


stimulus–response curve and the resetting that occurs during
exercise, with a particular emphasis on the movement of the Studies investigating the functional anatomy of central
operating point away from the centring point and closer to the command-induced changes in regional cerebral blood flow
threshold of the curve
(rCBF) have identified a network of structures activated in
The centring point is the point at which there is an equal depressor
and pressor response to a given change in blood pressure, the the human brain. These regions include the insular cortex
operating point is the prestimulus blood pressure, threshold is the (King et al. 1999; Nowak et al. 1999, 2005; Williamson
point where no further increases in heart rate are elicited by further et al. 1999, 2001, 2002; Critchley et al. 2000) and anterior
reductions in blood pressure, and saturation is the point where no cingulate cortex or the medial prefrontal region (King
further decreases in heart rate are elicited by further increases in blood
et al. 1999; Critchley et al. 2000; Thornton et al. 2001;
pressure. This relocation of the operating point away from the
centring point and closer to the threshold of the stimulus–response Williamson et al. 2001, 2002) as well as thalamic regions
curve with increasing exercise intensity positions the baroreflex in a (King et al. 1999; Thornton et al. 2001; Williamson et al.
more optimal position to counter hypertensive stimuli. 2001, 2002). These structures appear to be activated in


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58 Central cardiovascular control 55

response to an increased perception of effort during of skeletal muscle afferent input or blood pressure
exercise when heart rate and blood pressure are elevated. elevations, a recent study compared rCBF changes during
Focusing first on the insular cortex, it is possible that volitional static handgrip alone (central command/muscle
the observed rCBF changes within the insular cortex metaboreflex) and postexercise circulatory occlusion alone
during exercise were related to the blood pressure increases (no central command/muscle metaboreflex; Williamson
(Zhang & Oppenheimer, 1997) or the activation of skeletal et al. 2003). Blood pressures between the two protocols
muscle afferents (Ichiyama et al. 2004). Although the were closely matched. The primary finding was that there
activation of muscle afferents can lead to rCBF changes were distinct regions of the insular cortex and anterior
within specific regions of the insular cortex, insular cingulate cortex activated during static handgrip exercise
activation has been reported during attempted movement by central command that were independent of muscle
in spinal cord-injured subjects when afferent feedback metaboreflex activation or blood pressure elevations (see
was absent (Nowak et al. 2005). In addition, imagined Fig. 5). More specifically, the right inferior posterior
exercise has also been shown to cause insular activation, and left inferior anterior insular regions were activated
but only when the imagined effort elicited cardiovascular to a greater extent during exercise, but not during
responses (Williamson et al. 2002). Therefore, it is likely postexercise circulatory occlusion with elevated blood
that there are different regions within the insular cortex pressure. Findings of the left inferior anterior activation are
that are responsive to skeletal muscle afferent input and consistent with previous work demonstrating a significant
to central command during exercise. In this regard, correlation between left inferior anterior activation and
numerous afferent and efferent connections to and from heart rate during exercise (Williamson et al. 1999).
the insular cortex have been identified in the rat brain Careful examination of data provided by King et al.
(Fig. 4). Of note are the primary projections from the (1999) during a brief bout of handgrip exercise identifies
insular cortex to the hypothalamus, which may identify an that the right posterior insular region was activated
important linkage between the human studies focusing on during the handgrip, but not immediately postexercise.
activation of the insular cortex and the animal studies using Critchley et al. (2000) also reported activation of the right
electrical stimulation of hypothalamic or mesencephalic posterior insular region during handgrip, as well as in
locomotor regions to evoke ‘central command’-induced response to mental stress. Insular activation during mental
cardiovascular responses. stress supports the concept of similar cortical regions
In an attempt to localize regions of the insular cortex modulating cardiovascular responses for both exercise
responding to central command that are independent and non-exercise conditions. It should be noted that

Figure 4. A schematic diagram of a


horizontal cross-section through the rat
brain, illustrating both the afferent
inputs (left) and efferent outputs (right)
to and from the insular cortex
(Adapted from Cechetto & Saper, 1990.)


