Anda di halaman 1dari 10

Current Psychology

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00229-6

Multiple exposure to appearance-focused real accounts


on Instagram: Effects on body image among both genders
Silvia Casale 1 & Gabriele Gemelli 1 & Chiara Calosi 2 & Barbara Giangrasso 1 & Giulia Fioravanti 1

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
This study experimentally investigated the effects of a multiple exposure to appearance-focused real Instagram profile images on
body image among both genders. Experimental group participants were 54 young non-Instagram users (F = 50%) who were
assigned to view a set of appearance-focused real Instagram profiles for a one-week period and were compared with the same-age
non-exposed group (n = 76; F = 50%). Results showed that a multiple exposure to appearance-focused real Instagram profiles of
attractive same-sex people led to greater body dissatisfaction among women. Moreover, women in the experimental group
showed an increase in the extent to which they define themselves and their self-worth by their physical appearance. Among
men, we did not find an effect of exposure to same-sex attractive images on muscular dissatisfaction, fat dissatisfaction, and
investment in body image, although a tendency toward an increment of the scores was observed for all the dependent variables.
Implications for future research on Instagram use and body image are also discussed.

Keywords Body image . Body investment . Social media . Instagram

Negative effects from exposure to idealized media images et al. 2008) and a negative impact on body dissatisfaction was
presented in fashion magazines or on television on body dis- also reported by review studies focused on men (Blond 2008).
satisfaction have been widely documented in the past. Results Recently, there has been a growing research interest in the
from one of the first meta-analytic review in this field support- potential effect of new media (i.e. online social networks) on
ed the sociocultural perspective that mass media promulgate a body image. Instagram (Ig) is a social networking service used
slender ideal that elicits body dissatisfaction among women solely for photo and video sharing, which has risen in popu-
(Groesz et al. 2002). Subsequent meta-analyses confirmed larity, with over 600 million active users (Instagram 2016) of
that exposure to mass media depicting the thin-ideal body whom 200 millions are using it on a daily basis (Statista
may be linked to body image disturbance in women (Grabe 2017). The number of photos uploaded each day on
Instagram has increased from 80 million photos per day in
2015 (Instagram 2015) to 95 million photos per day in 2016
(Instagram 2016). Since Instagram allows individuals to care-
* Silvia Casale
fully select the personal photos they wish to post and to en-
silvia.casale@unifi.it
hance them with filtering and editing tools (see Holland and
Gabriele Gemelli Tiggemann 2016), a growing body of research has investigat-
gabriele.gemelli@stud.unifi.it ed the impact of Ig use on body image. The present study
Chiara Calosi experimentally investigated the effects of a multiple exposure
chiara.calosi@stud.unifi.it to appearance-focused real Ig profile images on body image
among both genders.
Barbara Giangrasso
barbara.giangrasso@unifi.it
Giulia Fioravanti
giulia.fioravanti@unifi.it Background
1
Department of Health Sciences, Psychology and Psychiatry Unit, Body image is a Bmultifaced psychological experience of
University of Florence, via di San Salvi 12, Florence, Italy embodiment^ that encompasses evaluative thoughts, beliefs,
2
School of Psychology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy feelings and behaviors related to one’s own physical
Curr Psychol

