Anda di halaman 1dari 5

Society for Music Theory

Review: Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation


Author(s): Elizabeth Hellmuth Margulis
Source: Music Theory Spectrum , Vol. 34, No. 2 (Fall 2012), pp. 145-148
Published by: Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Music Theory
Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/mts.2012.34.2.145

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Oxford University Press and Society for Music Theory are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Music Theory Spectrum

This content downloaded from


129.15.64.254 on Tue, 24 Sep 2019 13:35:34 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
reviews 145

Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. underspecified, even in extremely sophisticated and satisfying
By David Huron. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, music analyses.5 Huron, by contrast, is careful to define and
2006, xii þ 462 pages. ground all of his terms in concrete, real-world phenomena:
human behavioral tendencies and perceptions, or quantifiable
reviewed by elizabeth hellmuth margulis tendencies in particular music corpora. It is easy to argue, in
some cases, with the entities that have been selected for tallying,
or the kind of behavior that has been predicted, but this ease is
one of the work’s essential virtues: because Huron’s claims are
stated so clearly, the reader always knows what is being con-
In his farewell address as President of the Society for Music tended, and what methods and logic led to it. The inner
Theory, Justin London articulated a vision for the future of workings of his argument, in other words, are unusually trans-
music theory, arguing that work within the field should aim to parent for a monograph in music theory. This exposure makes it
be accessible and meaningful to people beyond the discipline.1 easier to reach in and find a faulty gear, but this is the kind of
He framed it, partially, as a matter of survival in an economic ease that advances a field. People can confront Huron’s claims,
downturn. David Huron’s Sweet Anticipation: Music and the make specific counterclaims, and collect evidence that confirms
Psychology of Expectation makes such interdisciplinarity seem less or disconfirms them. Over time, this process should result in
a matter of economic and more a matter of intellectual survival. faster and further advancement than might be expected when
Winner of the SMT’s Wallace Berry Award in 2007, it has been relatively opaque and disparate theoretical systems talk past one
reviewed by publications that reach as far outside the field as The another.
Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism,2 Philosophical Psychology,3 Huron’s basic theory, the ITPRA theory, schematized in
and Nature.4 In examining the mechanisms of expectation Example 1, lays out the time course and emotional correlates of
during music listening, Huron does not stop at the traditional the various stages involved in forming and sustaining expecta-
axioms and assumptions that form the conventional boundaries tions, as well as reacting after they have been fulfilled or denied.
of music theory, but looks beyond them, further and further, The theory outlines five psychological responses to expectation,
until he is in the realm of psychology and even biology. This two of which are pre-outcome. The first, the imagination
truly singular book starts by exploring an old and central concept response, plays a key role in motivation and planning, by enabling
in music theory—expectation—and by relentlessly pursuing this people to envision future scenarios and feel (more than just think
concept ends up opening fundamental questions about aesthetic about) their consequences. The second, the tension response,
and emotional experience, human learning and memory, and entails motorically and perceptually preparing for an imminent
communication and cross-culturalism. In the process, Huron anticipated event—intensifying arousal and attention in ways
does the discipline of music theory a great service by taking its that often manifest as stress. The remaining three responses are
materials and showing how they relate to larger intellectual post-outcome, occurring after the expected event has transpired
discourses in society. And if music theory is to improve the ratio or been withheld. The prediction response, conditioned by the
between its export of ideas to other disciplines and its import of obvious biological advantage of a capacity for accurate predic-
ideas from them, it will need more books such as this one. tions, ensures that a person experiences a positively valenced
The scope of Huron’s accomplishments has its roots in the glow after an expected event, and a negatively valenced pang
honesty and directness with which he articulates the intellectual after an unexpected one. The reaction response is fast and auto-
foundations of his project. ‘‘Any theory of musical expectation,’’ matic, well insulated from learning and habituation, making it
Huron concedes, ‘‘necessarily presupposes a general theory of possible, for example, to have a startle response to a loud chord
expectation’’ (3). Although it might easily be recognized that every before consciously realizing that it poses no threat. The appraisal
musical theory presupposes one or more such general theories, response, on the other hand, unfolds more slowly, and may
these dependencies are rarely made explicit. Huron not only include several parts, as the true consequences of a given event
acknowledges this relationship in the opening pages of his book, are considered.
but also works straightaway to develop the general theory that will Although couched in terms of biology, this taxonomy has real
ground his account of musical expectations. The resultant product, repercussions for musical experience. For one thing, it can help
the ITPRA theory, systematically links elements of musical explain ‘‘Wittgenstein’s puzzle’’—the tendency for ‘‘surprising’’
experience as diverse as chills, giggles, and boredom to some of events, such as a deceptive cadence, to retain their affective
their underlying causes within a general psychology of expectation. charge even when they are entirely predictable (because, for
A recent article in this journal examines the way in which example, a listener has already heard an album twenty times). In
usages of the term ‘‘expectation’’ are often ontologically Huron’s terms, the fast reaction response, not modulated by
learning or experience, continues to register surprise even as the
1 London (2009).
appraisal response affirms the cadence’s predictability.
2 Benjamin (2007).
3 Schmuckler (2008).
4 Janata (2006). 5 Margulis (2007).