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
56 J. W. Williamson and others Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58

activation of the right inferior posterior insular region Regions of the thalamus appear to be involved in the
was also reported to covary with blood pressure changes pathway from higher brain regions to midbrain areas. As
(Williamson et al. 1999; Critchley et al. 2000). noted previously, animal investigations often elicit central
With regard to a specific role of the anterior cingulate command signals via stimulation of mesencephalon and
cortex as related to central command, it is largely involved posterior hypothalamic regions (Waldrop et al. 1996).
in the discrimination of peripheral somatosensory input. Findings of thalamic activation coupled with that of
Thus, it could serve to interpret an individual’s level of higher brain centres provides some indirect evidence
central command (or effort sense). This region, defined as towards establishing a central command pathway to
the anterior cingulate cortex by both Critchley et al. (2000) brainstem structures. From human studies, activation
and Williamson et al. (2003), is included within the region of the right and left inferior thalamic regions has been
termed the medial prefrontal cortex by King et al. (1999). reported during both handgrip exercise and postexercise
These investigators reported that activation of this medial circulatory occlusion (Williamson et al. 2003). The inferior
prefrontal region during handgrip exercise was associated (or ventral) region of the thalamus activated was analogous
with cardiovascular activation. Reviews by Cechetto & to the ventroposterior region previously demonstrated to
Saper (1990) and Verberne & Owens (1998) have defined have reciprocal connections with the insular cortex (Saper,
a significant role for the medial prefontal cortex in 1982; Cechetto & Saper, 1990), which may be further
cardiovascular regulation, and this area appears to have related to baroreceptor activation (Cechetto & Saper,
a role in central command during exercise (Williamson 1990). Blood pressure changes have been show to elicit
et al. 2001, 2002, 2003). This suggests that the anterior activation in the thalamus (Cechetto & Saper, 1990; Zhang
cingulate cortex may work in conjunction with portions & Oppenheimer, 2000). Zhang & Oppenheimer (1997)
of the insular cortex as a ‘central command network’, determined that a significant portion of baroreceptor-
functioning to interpret an individual’s sense of effort and related neurones from the ventrobasal thalamus were
then eliciting appropriate autonomic adjustments to affect reciprocally connected with the posterior insula in the
cardiovascular responses. rat. Further, it has been reported that regions of the

Figure 5. Differences in brain activation from rest for static handgrip (SHG) (left) and postexercise
circulatory occlusion (PECO; middle) matched for the same mean blood pressure elevation
Coregistered single-photon-emission computed tomography (SPECT) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data
representing a transaxial slice from one subject. The coronal and sagittal MRI panels (right) show lines of orientation
for the tranasxial slice. The top and bottom of the transaxial panels correspond to an anterior and posterior
orientation, respectively. Changes in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) distribution from SPECT data were mapped
on the MRI by using an arbitrary colour scale with a positive range from 5 to 25% (from green through yellow
to red) and a negative range from −5 to −25% (from purple through dark blue to light blue). The white lines
denote the specific regions of interest assessed in this brain slice and encompass the right and left insular cortexes
for inferior anterior (ICia) and inferior posterior (ICip) regions, right and left inferior thalamic regions (THi), and
anterior cingulate cortex (AC). The image shows that during SHG there were significant increases in activation
for both anterior cingulate and insular regions that were not present during PECO. However, there is significant
activation in the thalamus and right inferior anterior insula during PECO, similar to that observed during handgrip.
(Reproduced with permission from Williamson et al. 2003.)