appearance (Cash 2004, p.1). In detailing the multifaced nature and the other main component of body image – i.e. body
of the construct, Cash (2002) firmly distinguishes between investment. This is curios for a couple of reasons. First, pro-
body evaluation (e.g., body satisfaction) and investment (e.g., cesses of social comparison are generally mentioned to ex-
the psychological importance one places on one’s appearance). plain the negative effects of media exposure—including
Body evaluation is defined as the subjective evaluation of one’s Instagram—on body satisfaction (Hendrickse et al. 2017;
body, including weight and shape (i.e. body satisfaction). Body Tiggemann et al. 2018). Social comparison theory (Festinger
image investment comprises two components: (a) motivational 1954) argues that individuals have a tendency to rate and
salience (i.e., the management of one’s own appearance for evaluate themselves through comparisons with others. Such
esthetic purposes) and (b) self-evaluative salience (i.e. the use comparisons-based evaluations might account for an incre-
of appearance as a self-defining feature). Especially body eval- ment in body investment since research has consistently
uation has been the subject of a number of correlational studies shown that comparisons with media appearance ideals is pos-
devoted to investigate if the positive association between tradi- itively associated not only with lower levels of body satisfac-
tional media exposure (e.g., idealized media images presented tion but also with higher levels of body image investment
in fashion magazines or on television) and body image dissat- (Rousseau et al. 2017). Moreover, research has shown that
isfaction can be extended to the new media (e.g., idealized more internalization of the ideal proposed by the media was
images presented on online social networks). These study significantly related to more investment in one’s own appear-
found a positive association between Instagram use and body ance (Argyrides 2013). A positive association between expo-
image concerns (e.g., Cohen et al. 2017; Hendrickse et al. sure to attractive images and body investment would also be
2017) and a few experimental research studies have also pro- consistent with communication theories that argue that repeat-
vided some initial evidence on the subject. In the first experi- ed exposure to media content leads viewers to accept media
mental study in this area, Tiggemann and Zaccardo (2015) portrayals as representations of reality (e.g., cultivation theory,
found that exposure to fitness-inspiring images but not travel see Gerbner et al. 2002; social learning theory, Brown 2002).
images leads to increased body dissatisfaction. Brown and In this case, it is believed that the media’s depiction of a thin
Tiggemann (2016) found that exposure to images of ideal leads women to see this ideal as normative and central to
celebrities and unknown attractive peers increased body attractiveness. Adopting this reality might lead not only to
dissatisfaction compared to exposure to travel images. decreased satisfaction with one’s own body but also to man-
Recently, Tiggemann et al. (2018) experimentally investigated age one’s own appearance to meet this ideal (i.e. an increment
the effect of the numbers of Blikes^ accompanying Instagram in the motivational salience component of the body invest-
images on women’s body dissatisfaction in order to explore the ment construct). Second, dysfunctional investment in body
impact of more naturalistic exposure to social media images. image seems to have more adverse consequences than body
Results showed that exposure to thin-ideal images lead to great- dissatisfaction in terms of risk for developing psychiatric
er body and facial dissatisfaction (i.e. dissatisfaction with facial symptoms. Prior research (e.g., Cash et al. 2004a; Cash et al.
features) than average images. Finally, Kleemans et al. (2018) 2004b) has shown that investment in one’s own body appear-
conducted an experiment to investigate the effect of manipulat- ance adds a significant amount of variance in explaining psy-
ed Instagram photos (i.e. improved pictures of faces and bod- chiatric symptoms beyond that already explained by body
ies) compared to original Instagram photos on body satisfac- dissatisfaction. Similarly, Jakatdar et al. (2006) found that
tion. This study found that exposure to manipulated Instagram body image investment predicted both symptoms of eating
photos leads to lower body satisfaction in comparison to expo- disorders and poor quality of life above and beyond body
sure to non-manipulated selfies. dissatisfaction. Additionally, it has already been found that
All these studies are extremely important, as taken together, women high in body investment experienced more negative
they suggest that the well-demonstrated positive association consequences following exposure to thin-ideal media than did
between traditional media exposure and body image dissatis- women that were less invested (Ip and Jarry 2008). To the best
faction (e.g., Levine and Murnen 2009; Tiggemann 2005a) of our knowledge, the association between exposure to attrac-
might be at least in part extended to new media, even after tive images via Instagram and body investment has never been
only a brief exposure to attractive images. This suggestion has explored.
been also supported by a recent study which showed that
comparisons through social media are associated with more
negative outcomes on body image than comparisons made in Limitations of the Existing Literature
person (Fardouly et al. 2017).
Although the link between exposure to attractive images As above described, the absence of empirical investigation on
via Instagram and body dissatisfaction has been confirmed the potential effect of exposure to Ig attractive images on body
through methodologically sound researches, fewer studies investment represents an important theoretical gap that needs
have paid attention to the association between Instagram use to be filled. In addition, our ability to answer the foundational
Curr Psychol