This content downloaded from


129.15.64.254 on Tue, 24 Sep 2019 13:35:34 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
146 music theory spectrum 34 (2012)

And although ITPRA provides the supporting framework,


Huron raises questions and explores issues that move far beyond
it, into the subtle characteristics of expectation that shape re-
sponses to musical style and individual pieces, that is, into the
realm of analysis. He looks at how schemas can ‘‘partition’’ ex-
pectations, ensuring that certain progressions (V–IV, for
example) can sound surprising in one repertory, but predictable
in another (say, reggae). Or, to take an example that follows
inevitably from the book’s title, he picks apart the familiar figure
of the anticipation, chronicling moment by moment how it
funnels expectations for a resolution onto the next beat, and how
this funneling process is weakened by a variant anticipation
figure that lacks the dotted rhythm (245–47). He then proceeds example 1. Huron’s Figure 1.1 (p. 17). Reprinted by permission of
to illustrate how this same temporal funneling can be achieved The MIT Press.
by pre-downbeat fills in pop music, ‘‘the hypermetric equivalent
of the melodic anticipation’’ (249). Throughout the book, rather, seems to set up a framework for the biological instanti-
Huron identifies these kinds of cross-genre musical gambits: ation of the cognitive processes Huron wants to discuss, and so
standard ways in which composers and performers take advan- long as the perceptual tendencies he identifies in more detailed
tage of perceptual tendencies to structure musical experience and musical analyses do not violate this governing framework, he is
affect. By retaining a broad perspective, Huron is able to find willing to discuss processes that lack a direct biological correlate.
shared roots between the classical anticipation and the pop fill, This leaves room for future work to further pursue the kind
or between expectation-thwarting strategies in composers as of disentangling of the concept of expectation suggested by
diverse as Wagner, Schoenberg, and Stravinsky. Margulis8 and implied throughout Sweet Anticipation. What
So Huron’s account of musical expectations goes well beyond Sweet Anticipation makes clear is that expectations unfold
physiology, into the realm of detailed analytic work. Yet he dynamically in time, can take as their object many different
preserves the biological orientation almost to the last, attributing kinds of musical entities (from a note to a theme to a time
a ‘‘positive limbic reward’’ to the anticipation figure’s heightened point), and that these expectations choreograph the experience
prediction effect (246). People have questioned the degree to of musical affect.
which physiology and neuroscience, in their current form, can Sweet Anticipation’s central claim is that because the essential
explain complex human behavior.6 Larissa MacFarquhar, for function of human cognition is prediction, music processing, as
example, chronicled the attempts of the husband-wife team of an action of human cognition, is fundamentally expectational.
philosopher/neuroscientists Pat and Paul Churchland to converse What remains for music theorists, it seems to me, is either to
about their behavior in ‘‘more advanced,’’ neuroscientific terms: argue that music listening is not by nature expectational, and
One afternoon recently, Paul says, he was home making dinner
provide an alternative account, or to work the notion of expec-
when Pat burst in the door, having come straight from a frustrating tation deeply into theories and analyses. This was brilliantly
faculty meeting. ‘‘She said, ‘Paul, don’t speak to me, my serotonin assayed by David Lewin, but since the publication of his 1986
levels have hit bottom, my brain is awash in glutocorticoids, my article, there have been remarkable methodological advance-
blood vessels are full of adrenaline, and if it weren’t for my endog- ments in psychology and neuroscience, providing new insight
enous opiates I’d have driven the car into a tree on the way home. My into the nature of expectation and perception, and the promise
dopamine levels need lifting. Pour me a Chardonnay, and I’ll be
for even more insight in the future.9 It would be a shame if music
down in a minute.’ ’’7
analysts outside the subarea of music cognition did not seek to
Can physiology and biology account directly for particular weave these ideas into their accounts of musical listening.
behavioral tendencies, or do they set up the conditions and It would also be a shame if psychologists did not take Huron’s
parameters for a system of cognition better explained (at least for ideas into the laboratory. There are a host of methodologies that
now) by experimental psychology? This issue with levels of could be brought to the questions and theories raised, from
explanation seems like it might plague Sweet Anticipation, given neuroimaging to behavioral tests, but Sweet Anticipation itself
the highly physiological thrust of the ITPRA theory presented relies heavily on corpus analysis. Huron is the inventor of
in the opening pages. But Huron is quite comfortable entering Humdrum, one of the first software environments that allows
into nuanced discussions of, for example, the relationship users to encode data from musical scores in a standardized form,
between what- and when-type expectations (expectations about such that these data can be queried by standard commands or
what might happen, versus when it might occur), without new, user-programmed ones. Once a sufficiently large set of
framing the discussion in terms of ITPRA. The basic theory, pieces has been encoded into the appropriate format, the system