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58 Central cardiovascular control 57

human ventrocaudal nucleus of the thalamus are involved defining the regions involved in centrally mediated
in the integration of afferent baroreceptor information cardiovascular modulation is of critical importance
(Oppenheimer et al. 1998). When directly stimulated, in furthering our understanding of this concept and
these thalamic regions can elicit increases in heart rate and may have clinical implications related to various types
blood pressure in humans (Thornton et al. 2002). Thus, of autonomic dysfunction (e.g. emotional syncope,
regions of the human thalamus appear to have a key role white coat hypertension). Future investigations must be
in the overall regulation of blood pressure via baroreflex performed in humans to more clearly define the specific
mechanisms and probably play a role in central command- sites within these regions responsible for changes in
induced changes in baroreflex function. autonomic function and how they interact to effectively
modulate cardiovascular responses during exercise as well
Summary as during non-exercise conditions.
The neural circuitry of central command appears to
encompass regions of the insular cortex and anterior References
cingulate cortex that interact with thalamic and brainstem
structures of cardiovascular integration, although there Asmussen E, Johansen SH, Jorgensen M & Nielsen M (1965).
On the nervous factors controlling respiration and
may be other cerebral cortical regions involved. While
circulation during exercise. Experiments with curarization.
we have redefined central command to imply an ‘effort- Acta Physiol Scand 63, 343–350.
induced modulation of autonomic function’, the classically Cechetto DF & Saper CB (1990). Role of the cerebral cortex in
used terminology in reference to a parallel activation of autonomic function. In Central Regulation of Autonomic
both cardiovascular and motor regions during exercise Function, ed. Spyer M & Lowey D, pp. 208–223. Oxford
remains accurate. However, it would appear that central University Press, Oxford, UK.
command can function as a feedback system, based on Critchley HD, Corfield DR, Chandler MP, Mathias CJ & Dolan
an individual’s sense of effort or exertion, and does not RJ (2000). Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular
require a parallel motor activation to exert its influence. arousal: a functional neuroimaging investigation in humans.
Further, it is likely that the same regions of the higher brain J Physiol 523, 259–270.
involved in cardiovascular modulation during exercise Fadel PJ, Gallagher KG & Smith SA (2004). Neural mechanisms
influencing baroreflex resetting during exercise. Recent
are involved in cardiovascular modulation during non-
Research Developments in Physiology, pp. 413–448. Research
exercise conditions. Signpost.
The study of central command and its functional Gallagher K, Fadel P, Stromstad M, Ide K, Smith S, Querry R,
anatomy poses unique challenges in trying to uncouple Raven P & Secher N (2001). Effects of partial neuromuscular
the role of higher brain cardiovascular centres from those blockade on carotid baroreflex function during exercise in
of the motor centres and the influences of working skeletal humans. J Physiol 533, 861–870.
muscle afferents and blood pressure changes during Gandevia SC, Killian K, McKenzie DK, Crawford M, Allen GM,
exercise. The redundancy between central command and Gorman RB & Hales JP (1993). Respiratory sensations,
skeletal muscle afferent feedback mechanisms during cardiovascular control, kinaesthesia and transcranial
exercise may be explained, at least in part, in that stimulation during paralysis in humans. J Physiol 470,
an individual’s perception of effort can be largely 85–107.
Goodwin GM, McCloskey DI & Mitchell JH (1972).
influenced by somatomotor sensations arising from
Cardiovascular and respiratory responses to changes in
working skeletal muscle during exercise. These signals central command during isometric exercise at constant
may serve as some type of feedback to help gauge the muscle tension. J Physiol 226, 173–190.
level of required effort or central command. Further, an Ichiyama RM, Waldrop TG & Iwamoto GA (2004). Neurons in
individual’s perception of effort, independent of afferent and near insular cortex are responsive to muscular
feedback, such as the perception of effort occurring contraction and have sympathetic and/or cardiac-related
during attempted exercise with paralysis, contributes discharge. Brain Res 1008, 273–277.
importantly to the central cardiovascular command Johansson JE (1895). Ueber die Einwirkung der
response. Muskelthatigkeit auf die Athmung und die Hertzhiitigkeit.
The insular cortex and anterior cingulate cortex appear Skandinaviesches Arch Fiir Physiologie 5, 20–66.
to be capable of responding to multiple inputs. However, Kaufman MP & Forster HV (1996). Reflexes controlling
circulatory, ventilatory and airway responses to exercise.
comparisons regarding activation patterns between studies
In Handbook of Physiology, chapter 11, section 12, Exercise:
are complicated by potential species differences as well Regulation and Integration of Multiple Systems, ed. Rowell LB
as anatomical and neurophysiological variations within & Sheperd DT, pp. 381–447. American Physiological Society,
species. Human investigation is hampered somewhat in Bethesda, MD, USA.
that it is not possible to determine the specific type King AB, Menon RS, Hachinski V & Cechetto DF (1999).
of neural activity (i.e. excitatory or inhibitory) based Human forebrain activation by visceral stimuli. J Comp
upon changes in patterns of brain activity. However, Neurol 413, 572–582.