questions about the relation between Ig use and body image is and Vartanian 2016). Moreover, an extensive review (Blond
limited in several ways because of some methodological is- 2008) of experimental studies exposing men to advertisements
sues. First, experimental research in this field evaluated the or commercials featuring idealized male bodies has shown
effects of the exposure on body image immediately after one- that exposure to images of idealized male bodies has a small
time exposure to Instagram images. This might limit the inter- but statistically significant negative impact on body dissatis-
nal validity of the studies, as even if an effect on body image faction. In keeping with research on media exposure and fe-
was found, it would be possible for the conclusions at this time male body image, body investment has received less scientific
not to be extended to a later period, or, by contrast, the effect attention relative to body dissatisfaction. Most studies suggest
found could increase over time as a consequence of a multiple that men attach less importance to their own appearance than
exposure. Indeed, previous longitudinal studies (de Vries et al. women (see Grogan 2012). Moreover, it has been shown that
2015) have shown that the association between social media if the muscular ideal (i.e. the socially prescribed body size and
use and body image dissatisfaction strengthens over time. shape for men, see Thompson and Cafri 2007) is internalized,
Unfortunately, on the one hand, all previous studies imple- it is not as influential as internalization of the thin ideal is for
mented one-time exposure research design. On the other hand, females in determining commitment to defining the self in
they did not control for the previous exposure to attractive terms of weight and/or muscularity (Levine and Chapman
images via Instagram. This implies that the repeatedly detect- 2012). Taken together, previous findings advise that the effect
ed effects on body image might be due to previous exposure to of media exposure on body image concerns among men
attractive images on Instagram. Especially when investigating should not be completely neglected. However, previous find-
body investment, there is a need to control for previous ings also suggest that this effect is likely to be less strong
Instagram use as it is plausible to suppose a bidirectional effect among men relative to women.
between one’s own body investment and appearance-focused
activities on Instagram (i.e., those who attach greater impor-
tance to physical appearance will be more likely to possess an The Present Study
Instagram account and to share appearance images).
Second, as already mentioned, all previous experimental The present study aims to build upon previous research by a)
studies examined the effect of one-time exposure to Ig images. including the psychological investment in the physical appear-
This is a limit in that the limited experimental exposure is not ance as a dependent variable; b) recruiting a sample of partic-
representative of the actual exposure that people experience ipants who did not have previous contact with Instagram; c)
(Slater et al. 2017). Moreover, one-time exposure might not be considering a multiple exposure to attractive images, and d)
sufficient to put body image of a resilient person at risk. including men in the sample. On the basis of the existing
Third, self-esteem was rarely considered by previous studies research, we predicted that being repeatedly exposed to attrac-
as a control variable. Recent research has shown that under- tive images will negatively affect both body dissatisfaction
graduates with low self-esteem are increasingly likely to report and body investment among women and men. Based on the
discontent with their body size or shape (e.g. Pop 2016), re- existing literature, we hypothesized that the effect of the ex-
gardless of age and gender (see O’Dea 2012). In fact, the asso- posure on women may be more pronounced than its effect on
ciation between self-esteem and body image concerns has been males.
well-established (e.g., van den Berg et al. 2010), even if pro-
spective studies highlighted that body dissatisfaction might be
more responsible of self-esteem decrement than vice versa Method
(Tiggemann 2005b), thus suggesting a complex bidirectional
link. A strong sense of self-worth is likely to help young people Research Design
becoming more resistant to the unrealistic body ideals present-
ed in the media, acting as a moderating variable especially A quasi-experimental pre-test/post-test control group design
among men (Ricciardelli and McCabe 2001). Taken together, was implemented. The dependent variables were psychologi-
these results suggest the rationality of assessing self-esteem cal investment in the physical appearance and body dissatis-
levels when investigating the effect of the exposure to attractive faction. Self-esteem was tested as a control variable.
images on body dissatisfaction and investment.
Fourth, men were excluded by all previous experimental Participants
research on the effects of attractive images exposure on body
image. Even if body image concerns are more salient among The experimental group comprised 54 college students (50%
young women than in young men, correlational studies con- women), and the control group comprised 76 college students
sistently show that social media usage is associated with body (50% women). To determine whether the sample size was
image concerns among both women and men (see Fardouly adequate, a power analysis was conducted using G*Power
Curr Psychol