6 Harley (2004); Coltheart (2006). 8 Margulis (2007).


7 MacFarquhar (2007, 69). 9 Lewin (1986).

This content downloaded from


129.15.64.254 on Tue, 24 Sep 2019 13:35:34 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
reviews 147

can be used to provide quantitative answers to questions that of tones.11 Huron makes the case that listeners pervasively track
previously have only been speculated about: are dissonances more the statistical properties of music, and use that implicit knowl-
common in a particular metric position in a particular repertory? edge—knowledge unavailable to conscious awareness—to form
Are the transition probabilities from note to note more constrained expectations.
in, say, Bach than in Wagner? Does variety in the usage of But, to adopt the perspective of expectational accounts of
particular rhythmic values increase with historical time? musical affect, if composers aim to generate expectations and
The corpus analyses Huron relies on to advance his argument then thwart them for aesthetic effect, and listeners form ex-
are methodologically innovative and appealingly commonsen- pectations based on what composers do, then a rapid devaluation
sical. Most musicians have, at one point or another, probably should occur, whereby composers try new things, listeners come
intuited that a certain trend might hold across a particular body to expect them, and composers must try something even newer.
of repertory, but lacked sufficient obsessiveness or time to tally Indeed, something akin to this process has been theorized to
up the data by hand, paging through scores and penciling down account for stylistic evolution across time; however, this cannot
notes; refreshingly, Huron takes such questions, questions that be the whole story. People continue to experience affective re-
were previously only hypothetically answerable, and provides sponses to Beethoven even when they have become highly
concrete answers expressed in quantitative terms. familiar with his ‘‘bag of tricks.’’ Many of these responses, no
Naturally, there can be reservations about the way a certain doubt, have their origins in mechanisms other than expectation,
question was translated into quantifiable terms, and the gener- but the privileged place expectation has enjoyed in the study of
alizability to larger pools of repertory from the dataset available musical affect suggests that many theorists believe that expec-
in Humdrum, but at the very least, these findings provide tation continues to play an important role even in responses to
a tractable place to begin a debate about what are often very highly familiar music.
messy, culturally embedded phenomena. For example, Huron How is this possible? Huron believes that listeners, despite
and Paul von Hippel compared all forty-two twelve-tone rows possessing impressive statistics-tracking skills, use imperfect
used by Schoenberg with 200 randomly generated rows, illus- heuristics to deal with musical input, presumably to speed
trating that ‘‘tonal correlation,’’ as assessed by the Krumhansl- processing and increase efficiency. For example, although corpus
Schmuckler key estimation method, was significantly higher in analyses showed that melodies exhibit regression to the mean
randomly selected rows than in the ones used by Schoenberg (a tendency to return to a mean or median pitch when
(340). Huron concludes that Schoenberg actively sought to diverging from it above or below), a preponderance of down-