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
58 J. W. Williamson and others Exp Physiol 91.1 pp 51–58

Krogh A & Lindhard J (1913). The regulation of respiration Saper CB (1982). Convergence of autonomic and limbic
and circulation during the initial stages of muscular work. connections in the insular cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol
J Physiol 47, 112–136. 210, 163–173.
Leonard B, Mitchell JH, Mizuno M, Rube N, Saltin B & Secher Sheriff DD, O’Leary DS, Scher AM & Rowell LB (1990).
NH (1985). Partial neuromuscular blockade and Baroreflex attenuates pressor response to graded muscle
cardiovascular responses to static exercise in man. J Physiol ischemia in exercising dogs. Am J Physiol 258, H305–H310.
359, 365–379. Smith JC, Stephens DP, Winchester PK & Williamson JW
McCloskey DI & Mitchell JH (1972). Reflex cardiovascular and (1997). Facial cooling-induced bradycardia: attenuating
respiratory response originating in exercising muscle. effect of central command at exercise onset. Med Sci Sports
J Physiol 224, 173–186. Exerc 29, 320–325.
McIlveen SA, Hayes SG & Kaufman MP (2001). Both central Thornton JM, Aziz T, Schlugman D & Paterson DJ (2002).
command and exercise pressor reflex reset carotid sinus Electrical stimulation of the midbrain increases heart rate
baroreflex. Am J Physiol 280, H1454–H1463. and arterial blood pressure in awake humans. J Physiol 539,
Mitchell JH (1990). Neural control of the circulation during 615–621.
exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc 22, 141–154. Thornton JM, Guz A, Murphy K, Griffith AR, Pedersen DL,
Mitchell JH, Kaufman MP & Iwamoto GA (1983). The exercise Kardos A, Leff A, Adams L, Casadei B & Paterson DJ (2001).
pressor reflex: its cardiovascular effects, afferent mechanisms, Identification of higher brain centres that may encode the
and central pathways. Ann Rev Physiol 45, 229–242. cardiorespiratory response to exercise in humans. J Physiol
Nowak M, Holm S, Biering-Sorensen F, Secher NH & Friberg L 533, 823–836.
(2005). ‘Central command’ and insular activation during Verberne AJ & Owens NC (1998). Cortical modulation of the
attempted foot lifting in paraplegic humans. Hum Brain cardiovascular system. Prog Neurobiol 54, 149–168.
Mapp 25, 259–265. Waldrop TG, Eldridge FL, Iwamoto GA & Mitchell JH (1996).
Nowak M, Olsen KS, Law I, Holm S, Paulson OB & Secher NH Central neural control of respiration and circulation during
(1999). Command-related distribution of regional cerebral exercise. In Handbook of Physiology, chapter 9, section 12,
blood flow during attempted handgrip. J Appl Physiol 86, Exercise: Regulation and Integration of Multiple Systems, ed.
819–824. Rowell LB & Shepherd JT, pp. 333–380. American
Ogoh S, Fisher JP, Dawson EA, White MJ, Secher NH & Raven Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD, USA.
PB (2005). Autonomic nervous system influence on arterial Williamson JW, McColl R & Mathews D (2003). Evidence for
baroreflex control of heart rate during exercise in humans. central command activation of the human insular cortex
J Physiol 566, 599–611. during exercise. J Appl Physiol 94, 1726–1734.
Ogoh S, Wasmund WL, Keller DM, O-Yurvati A, Gallagher Williamson JW, McColl R, Mathews D, Ginsburg M & Mitchell
KM, Mitchell JH & Raven PB (2002). Role of central JH (1999). Activation of the insular cortex is affected by the
command in carotid baroreflex resetting in humans during intensity of exercise. J Appl Physiol 87, 1213–1219.