(Faul et al. 2007). Assuming a medium-sized effect (partial behaviors. Participants respond on a 5-point Likert scale from
η2 = .06) and an alpha level of .05, power analysis results 1 = completely disagree to 5 = completely agree. In the current
indicated that 24 participants (12 per group) would be neces- study, the Italian version of the ASI-R was obtained using a
sary to achieve a power of .80 (Cohen 1992), suggesting that back-translation method in which one bilingual translator
analyses could be conducted separately for men and women. translated the test from the source language to the target lan-
No significant differences were found regarding age be- guage (Italian). A second translator, without having seen the
tween women and men (respectively M ± SD = 23.22 + 1.73 original test, translated the new version of the test back to the
and M ± SD = 23.29 + 1.77, F(1,128) = 0.63, n.s.). No signifi- source language. The original and the back-translated version
cant differences were found in age neither between the exper- of the tests were then compared and judgments were made
imental women and the control women (respectively M ± about their equivalence.
SD = 22.93 ± 1.96 and M ± SD = 23.42 ± 1.53, F(1,63) = 1.30,
n.s.) nor between the experimental men and the control men Body Dissatisfaction The Contour Drawing Rating Scale
(respectively M ± SD = 23.19 ± 1.68 and M ± SD = 23.37 ± (CDRS; Thompson and Gray 1995) consists of nine female
1.85, F (1 ,63) = 0.16, n.s.). All the participants were contour drawings, and it was used to assess women’s body
Caucasians. Participation was on a voluntary basis, and the dissatisfaction. These silhouette forms present participants
participants did not receive incentives for their participation. with several body silhouettes of women ranging from very
slender to obese. The drawings were designed with detailed
Experimental Stimuli features, are of precisely graduated sizes, and are easily split at
the waist for accurate upper and lower body comparisons.
An initial pool of 120 public Instagram profiles (50% female Participants are asked to select the figure that better represents
profiles) mainly focused on the appearance of unknown their current body and the figure that better represents their
young people was tested by 10 independent raters aged 20– ideal body. Body dissatisfaction is then measured as the dif-
25 years old who were blinded to the purposes of the study. In ference between individual’s rating of their current body and
order to be considered as an appearance-based profile, the the body they would most like to possess. Higher scores indi-
profile was required to have at least 70% of photos concerning cate higher body dissatisfaction.
personal appearance (photos of themselves, their own face or In order to assess men’s body dissatisfaction, we used the
their body parts). One independent rater checked each day for silhouette forms developed by Frederick et al. (2007) that
a week that the 120 Instagram profile was updated on a daily separately assess muscularity (the Muscle Silhouette
basis mainly with images of one’s body. Five men and women Measure, MSM) and body fat level (the Fat Silhouette
evaluated 60 male and female profiles, respectively. Measure, FSM). Because men’s body dissatisfaction might
Fifteen male and 20 female Instagram profiles were select- entail the desire to be slimmer or heavier (Drewnowski and
ed from the initial pool on the basis of rating of attractiveness Yee 1987), we analyzed absolute values rather than signed
(from 1 = extremely unattractive to 5 = extremely attractive). discrepancies in order to avoid erroneous conclusions as sug-
Profiles were included as stimulus materials if all the indepen- gested by previous studies (Cash et al. 2004c). Higher scores
dent raters evaluated the owner of the profile as Bextremely indicate higher muscle and fat dissatisfaction.
attractive.^ The profiles were of people of Caucasian descent
(like all the participants of the sample). Self-Esteem Self-esteem was assessed as a control variable
through the Italian version (Prezza et al. 1997) of the 10-
Measures item Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (Rosenberg 1965). Each
item was answered on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 =
Psychological Investment The Appearance Schemas completely disagree to 5 = completely agree). Higher scores
Inventory-Revised (ASI-R; Cash 2009) is a revision of Cash indicate higher self-esteem. Pre-test Cronbach’s alpha in the
and Labarge’s (1996) original assessment of individuals’ psy- current sample was α = .89.
chological investment in their physical appearance. This 20-
item measure consists of two subscales. Self-Evaluative Procedure
Salience subscale (12 items; pre-test Cronbach’s alpha in the
current sample was α = .85) reflects the extent to which indi- Instagram is a complex multi-faceted platform in that it pro-
viduals define or measure themselves and their self-worth by vides a variety of different activities and components (i.e.,
their physical appearance, which they deem influential in their viewing others’ images; making comments; posting one’s
social and emotional experiences. Motivational Salience sub- own images; receiving comments; receiving likes; viewing
scale (8 items; pre-test Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample likes received by others). In order to control for the effect of
was α = .79) pertains to the extent to which persons attend to these potential confounding factors, the participants were re-
their appearance and engage in appearance-management quired to have never had an Instagram account.
Curr Psychol