avoid creating tonal implications, and proposes that his work is ward over upward steps, and arch shapes (beginning rises,
better described not simply as atonal, but as intentionally con- ending descents), behavioral experiments suggest that listeners
tratonal (343). Tonally oriented listeners, Huron reasons, lack- expect post-skip reversal (a change of direction after a leap),
ing the reliable pleasure of successful prediction effects, will step inertia (small intervals following small intervals), and late
experience ‘‘confusion and discomfort’’ (344). As elsewhere in phrase declination (melodic descents at phrase ends). All of
the book, behavioral studies could pursue this argument further, these expectations capture something real about musical ten-
by, say, ‘‘transposing’’ some Schoenberg works to rows with dencies, but simplify them such that they are a sufficiently good
higher tonal correlation scores and asking participants to rank rather than perfect estimation. This slight mismatch between
the confusion associated with modified and original excerpts. expectational heuristics and statistical realities could account,
Huron makes the same corpus-based argument for a contra- in part, for the lack of rapid expiration in expectation-based
metricism in Stravinsky, illustrating that ‘‘Stravinsky’s accents affect. Another possible explanation is the distinction Huron
are forty times more difficult to predict than a purely random proposes between the informationally encapsulated reaction
sequence of accented moments generated using a normative response and the slower appraisal response. ( Jamshed Bharucha
metrical distribution’’ (346). Huron also acknowledges the way previously proposed a related distinction between expectations
that expertise in these repertories can cause listeners to build that are veridical—related to the specific piece—and sche-
different sets of expectations, accounting for the experiential gulf matic—related to more general tendencies within a style.12)
between aficionados and neophytes. Furthermore, it is important to note that, according to Huron’s
Huron’s focus on corpus analysis, or the systematic exami- model, affect stems not merely from the violation of expecta-
nation of what actually happens in various repertories, is rooted tion, but also from its generation (the imagination and tension
in his commitment to the theory of statistical learning. Jenny R. responses). The relationship between expectation generation
Saffran et al. showed that eight-month-old infants were able to and affect has been generally neglected in favor of studies
track transition probabilities (the likelihood of one element related to expectation violation, and remains a promising area
proceeding to the next) in strings of otherwise undifferentiated for future research.
syllables, theoretically explaining how they are able to segment Huron examines several different types of affective responses
the speech stream in the absence of explicit instruction.10 In to surprise: frisson (chills), laughter, and awe. He theorizes that
a later study Saffran et al. demonstrated the same effect for series
11 Saffran et al. (1999).
10 Saffran et al. (1996). 12 Bharucha (1987).

This content downloaded from


129.15.64.254 on Tue, 24 Sep 2019 13:35:34 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
148 music theory spectrum 34 (2012)