static exercise. J Physiol 543, 349–364. Williamson JW, McColl R, Mathews D, Mitchell JH, Raven PB
Oppenheimer SM, Gelb A, Girvin JP & Hachinski VC (1992). & Morgan WP (2001). Hypnotic manipulation of effort
Cardiovascular effects of human insular cortex stimulation. sense during dynamic exercise: cardiovascular responses and
Neurology 42, 1727–1732. brain activation. J Appl Physiol 90, 1392–1399.
Oppenheimer SM, Kulshreshtha N, Lenz FA, Zhang Z, Williamson JW, McColl R, Mathews D, Mitchell JH, Raven PB
Rowland LH & Dougherty PM (1998). Distribution of & Morgan WP (2002). Brain activation by central command
cardiovascular related cells within the human thalamus. during actual and imagined handgrip under hypnosis. J Appl
Clin Auton Res 8, 173–179. Physiol 92, 1317–1324.
Potts JT & Mitchell JH (1998). Central interaction between the Yasui Y, Breder CD, Saper CB & Cechetto DF (1991).
carotid baroreflex and the exercise pressor reflex. J Exerc Sci Autonomic responses and efferent pathways from the insular
8, 1–20. cortex in the rat. J Comp Neur 303, 355–374.
Potts JT, Shi XR & Raven PB (1993). Carotid baroreflex Zhang Z & Oppenheimer SM (1997). Characterization,
responsiveness during dynamic exercise in humans. Am J distribution and lateralization of baroreceptor-related
Physiol 265, H1928–H1938. neurons in the rat insular cortex. Brain Res 760, 243–250.
Querry RG, Smith SA, Stromstad M, Ide K, Raven PB & Secher Zhang ZH & Oppenheimer SM (2000). Baroreceptive and
NH (2001). Neural blockade during exercise augments somatosensory convergent thalamic neurons project to the
central command’s contribution to carotid baroreflex posterior insular cortex in the rat. Brain Res 861, 241–256.
resetting. Am J Physiol 280, H1635–H1644. Zuntz N & Geppert J (1886). Über die Natur der normalen
Raven PB, Fadel PJ & Smith SA (2002). The influence of central Atemreize und den ort ihrer Wirkung. Arch General Physiol
command on baroreflex resetting during exercise. Exerc 38, 337–338.
Sport Sci Rev 30, 39–44.
Raven PB, Potts JT & Shi X (1997). Baroreflex regulation of
blood pressure during dynamic exercise. Exerc Sport Sci Rev Acknowledgements
25, 365–389.
Ruggiero DA, Mraovitch S, Granata AR, Anwar M & Reis DJ J. W. Williamson was supported by an American Heart
(1987). A role of insular cortex in cardiovascular function. Association Grant-in-Aid from the Texas Affiliate, Inc. no.
J Comp Neurol 257, 189–207. 0050726Y (J.W.W.) and NIH R01 HL59145-04 (J.W.W.).


C 2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 
C 2006 The Physiological Society
Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007
New insights into central cardiovascular control during exercise in humans: a
central command update
J. W Williamson, P. J Fadel and J. H Mitchell

Exp Physiol 2006;91;51-58; originally published online Oct 20, 2005;

DOI: 10.1113/expphysiol.2005.032037
This information is current as of December 13, 2007

Updated Information including high-resolution figures, can be found at:


& Services http://ep.physoc.org/cgi/content/full/91/1/51
Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the
following collection(s):
Cardiovascular control
http://ep.physoc.org/cgi/collection/cardiovascular_control
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables)
or in its entirety can be found online at:
http://ep.physoc.org/misc/Permissions.shtml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://ep.physoc.org/misc/reprints.shtml

Downloaded from ep.physoc.org at FMRP/USP on December 13, 2007

Anda mungkin juga menyukai