Two research assistants approached undergraduates stu- evaluative Salience between the experimental and control
dents from the University of **** after their lessons and asked groups. No significant differences were found between the
them if they had an Instagram account. All those who experimental and control groups on self-esteem initial levels.
responded Bno^ (21.67%) were asked about their willingness Consequently, we did not control for self-esteem in the hy-
to participate in a research concerning psychological dimen- potheses testing. On the contrary, significant differences were
sions associated to Instagram use. All those who were asked found on the initial levels of ASI-R Motivational Salience
accepted to participate (n = 130). Students who accepted were with the experimental group scoring higher than the control
invited to a meeting room at the University to sing the in- group. For this reason, the effect of the experimental manipu-
formed consent and complete the questionnaires. The partici- lation was tested only on body dissatisfaction and ASI-R Self-
pants were assigned to the experimental condition or the con- evaluative Salience and the ASI-R Motivational Salience
trol group on the basis of their willingness to open an scores were added as covariate in the analysis in order to
Instagram account for a one-week period. control for the effect on the dependent variables.
An Instagram account was created for each participant of A mixed 2 × 2 ANOVA showed a significant effect of Time
the experimental group. Only the unknown attractive same x Group Interaction on body dissatisfaction (F(1,63) = 4.21,
sex profiles were added to the Instagram profile. The profile p < .05; partial η2 = .06). T-tests showed that this effect is
was private in order to guarantee that nobody but the experi- due to the experimental manipulation as a significant incre-
menter could have contacted the participant. After providing ment in body dissatisfaction was observed in the experimental
consent, participants from both the experimental and the con- group [t (26) = − 2.21, p < .05, d = 0.26) from pre-test
trol group completed the pre-test measures. Participants from (M + SD = 1.22 + 1.22) to post-test (M + SD = 1.56 + 1.21)
the experimental group were asked to attend the Instagram among women exposed to attractive images, while no signif-
profiles every evening for 1 week and to attach like to each icant changes occurred in the control group [t(37) = 0.92,
of the new-posted photographs in order to be sure that they p = .36] from pre-test (M + SD = 0.82 + 1.25) to post-test
were exposed to the attractive images. Moreover, each eve- (M + SD = 0.66 + 1.56). Results are shown in Fig. 1.
ning, the experimenter blinded to the purposes of the study Moreover, when the baseline levels of the ASI-R motivational
contacted each participant from the experimental group and subscale were added as covariate, the effect of Time x Group
asked questions about details of the new-posted photographs Interaction on body dissatisfaction remained significant
(e.g., the color of the paints). All participants attached Blike^ (F(1,63) = 4.08, p < .05; partial η2 = .06) and no significant in-
to each photograph and answered each question about the teraction effect of TimeXCovariate was obtained.
asked details. As a consequence, no participant was excluded A significant effect of Time x Group Interaction on ASI-R
from the statistical analyses as the multiple exposure to the Self-evaluative Salience was also found (F(1,63) = 7.75,
attractive images was guaranteed. p < .01; partial η2 = .11). When the baseline levels of the
The survey was re-administered after 8 days to participants ASI-R motivational subscale were added as covariate, the ef-
to both the experimental and the control group. The study was fect of Time x Group Interaction on ASI-R Self-evaluative
approved by the Director of the Department of Psychology at Salience remained significant (F(1,63) = 7.64 p < .01; partial
the University of ****. η 2 = .11) and no significant interaction effect of
TimeXCovariate was obtained. T-tests showed a trend toward
Data Analyses significance [t (26) = − 1.90, p = .0.69, d = 0.14) in Self-
evaluative Salience in the experimental group from pre-test
As the first step, the baseline equivalence of the experimental (M + SD = 38.74 + 8.03) to post-test (M + SD =
and control groups was tested using one-way ANOVA. As the 39.85 + 7.97). However, we also found a significant decre-
second step, we analyzed the experimental manipulation ef- ment [t(37) = 2.42, p < .05, d = 0.17] from pre-test (M + SD =
fect conducting a Mixed 2 × 2 ANOVA with Time (pre- and 36.87 + 7.96) to post-test (M + SD = 35.34 + 7.04) among
post-test) as within factor and Group (experimental and con- those who were not exposed to attractive images. Results are
trol) as between factor for each dependent variable. All anal- shown in Fig. 2.
yses were conducted separately for women and men.
Men