these stem, in turn, from three separate types of fear-related Janata, Petr. 2006. ‘‘Hitting the Right Note.’’ Review of Sweet
behavioral responses: fight (linked to chills via hair-bristling/ Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation by David
piloerection), flight (linked to laughter via panting), and freeze Huron. Nature 443 (7107): 29–30.
(linked to awe via breath-holding). In addition to connecting Lewin, David. 1986. ‘‘Music Theory, Phenomenology, and
each of these reaction responses to a biological origin, Huron Modes of Perception.’’ Music Perception 3 (4): 327–92.
provides musical examples of each, ranging from passages by Repr. in Studies in Music with Text. Oxford Studies in
Schoenberg to passages by Holst and Peter Schickele. He even Music Theory. 53–108. Oxford: Oxford University Press,
briefly treats the role of surprise in impressions of musical 2006.
weirdness and quirkiness. But most musical experience does not London, Justin. 2009. ‘‘Farewell Address as President of the
entail such overt affective responses as chills, laughter, awe, or Society for Music Theory.’’ Paper presented at the Thirty-
recognition of the weird. Neither does musical listening seem to Second Annual Meeting of the Society for Music Theory, 28
be primarily motivated by the pursuit of these particular sensa- October–1 November, Montreal.
tions. If expectation is to remain viable as an important expli- MacFarquhar, Larissa. 2007. ‘‘Annals of Science: Two Heads.’’
cator of musical affect, subtler connections should be drawn The New Yorker, 12 February: 58–69.
between the stages of expectational processing outlined by Margulis, Elizabeth Hellmuth. 2007. ‘‘Surprise and Listening
Huron and everyday forms of musical engagement. Ahead: Analytic Engagements with Musical Tendencies.’’
Huron’s book is remarkable in the way it tunnels through so Music Theory Spectrum 29 (2): 197–217.
many fundamental questions about musical experience and Saffran, Jenny R., Richard N. Aslin, and Elissa L. Newport.
behavior and arrives at a general theory of the psychology of 1996. ‘‘Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants.’’
expectation. As the famous Vincent van Gogh line goes, ‘‘If one Science 274 (5294): 1926–28.
is master of one thing and understands one thing well, one has at Saffran, Jenny. R., Elizabeth K. Johnson, Richard N. Aslin, and
the same time, insight into and understanding of many Elissa L. Newport. 1999. ‘‘Statistical Learning of Tone
things.’’13 Sweet Anticipation shows music theory how, in taking Sequences by Human Infants and Adults.’’ Cognition 70
its own problems seriously, it can find itself interfacing with (1): 27–52.
central questions in the humanities and sciences writ large. This Schmuckler, Mark A. 2008. Review of Sweet Anticipation: Music
broad engagement yields a sense throughout the book of real and the Psychology of Expectation by David Huron.
intellectual ferment; when I assign chapters in class, students Philosophical Psychology 21 (1): 137–42.
invariably report it was their favorite read of the semester. Van Gogh, Vincent. [1937] 1995. Dear Theo: The Autobiography
Moreover, whatever the reader thinks about the intellectual of Vincent van Gogh. Ed. Irving Stone and Jean Stone. New
approach to music embodied by the book, its assumptions and York: Plume.
foundational principles are always clear. This grounding makes
it possible for people with different backgrounds to read and
understand the material, increasing the chances of its dissemi-
nation and impact on other areas of inquiry. In this way, Sweet
Anticipation provides a superlative example of music theory that
goes deep into musical listening and in so doing reaches out to
the world.

works cited

Benjamin, William. 2007. Review of Sweet Anticipation: Music


and the Psychology of Expectation by David Huron. The Journal
of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 65 (3): 333–35.
Bharucha, Jamshed J. 1987. ‘‘Music Cognition and Perceptual
Facilitation: A Connectionist Framework.’’ Music Perception
5 (1): 1–30.
Coltheart, Max. 2006. ‘‘Perhaps Functional Neuroimaging Has
Not Told Us Anything About the Mind (So Far).’’ Cortex
42 (3): 422–27.
Harley, Trevor A. 2004. ‘‘Does Cognitive Neuropsychology Music Theory Spectrum, Vol. 34, Issue 2, pp. 145–148, ISSN 0195-6167,
Have a Future?’’ Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 (1): 3–16. electronic ISSN 1533-8339. © 2012 by The Society for Music Theory. All
rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or
reproduce article content through the University of California Press’s
Rights and Permissions website, at http://www.ucpressjournals.com/
13 Van Gogh ([1937] 1995, 33). reprintinfo.asp. DOI: 10.1525/mts.2012.34.2.145

This content downloaded from


129.15.64.254 on Tue, 24 Sep 2019 13:35:34 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

Anda mungkin juga menyukai