Results As shown in Table 2, no significant differences were found


between the experimental and control groups on the initial
Women levels of body dissatisfaction, ASI-R Self-evaluative
Salience, and ASI-R Motivational Salience.
As shown in Table 1, no significant differences were found on Experimental manipulation effect was tested by performing
the initial levels of body dissatisfaction and ASI-R Self- a series of Mixed 2 × 2 ANOVA with Time (pre- and post-test)
Curr Psychol

Table 1 Mean Comparisons


(One-Way ANOVA) on the initial Variable Experimental group (n = 27) Control group (n = 38) F(1,63) p η2
levels of study variables between M + SD M + SD
the experimental and control
groups among women Control variable
Self-esteem 30.55 + 4.54 29.21 + 6.21 0.92 .34 –
Dependent variables
Body dissatisfaction 1.22 + 1.22 0.81 + 1.25 1.70 .20 –
ASI - Self-Evaluative 38.74 + 8.03 36.86 + 7.96 0.87 .35 –
Salience
ASI - Motivational 28.29 + 4.66 25.90 + 4.73 4.26* .04 .03
Salience

as a within factor and Group (experimental and control) as a effect over the time rather than as immediate response after the
between factor. As shown in Table 3, no significant interaction manipulation.
effect was obtained for the dependent variables among men. Results of this study also show a tendency toward an in-
crement of the ASI-R Self-evaluative Salience scores, with
women exposed to attractive images showing an increase in
the extent to which they define themselves and their self-
Discussion worth by their physical appearance. This is particularly impor-
tant as the ASI-R Self-evaluative factor revealed more dys-
The present study aimed to extend the experimental investi- functional nature relative to the ASI-R Motivational Salience
gation of Instagram use and body image by a) including body factor with respect to a variety of outcomes (Jakatdar et al.
investment, b) considering a multiple exposure to attractive 2006). However, we also noted that ASI-R Self-evaluative
images and controlling for previous Instagram use, and c) scores showed significant decrement in the control group.
including men. We also aimed to provide more ecological Given that Instagram use is widespread among young people,
findings in that the participants were exposed to actual the refusal to open an Instagram account might be considered
Instagram accounts. As the first finding, our study confirmed as an uncommon behavior. One possible explanation is that
all the previous experimental results (e.g., Brown and women refusing to open an Instagram account and reporting
Tiggemann 2016; Tiggemann and Zaccardo 2015) on the ef- lower body investment on the post-test scores may have been
fect of attractive images via Instagram on body dissatisfaction defensive in an effort to maintain and protect self-esteem do-
levels among women. Previous findings were confirmed but mains. However, we recognize that this is no more than a
were also extended as we controlled for the previous exposure speculation and further research is needed.
to attractive images via Instagram by recruiting people who We found that both women and men of the control group
never had an Instagram account. Moreover, previous findings obtained lower pre-test scores in all the measures, even if the
were extended as we found evidence for the existence of the statistical significance was reached only for the ASI-R

Fig. 1 Effect of Time x Group


Interaction on body
dissatisfaction among women
Curr Psychol

Fig. 2 Effect of Time x Group


Interaction on ASI-R Self-evalu-
ative Salience among women

Motivational Salience factor among women. In the current regarding the lower salience on body image among men rela-
study, the subjects were assigned to the control group on the tive to women, which is also the reason why none of the
basis of their unwillingness to open an Instagram account and previous experimental research in this field included men.
vice versa (the experimental group comprised persons willing However, the score increment in all the measures at the post-
to open the account with no incentives given). This suggests test suggests that this population should not be neglected. It is
that those who open an Instagram account might already have possible that because of the lower salience of body image,
concerns about body image to some extent, and as a conse- more exposure is needed in order to detect an effect on men.
quence, they might be more vulnerable to an increment in It is also possible that male population is less homogeneous
body dissatisfaction and/or body investment. Given that the with regard to body dissatisfaction and/or body investment
body dissatisfaction/investment scores were higher in the ex- relative to women.
perimental group, the findings from the present study seems to The present study has a number of limitations, which need
confirm this possibility. However, one of the few longitudinal to be considered. The first main limitation has been already
studies in this field (e.g., de Vries et al. 2015) has shown that mentioned, and it is related to the fact that the random assign-
body dissatisfaction did not predict social media usage, sug- ment of subjects to the groups was not possible. As a conse-
gesting that social media negatively impacts people’s body quence, the external validity of the present study is threatened
image, rather than social media being sought out by those with by the fact that the findings obtained should be only general-
high levels of body dissatisfaction. ized to those who are willing to open an Instagram account.
We did not find an effect of the exposure to same-sex at- Even if the majority of the studies report that Instagram is very
tractive images on any of the dependent variables among men widespread among young people (e.g., Perrin et al. 2015), the
even if a tendency to an increment in the scores was observed current study shows that some of them might not be interested
for all the dependent variables. One possible explanation is at all in opening an Instagram account. Future research should
provided by the huge amount of empirical evidence available give a closer look to this not-interested small population.

Table 2 Mean Comparisons


(One-Way ANOVA) on initial Variable Experimental group (n = 27) Control group (n = 38) F(1,63) p
levels of study variables between M + SD M + SD
the experimental and control
groups among men Control variable
Self-esteem 30.40 + 5.30 30.97 + 5.38 0.18 .68
Dependent variables
Muscular dissatisfaction 1.44 + 1.12 1.18 + 1.09 0.88 .35
Fat dissatisfaction 0.22 + 1.25 0.18 + 1.03 0.02 .89
ASI- Self-Evaluative 34.74 + 6.87 32.94 + 8.23 0.86 .36
Salience
ASI- Motivational 26.96 + 5.91 25.76 + 5.10 0.76 .38
Salience
Curr Psychol

Table 3 Effect of experimental manipulation among men has been widely criticized (e.g., Gardner et al. 1998) for the
Measures Pre-test M + SD Post-test M + SD F(1,63) p finite number of drawings and for the restriction of range.
Despite these limitations, the current study contributes the
Muscular Dissatisfaction growing body of literature by highlighting that prolonged ex-
Experimental group 1.44 + 1.12 1.74 + 1.25 1.91 .17 posure to appearance-based Instagram profiles of very attrac-
Control group 1.18 + 1.08 1.18 + 1.13 tive same-sex people might not only increase body dissatis-
Fat Dissatisfaction faction but also body investment, at least among women.
Experimental group 0.22 + 1.25 0.22 + 1.55 0.57 .45 Future research should pay more attention to this body image
Control group 0.18 + 1.03 0.05 + .98 component as a large body of research has demonstrated its
ASI-R Self-Evaluative Salience negative impact on psychological well-being and on vulnera-
Experimental group 34.74 + 6.87 35.66 + 7.82 0.77 .38 bility for eating disorder development (Cash et al. 2004a;
Control group 32.94 + 8.23 32.89 + 7.97 Cash et al. 2004b).
ASI-R Motivational Salience Despite the repeatedly demonstrated effect of Instagram
Experimental group 26.96 + 5.91 27.85 + 4.79 2.89 .09 use on body image concerns, early adolescent girls tend to
Control group 25.76 + 5.10 24.92 + 5.99 minimize the influence of social media on body image
(Burnette et al. 2017). At the practical level, this suggests that
knowledge and awareness about the negative effects of
appearance-focused activities on Instagram should be a focus
Second, participants were asked to view the Instagram pro- of preventive intervention, especially among young women.
files in a way that they probably would not do in a naturalistic
setting. We do not know whether experimental group partici- Data Availability The dataset analyzed during the current study is not yet
pants would have been interested to follow appearance-based publicly available because the authors are waiting for the consent from 7
participants to public the data on an open platform. However, the data are
Instagram profiles of very attractive same-sex people. Indeed, available from the corresponding author on request from the Editor/
the social comparison theory (Festinger 1954) argued that reviewers.
people seek out comparison with others perceived as similar
to themselves, and we did not control for similarity perception Compliance with Ethical Standards
in the current study. However, Brown and Tiggemann (2016)
found no differences on body dissatisfaction between women Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
exposed to peer images and women exposed to celebrity im- interest.
ages. In general, a large body of research documented the
Ethical Approval BAll procedures performed in studies involving hu-
effect of exposure to thin fashion images on body image con- man participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the
cerns. Thus, it is possible that different mediators beyond so- institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964
cial comparison might explain the association between expo- Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
sure to celebrity images and an increase in body dissatisfac- standards.^
tion. Third, the sample consisted of college Caucasian stu-
dents, and thus, the results may not be generalizable to other
populations. For example, adolescents might be more vulner-
able to being influenced by media images (e.g., Borzekowski
et al. 2000). Fourth, we did not control for the actual exposure References
that participants had on Instagram imagery (i.e. we only de-
Argyrides, M. (2013). Mass media, feelings of attractiveness, investment
fined the minimum attendance), and some participants might in body image and disorder eating in Cyprus. Mediterranean e-
have spent more time than others. Moreover, recent experi- journal of Communication and Media, 2, 1–8.
mental research (Kleemans et al. 2018) has shown that expo- Blond, A. (2008). Impacts of exposure to images of ideal bodies on male
sure to manipulated Instagram photos led to lower body satis- body dissatisfaction: A review. Body Image, 5, 244–250.
faction than original photos. However, as all the previous ex- Borzekowski, D. L. G., Robinson, T. N., & Killen, J. D. (2000). Dose the
camera adds 10 pounds? Media use, perceived importance of ap-
perimental research in this field, Kleemans and colleagues pearance and weight concerns among teenage girls. Journal of
(Kleemans et al. 2018) used a one-time exposure design, thus Adolescent Health, 26, 36–41.
not giving information about the stability of the effect. As a Brown, J. D. (2002). Mass media influences on sexuality. Journal of Sex
consequence, it would be interesting to replicate the study by Research, 39, 42–45.
comparing original and manipulated photos. Fifthly, data for Brown, Z. A., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). Attractive celebrity and peer
images on Instagram: Effect on women's mood and body image.
men and women were analysed separately, and thus compar- Body Image, 19, 37–43.
isons across gender cannot be made. Finally, we used the Burnette, B. C., Kwitowski, M. A., & Mazzeo, S. E. (2017). BI don’t need
silhouette methodology to assess body dissatisfaction, which people to tell me I’m pretty on social media:^ a qualitative study of
Curr Psychol

social media and body image in early adolescent girls. Body Image, satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of
23, 114–125. Eating Disorders, 31, 1–16.
Cash, T. F. (2002). Cognitive behavioral perspectives on body image. In Grogan, S. (2012). Body image development in adulthood. In T. F. Cash
T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: An handbook of & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of science, practice,
theory, research and clinical practice (pp. 38–46). New York: and prevention (pp. 93–100). New York: The Guilford Press.
Guilford Press. Hendrickse, J. M. A., Arpan, L. M., Clayton, R. B., & Ridgway, J. L.
Cash, T. F. (2004). Body image: Past, present and future. Body Image: An (2017). Instagram and college women’s body image: Investigating
International Journal of Research, 1, 1–5. the roles of appearance-related comparisons and intrasexual compe-
Cash, T. F. (2009). Brief manual for the appearance schemas inventory- tition. Computers in Human Behavior, 74, 92–100.
revised. Available from the author at www.body-images.com. Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact
Cash, T. F., & Labarge, A. S. (1996). Development of the appearance of the use of social networking sites on body image and disorder
schemas inventory: A new cognitive body image assessment. eating outcomes. Body Image, 17, 100–110.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 37–50. Instagram (2015). Instagram statistics. Retrieved from https://www.
Cash, T. F., Melnik, S. E., & Hraboski, J. I. (2004a). The assessment of instagram.com/press/.
body image investment: An extensive revision of the appearance Instagram (2016). Instagram statistics. Retrieved from https://www.
schemas inventory. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, instagram.com/press/
35, 305–316. Ip, K., & Jarry, J. L. (2008). Investment in body image for self-definition
Cash, T. F., Phillips, K. A., Santos, M. T., & Hraboski, J. I. (2004b). results in greater vulnerability to the thin media than does invest-
Measuring negative body image: Validation of the body image dis- ment in appearance management. Body Image, 5, 59–69.
turbance questionnaire in a nonclinical population. Body Image, 1, Jakatdar, T. A., Cash, T. F., & Engle, E. K. (2006). Body image thought
363–372. processes: The development and initial validation of the assessment
Cash, T. F., Morrow, J. A., Hrabosky, J. I., & Perry, A. A. (2004c). How of body-image cognitive distortions. Body Image, 3, 325–333.
has body image changed? A cross-sectional investigation of college Kleemans, M., Daalmans, S., Carbaat, I., & Anschütz, D. (2018). Picture
women and men from 1983 to 2001. Journal of Consulting and perfect: The direct effect of manipulated Instagram photos on body
Clinical Psychology, 72, 1081–1089. image in adolescent girls. Media Psychology, 21, 93–110.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
Levine, M. P., & Chapman, K. (2012). Media influences on body image.
Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2017). The relationship be-
In T. F. Cash & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of
tween Facebook and Instagram appearance-focused activities and
science, practice, and prevention (pp. 101–109). New York: The
body image concerns in young women. Body Image, 23, 183–187.
Guilford Press.
de Vries, D. A., Peter, J., de Graaf, H., & Nikken, P. (2015). Adolescents’
Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2009). BEverybody knows that mass
social network site use, peer appearance-related feedback, and body
media are/are not (pick one) a cause of eating disorders^: A critical
dissatisfaction: Testing a mediation model. Journal of Youth and
review of evidence for a causal link between media, negative body
Adolescence, 45, 211–224.
image, and disorder eating in women. Journal of Social and Clinical
Drewnowski, A., & Yee, D. K. (1987). Men and body image: Are males
Psychology, 28, 9–42.
satisfied with their body weight? Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 626–
O’Dea, J. A. (2012). Body image and self-esteem. Encyclopedia of Body
634.
Image and Human Appearance, 141–147.
Fardouly, J., & Vartanian, L. R. (2016). Social media and body image
concerns: Current research and future directions. Current Opinion in Perrin, A., Duggan, M., Rainie, L., Smith, A., Greenwood, S., Porteus,
Psychology, 9, 1–5. M., et al. (2015). Social media usage: 2005–2015. Pew Research
Fardouly, J., Pinkus, R. T., & Vartanian, L. R. (2017). The impact of Center.
appearance comparisons made thorugh social media, traditional me- Pop, C. (2016). Self-esteem and body image perception in a sample of
dia, and in person in women’s everyday lives. Body Image, 20, 31– university students. Eurasian Journal of Educational Research,
39. 16(64), 31–44.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A Prezza, M., Trombaccia, F. R., & Armento, L. (1997). La scala
flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, dell'autostima di Rosenberg: Traduzione e validazione Italiana
and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. [The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Italian translation and valida-
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human tion]. Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata, 223, 35–44.
Relations, 7, 117–140. Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2001). Self-esteem and negative
Frederick, D. A., Buchanan, G. M., Sadehgi-Azar, L., Peplau, L. A., affect as moderators of sociocultural influences on body dissatisfac-
Haselton, M. G., Berezovskaya, A., & Lipinski, R. E. (2007). tion, strategies to decrease weight, and strategies to increase muscles
Desiring the muscular ideal: Men's body satisfaction in the United among adolescent boys and girls. Sex Roles, 44, 189–207.
States, Ukraine, and Ghana. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 8, Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescent self-image. Princeton:
103–117. Princeton University Press.
Gardner, R. M., Friedman, B. N., & Jackson, N. A. (1998). Rousseau, A., Gamble, H., & Eggermont, S. (2017). The role of appear-
Methodological concerns when using silhouettes to measure body ance schematicity in the internalization of media appearance ideals:
image. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 387–395. A panel study of preadolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 60, 27–38.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., & Morgan, M. (2002). Growing up with televi- Slater, A., Varsani, N., & Diedrichs, P. C. (2017). #fitspo or
sion: Cultivation processes. In J. Bryant & D. Zillman (Eds.), Media #loveyourself? The impact of fitspiration and self-compassion
effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 43–67). Instagram images on women’s body image, self-compassion, and
Mahwah: Erlbaum. mood. Body Image, 22, 87–96.
Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in Statista (2017). Instagram daily active users 2017. Retrieved from https://
body image concerns among women: A meta-analysis of experi- www.statista.com/statistics/657823/number-of-daily-active-
mental and correlational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 460– instagram-users
476. Thompson, J. K., & Cafri, J. (2007). The muscular ideal: Psychological,
Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of social, and medical perspectives. Washington, DC: American
experimental presentation of thin media images on body Psychological Association.
Curr Psychol

Thompson, M. A., & Gray, J. J. (1995). Development and validation of a Tiggemann, M., Hayden, S., Brown, Z., & Veldhuis, J. (2018). The effect
new body-image assessment scale. Journal of Personality of Instagram Blikes^ on women’s social comparison and body dis-
Assessment, 64, 258–269. satisfaction. Body Image, 26, 90–97.
Tiggemann, M. (2005a). Television and adolescent body image: The role van den Berg, P. A., Mond, J., Eisenberg, M., Ackard, D., & Neumark-
of program content and viewing motivation. Journal of Social and Sztainer, D. (2010). The link between body dissatisfaction and self-
Clinical Psychology, 24, 361–381. esteem in adolescents: Similarities across gender, age, weight status,
Tiggemann, M. (2005b). Body dissatisfaction and adolescent self-esteem: race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Journal of Adolescent
Prospective findings. Body Image, 2, 129–135. Health, 47, 290–296.
Tiggemann, M., & Zaccardo, M. (2015). BExercise to be fit, not skinny^:
The effect of fit inspiration imagery on women’s body image. Body
Image, 15, 61–67. Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai