Anda di halaman 1dari 29

Psychological Bulletin

1980, Vol. 88, No. 2, 259-287

Environmental Cognition
Gary W. Evans
Program in Social Ecology, University of California, Irvine

Research is reviewed on human spatial cognition in real, everyday settings. The


cognitive mapping literature is organized into five empirical categories: age,
familiarity, gender, class and culture, and physical components of settings.
Methodological and conceptual issues in the environmental cognition literature
are discussed, emphasizing problems with hand-drawn sketch map methodologies
and theoretical ambiguities about the cognitive-mapping process. In particular,
the lack of integration of pertinent cognitive research with the environmental
psychology literature on cognitive mapping is noted. Finally, the potential appli-
cations of environmental cognition work to architecture, planning, and education
are discussed.

How human beings comprehend real-world a passive observer of stimulus objects. The
environments is a major question within en- environment surrounds the observer and is
vironmental psychology and has also recently viewed from multiple vantage points as it is
been addressed by cognitive psychologists.1 explored. Further, information in real-world
As early as 1913, however, Trowbridge stud- settings is not isolated, nonsensical material;
ied how individuals oriented in geographic instead, the information has meaning within
space. Scant human research ensued until the the context of the setting (Ittelson, 1973).
early 1960s (see I. P. Howard & Templeton, Cognitive maps can also be distinguished from
1966, for a review), but important animal other cognitive representations of information.
work was initiated during this period by Tol- First, cognitive maps primarily represent spa-
man (1948) in his classic studies of place tial relationships among loci. Principles of
learning that challenged strict S-R learning commutativity and associativity are impor-
theories. Although controversy still exists re- tant criteria for map utility. Second, the rep-
garding place versus cue response theories of resentation, although not strictly cartographic,
learning, the term cognitive map has remained experientially contains some maplike qualities.
as a general descriptor of the cognitive pro- A good cognitive map facilitates movement
cesses involved in the acquisition, representa- through the actual physical setting represented
tion, and processing of information about ac- by the cognitive schemata of that space. In-
tual physical settings (Downs & Stea, 1973; formation about location and not simply con-
Moore, 1979; Moore & Golledge, 1976). tent (e.g., semantic meaning) is a central com-
Real-world settings differ from stimulus ar- ponent of cognitive representations of physi-
rays used in most experimental studies of cal space (Pick, Note 1).
cognition. In a real-world setting, the observer This article addresses several salient issues
is an interactive part of the environment, not in environmental cognition. First, problematic
aspects of the empirical work pervade the
literature, particularly poor methodology and
This research was partially supported by a Uni- theoretical difficulties. Specific criticisms of
versity of California Regents' Faculty Fellowship. I
thank Sheldon Cohen, Douglas Hintzman, Kathy
Pezdek, and Craig Zimring for critical comments on
1
earlier drafts. This article does not discuss research on environ-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Gary \V. mental assessment or preference that has been called
Evans, Program in Social Ecology, University of environmental perception (Gould & White, 1974;
California, Irvine, California 92717. Lowenthal, 1967).

Copyright 1980 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 003.3-2909/80/8802-02S9$00.7S

259
260 GARY W. EVANS

hand-drawn sketch map and small-scale model mental cognition literature and examines rele-
methodologies are presented, and alternative vant information-processing research.
techniques for data acquisition and analysis The seminal work on environmental cog-
are offered. Information-processing perspec- nition is The Image of the City by Lynch
tives on how visual and spatial information in(I960), an urban planner. Lynch reasoned
actual settings is mentally encoded and manip-that cognitive maps of cities function primarily
ulated are discussed, emphasizing proposition as orientation aids and reflect basic elements
and analog explanations of cognitive repre- of the physical, city form. His own research
sentation. plus that of several other planners (Appleyard,
In addition, several empirical issues are ex-
1969, 1970; de Jonge, 1962; Francescato &
amined. Research on both developmental and Mebane, 1973; Gulick, 1963) suggest five key
familiarity variables has emphasized quali- features that comprise cognitive maps of urban
tatively different stages of knowledge ac- settings: paths, path intersections (nodes),
quisition with increased age or environmental landmarks, districts, and boundaries (edges).
experience. Some theorists have also suggestedLandmarks are external points of reference
that the developmental and familiarity trends from the observer that possess some distinct
parallel one another. Although there is some form that contrasts with background informa-
support for the similarity of the ontogenetic tion. Districts are medium-sized subsections of
and familiarity trends in the literature, I con-
the city that one may enter and feel "inside
clude that it is difficult to distinguish quali-
of" (Lynch, 1960). This taxonomy of urban
tatively distinct stages of cognitive develop-elements of cognitive maps has been derived
ment or familiarity. A reasonable alternative primarily from subjective analyses of hand-
is that quantitative improvements in accuracy drawn, sketch maps. Magafia (1978), how-
occur. Another issue in the familiarity litera-
ever, has recently verified Lynch's five-part
ture is how environmental experience has beentypology through cluster analysis of individu-
operationalized. Although most investigators al's free-pile sorts of verbal labels for physical
have made cross-sectional comparisons (e.g., city features. Cognitive maps also reflect in-
newcomers vs. long-term residents), recent re-
formation about the hierarchical arrangement
search has analyzed more precisely where and of points in space, with respect to relative dis-
how frequently people actually use settings. tance and size. They also contain information
The few gender or class/cultural differ- about the degree of interconnectedness among
ences found in environmental knowledge may points in the geographic environment (Stea,
be explained by individual's daily activity pat-
1969).
terns. Individuals with the greatest extent of Until recently there have been two relatively
daily range have greater and more accurate distinct models in cognitive psychology on
knowledge of their environments. Clear evi- how information is cognitively represented.
dence indicates that various physical featuresPropositional models of cognitive representa-
of settings such as landmark placement and tion state that information is stored in lists
street grid configurations affect setting knowl-
or associated networks based on abstract rep-
edge. Yet few investigators have simultane- resentations of meaning (Anderson & Bower,
ously examined both individual and physical 1973; Pylyshyn, 1973). Closely related to the
variables in environmental cognition. Finally,propositional view is the concept of schemata.
practical applications of environmental cog- Cognitive maps have been viewed as a type of
nition in planning, architecture, and education
schematic structure that helps humans search
are discussed. for and comprehend environmental informa-
tion critical to location and orientation de-
Cognitive Representations of the Physical cisions (S. Kaplan, 1973b, Neisser, 1976;
Environment Stea, 1969). A second perspective on cogni-
tive representations of information is the ana-
This section discusses prevailing models of logical view which states that mental repre-
the cognitive-mapping process in the environ- sentations maintain some rough, isomorphic
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 261

correspondence to the actual physical struc- stemmed from its unusual construction. The
ture of the information in the world (Kosslyn, balcony was recessed within the exterior plane
1975; Shepard, 1975). The correspondence of the building instead of overhanging as most
between the internal, imaginal construct and balconies do.
the external object is considered functional, The influence of higher order organizational
not literal. The reconstruction of the external structures on knowledge of spatial location has
array by the internal representation reexcites been demonstrated in a series of studies by
neural processes that are functionally equiva- Stevens and Coupe (1978). They found that
lent to those mental processes elicited by the the relative spatial position of geographic
external array directly. These opposing views items was distorted in a predictable fashion
have been modified to the position that cog- by the relationship of an item with the super-
nitive representations may code information ordinate unit in which it was situated. Thus
in the form of propositions but can manipulate in judging the relative position of Reno,
it analogically (Kosslyn & Pomerantz, 1977). Nevada, and San Diego, California, most
The view adopted here is that knowledge people will incorrectly state that San Diego
about the content and location of places in is west of Reno. This effect was found for
the geographic environment is stored in both real city locations as well as novel laboratory
prepositional and analogical form. Cognitive stimuli in which the predominant latitudinal
maps include abstract labelings of environ- or longitudinal relationship among superordi-
mental elements (paths, landmarks, etc.) and nate units (U.S. states or novel counties)
cardinal directions and are affected by previ- would strongly bias directional judgments of
ous knowledge of settings in general. Some in- cities contained within the states or counties.
formation, however, such as the relative spa- Similarly, Wilton (1979) has recently shown
tial positions of objects in settings may be that it takes subjects less time to verify if
processed analogically. one town is north of another if the first town
Several studies have shown the influence of is in Scotland and the second in England, than
schemata on human spatial memory. Data in- if two towns are equidistant along the north/
dicate that schemata operate selectively, em- south axis in the same country.
phasizing the spatial arrangement of objects Finally, several studies have focused on the
in complex visual arrays and not the descrip- influence of higher order structure on distance
tive detail of individual items (Mandler & judgments. Lea (1975) had people learn an
Parker, 1976). array of objects located around a circle by
When individuals draw maps of familiar visualizing the objects in space. Subjects were
settings, certain systematic distortions occur, then asked to name the first, second, or nth
which suggests the influence of prototypic bi- target item in a given direction around the
ases in the configuration of geographic settings. circle when given an initial starting point on
Among some of the more commonly noted the circle. Reaction time was a function of the
distortions are the straightening of long, grad- number of intervening objects around the
ual curves, the squaring of nonperpendicular circle, not the actual distance to be traversed
intersections, and the aligning of nonparallel
from the starting point to target. These data
streets (Appleyard, 1969, 1970; Byrne, 1979;
Lynch, 1960). In another example of system- support a propositional model, since people
atic drawing distortions, Norman and Rumel- were apparently searching through a listlike
hart (1975) found that when residents drew set of items and not scanning their visual im-
their apartment floor plans in a particular age. Finally, in making interpoint distance
housing complex, nearly half of them incor- estimates, adults were equally accurate when
rectly extended their balcony beyond the they had learned a novel route through ran-
flush, exterior plane of the apartment. An ad- dom versus sequential presentation. Perceptual
ditional 20% had to redraw the balcony sev- constancy was not necessary for the acquisi-
eral times. The authors suggested that the resi- tion of route knowledge (Allen, Siegel, &
dent's difficulty in drawing the balcony Rosinski, 1978).
262 GARY W. EVANS

Summarizing, several cognitive studies sug- Summary


gest that aspects of spatial representations
of geographic settings manifest propositional It is evident that human beings have cog-
form. Spatial memory reflects higher order nitive representations of various physical set-
schematic structures partially derived from tings they have experienced. These repre-
past geographic experiences. Further, distance sentations may function as schemata that help
estimation tasks under some circumstances facilitate and organize information extraction
rely on apparent list search strategies rather and storage of real-world scenes. Controversy
than scanning a visual image of the points in exists over the nature of these representations,
space. particularly whether they are imaginal, func-
Cognitive maps may also contain informa- tional analogues of actual stimuli with second-
tion in forms that have a more direct, albeit order isomorphic structure or if they are prop-
rough correspondence to actual setting struc- ositional statements about real-world infor-
ture. Evans and Pezdek (1980) asked stu- mation.
dents to judge which of two pairs of campus
buildings or U.S. states was closer together in Methodological Issues
physical distance. They found that for either
The essential methodological problem faced
type of stimuli, decision time increased lin-
by investigators of cognitive representations
early as the ratio of the two intrapair dis-
of the real-world environment is how to ex-
tances approached one. Another investigator
ternalize the individual's mental map of the
reported that subjects who had previously
environment. Researchers in environmental
learned a novel map, took longer to scan their
cognition have depended largely on individu-
images of greater interpoint distances. Fur-
al's hand-drawn sketch maps of their immedi-
thermore, scanning time was a direct linear
ate surroundings as indicators of the cognitive
function of actual interpoint distance on the
processes invoked in the perception and com-
original map (Kosslyn, Ball, & Reiser, 1978).
prehension of the everyday environment. In-
Thus larger physical distances in memory
vestigators have also used modeling tech-
take longer to scan, and physically closer dis-
niques, and a few have probed cognitive repre-
tance discriminations take longer to make.
sentations of the real environment with pic-
Evans and Pezdek (1980) also examined
ture recognition tasks. Unfortunately, there
the ability of individuals to manipulate geo-
have been few psychometric investigations of
graphic information. They found that sub-
the measurement techniques used in cognitive-
jects' reaction time to determine the accuracy mapping research. This section begins with an
of the relative spatial position of triads of
overview of some psychometric aspects of
U.S. states was an increasing, linear function these techniques, followed by a critique of
of the degree of triad rotation from the 0° sketch maps, and then an overview of proce-
Cartesian plane of a standard map of the dural and analysis issues.
United States. A similar rotation function was
found for subjects who learned campus build-
ing loci from a campus map but not for resi- Measurement Issues
dents of that campus. Together these data The use of models and photographs in en-
indicate that when settings are learned from vironmental cognition research raises a funda-
multiple perspectives, as opposed to one per- mental issue—Are these techniques valid simu-
spective only, knowledge of spatial relation- lations of actual settings? Data suggest that
ships in that setting may be flexible and not the degree of scale reduction in models and the
perspective bound. When information about use of photographs can have important effects
the relative locations of geographic elements on cognition. Dirks and Neisser (1977) com-
is learned from maps, knowledge is perspec- pared children's and adults' memory for com-
tive bound and reflects some structural analogy plex visual scenes presented as models and
to the physical relationships among the ac- photographs and found no differences between
tual geographical elements in the setting. the two media.
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 263

Small-scale models preclude motoric experi- to be reliable, with reliability coefficients rang-
ence that, as shown later, is a critical compo- ing from .987 to .995. Correlations among the
nent of learning to orient in a setting. Per- subjective distance estimates and the actual
ception of a small model is also from an distances were high, with a minimum correla-
oblique visual perspective rather than on the tion of .98 out of 25 correlations, Similar high
same visual plane on which real-world settings levels of reliability were found in adults' draw-
are viewed. Acredolo (1977) found that sig- ings of their apartment floor plans (Roth-
nificant developmental differences in the use well, 1976).
of landmarks in a full-scale room were not These two studies suggest relatively good
replicated in a small-scale model of the same reliability and some validity for hand-drawn
room. Furthermore, greater place responding maps of macrospaces. Several cautions, how-
(using objects in the room as frames of refer- ever, are in order. First, both studies focused
ence), as opposed to egocentric responding, on interobject distance and did not examine
occurred in the small-scale model. In addition, the accuracy of relative object placement in
the relative influence of landmarks diminished space. Second, because each distance judgment
substantially in the small-scale model. Her- was not independent from others, the predic-
man and Siegel (1978) also found less devel- tive validity of the correlation data was lim-
opmental differences when children were tested ited. Furthermore, one cannot establish va-
on scale models placed within a smaller, lidity solely on the basis of high convergence
bounded space (classroom) than in a larger, among measures; discriminant validiation is
unbounded space (gymnasium). They also also necessary.
manipulated the scale of their model (300 Several studies have compared the accuracy
square m vs. 80 square m) and found signifi- of different methodologies used in environ-
cantly greater accuracy, independent of grade mental cognition research. Baird (1979) com-
level, in the larger model. On the other hand, pared the nonmetric multidimensional scaling
Siegel, Herman, Allen, and Kirasic (1979) re- (MDS) solutions of magnitude estimates of
ported that children performed comparably interpoint distances among familiar buildings
in large- and small-scale models, provided and a computer map technique in which sub-
that reconstruction tasks were required at the jects arranged buildings on a matrix display.
same scale. However, when children learned Although both methods yielded comparable
a setting on a small-scale model and then re- high accuracy, both the subjects and inde-
constructed it on a large-scale model, they pendent judges rated the computer maps as
had considerable difficulty. If children were more accurate. MacKay (1976), however,
first exposed to a large-scale model, the scale found that MDS solutions of sketch maps
of the model for the reconstruction task did were more accurate than were MDS outputs
not matter. Thus the scale of models, their from the sorting of item pairs into similar
placement in rooms, and the extent of scale distance categories. Finally, Magana, Evans,
transition are important factors in research and Romney (in press) found comparable high
that uses models. accuracy among the MDS solutions of magni-
Two studies have examined psychometric tude estimations of interpoint distances, tri-
properties of sketch maps (R. B. Howard, adic comparisons of interpoint distances, and
Chase, & Rothman, 1973; Rothwell, 1976). hand-drawn maps. In all of these studies, ac-
R. B, Howard and colleagues had adults per- curacy has been operationalized as the degree
form one of the following tasks in a familiar, of fit between the MDS solutions of the in-
outdoor environment: (a) Draw a map of the terpoint distance matrix from the actual set-
environment, (b) place objects in scale mod- ting and the interpoint distance matrix pro-
els, (c) make magnitude estimation judg- duced by the subject using the respective
ments of interobject distances, and (d) make methodologies,
ratio estimates of interobject distances by Different measurement techniques have also
marking off a standarized line in proportion been compared in the amount of information
to the real distance. All four methods proved remembered in verbal and drawing recall
264 GARY W. EVANS

probes, picture recognition, and eye fixation floor plan drawings. For children, however,
data. Carr and Schissler (1969) found some there was a substantial correlation (.62) be-
discrepancies between subjects' eye movement tween judges' ratings of the accuracy of chil-
data, free-recall data, and sketch maps of the dren's floor plan sketches and the children's
same area collected on different individuals scores on the Goodenough-Harris Draw-A-
earlier by Appleyard, Lynch, and Myer (1964). Man-Test. Although this test may indicate
There was high correlation between verbal graphic ability, it is highly correlated with
free recall of elements and recall on sketch standard intelligence measures. Rothwell's
maps, but neither was highly related to eye adult data indicate that graphic ability may
fixation data. Some data indicate possible have only slight effect on sketch map pro-
retrieval difficulties in sketch map reconstruc- ductions. His developmental findings, when
tions. Both Banerjee (1971) and Milgram and coupled with Kosslyn et al.'s data, however,
Jodelet (1976) found that accurate picture indicate that children's drawings are not a
recognition was slightly greater for some ob- suitable measurement technique for probing
jects than free recall on sketch maps. Further- their cognitive representations of large-scale
more, verbal free-recall descriptions of city environments.
elements exceeded the recall of elements on Parameters of the drawing task itself may
sketch maps for the same city, particularly for be problematic as well. The issue of scale is
low-frequency items (Appleyard, 1976; Baner- pertinent. Artifactual variables that could
jee, 1971; Lynch, 1960; Magana, 1978; Mil- have an impact on scale include the size of
gram & Jodelet, 1976). the drawing surface and the order in which
Summarizing, preliminary data suggest things are drawn. Initially drawn elements
problems with the use of small-scale models may have substantial effects on the relative
to test individuals' cognitive representations size and/or position of subsequent elements.
of macroenvironments. Psychometric research The more elements one has put down on a
on sketch map techniques indicates adequate drawing, the fewer are the degrees of freedom
reliability, but important questions about that remain to maintain size and distance
validity remain. scale. Research on young children's drawings
of simple objects (typically human figures)
has shown that initial picture units impose
Problems With Hand-Drawn Sketch Map restrictions on the size and relative position
Methodologies of subsequent picture components (Goodnow,
1977). The lack of independence among drawn
The use of sketch maps as a source of data
elements also creates statistical problems.
raises several questions. For example, do indi-
vidual differences in drawing ability seriously Another point of concern is the tendency of
confound sketch map output? Golledge (1976) some investigators to treat the temporal or-
and Blaut and Stea (1974) have argued that dering of the recall of elements on a map as
sketch map data underrepresent a person's indicative of relative importance (Golledge,
knowledge because of limitations in drawing 1976). On the other hand, Milgram and Jode-
ability. This may be particularly true for let (1976) did find a high correspondence
children. Kosslyn, Heldmeyer, and Locklear among first-drawn elements and those most
(1977), who systematically studied children's frequently recognized in a picture recognition
probe.
drawings of simple objects, concluded that
cognitive representations of objects in school Finally, the potential influence of individual
children cannot be inferred on the basis of differences in map interpretation experience
their drawings. Little data are available on the has not been systematically examined. Several
effects of graphic ability on sketch maps of studies provided suggestive evidence that map
actual environments. Rothwell (1976) found experience enhances the accuracy and com-
a small but significant correlation (.14) be- plexity of hand-drawn sketch maps (Beck &
tween adults' graphic skills (Lurcat Test of Wood, 1976; Dart & Pradham, 1967; Magana,
Graphic Ability) and the accuracy of home 1978). Furthermore, as discussed earlier,
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 265

Evans and Pezdek (1980) concluded that the landmarks. Several studies have also been
use of maps affected how environments were vague in describing the judges who scored
cognitively encoded. the maps or models. Frequently the criteria
used in judging accuracy, complexity, and so
forth have not been clearly delineated. Fur-
General Procedural Issues thermore, judgment criteria, when described
adequately, have varied across studies. Thus
There are numerous procedural issues in-
basic methodological studies are sorely needed
volved in cognitive mapping research that have
to compare different rating procedures of
not been adequately explored. Instructions to
accuracy.
subjects in environmental cognition articles are
A final problem is how to best present ag-
frequently not described adequately. For ex-
ample, what degree of information should one gregate descriptions of data. Few researchers
provide subjects in an orientation problem or have described how they have constructed
sketch map task? Some investigators have modal or mean descriptions of sets of individu-
provided subjects with basic landmarks and/ als' map or model data. Furthermore, the ex-
tent of individual variability often goes unre-
or boundaries, whereas others have measured
ported or cannot be adequately determined.
free recall without cues. Another methodo-
logical difficulty is the level of linguistic com- Two possible solutions are first, use of non-
petence assumed in instructions. For example, metric MDS techniques to aggregate data on
can a child comprehend what it means to im- an objective basis (Evans, Marrero, & Butler,
agine standing in front of his/her bathroom in press; Golledge, 1976, 1977; Kosslyn, Pick,
& Fariello, 1974). A second approach is the
sink and describe what one could see on the
use of computer mapping capabilities (Baird,
other side if they were able to see through
the wall (Pick, Acredolo, & Gronseth, Note 1979). Both of these techniques avoid con-
2) ? With young children in particular we structions of aggregate or modal maps based
need to carefully consider whether inferior on judges' or authors' subjective criteria. In-
performance indicates deficiencies in cognitive stead, these techniques use scaling algorithms
representation or inability to understand in- that compute aggregates on the basis of sum-
structions. mated, interpoint distance matrices taken di-
Another set of problems concerns the con- rectly from map displays. More detailed de-
founding of age and experience. When older scriptions are available in Golledge and Baird.
children represent an environment more ac- Alternatively, one can circumvent some of
curately, is it because they have had more the problems raised here by using photo-
experience with it? Developmental studies can graphic probes as Milgram has recommended
avoid confounding experience with age by (Milgram, Greemvald, Kessler, McKenna, &
examining individuals at different age levels Waters, 1972) whereby scoring and analysis
crossed with groups with different amounts difficulties are substantially reduced. Recogni-
of experience in a given setting. tion paradigms, however, eliminate important
Data analysis is also a concern. Golledge data such as estimates of subjective distance
(1976, 1977) has noted that many cognition and the relative locations of objects. Hard-
researchers have treated sketch map data at an wick, Mclntyre, and Pick's (1976) sighting
internal or ratio level when they should be tube methodology also provides an innovative
approach to cognitive map research that is
treated either descriptively or analyzed within
ordinal restrictions. For example, when some- free of many of the procedural and analysis
one draws two landmarks 6 cm apart and two issues raised. Subjects sight several target
other landmarks 3 cm apart on a sketch map, places in space from three different viewing
it seems more valid to assume that the indi- stations behind opaque barriers. The inter-
vidual knows that the first pair of landmarks section of the three sighting lines (triangula-
is farther apart than the second pair, rather tion) provides a location estimate. Kozlowski
than assuming she/he knows that the first pair and Bryant's (1977) simple sketching proce-
is twice as far apart as the second pair of dures using the basic outline shape of an area
266 GARY W. EVANS

embellished with one or two existing land- with changes in projective spatial relation-
marks also provides researchers with a less ships involving shifts in viewing perspective.
error-prone method for drawing than a free- Initially, there is a strong egocentric bias in
hand sketch map. Finally, orientation tasks in projective spatial comprehension. Finally,
the actual environment can be experimentally children comprehend Euclidean spatial prop-
examined, given sufficient ingenuity, as evi- erties (e.g., angularity, distance) with re-
denced by the research programs of Acredolo spect to an abstract set or sets of coordinates.
and Pick and their colleagues (Acredolo, 1976, Frames of reference research in children.
1977; Acredolo, Pick, & Olsen, 197S). Several investigators have suggested that
younger children learn to orient in settings
Cognitive-Mapping Research: Empirical primarily through egocentric relationships.
Findings This is followed by place learning in which
one orients to the fixed location of proximate
Research from the environmental cognition elements such as landmarks. Finally, orienta-
literature is reviewed and summarized below tion occurs with respect to coordinated sys-
in five empirical categories: age, familiarity, tems of reference. At this stage children do not
gender, class and culture, and physical com- orient in terms of the relationship of their
ponents of environments. body position in space or the spatial relation-
ships among proximate landmarks. Instead,
Age location is determined with respect to the
broader area and is expressed in more ab-
Ontogenetic research on environmental cog- stract terms such as cardinal directions (Hart
nition can be divided into two broad cate- & Moore, 1973).
gories: frame of reference and representation
Empirical research on this topic is gener-
research. Frame of reference work focuses on
ally consistent with the views of Piaget and
the types of information people use to spa-
their extension to geographic settings by Hart
tially orient themselves in space. Representa-
and Moore. Several studies have examined the
tion research examines the degree of accuracy
cues children use to describe others' perspec-
and complexity in an individual's memory for
tives in scale models. Piaget and his colleagues
spatial relationships in the environment. The
in an early study of perspective taking asked
Piagetian perspective on spatial cognition has
children to determine what a doll's view of
greatly influenced both lines of developmental
three, distinctive modeled mountains would
research on environmental cognition. This
be when it was placed at various viewpoints.
perspective states that the ontogeny of spatial
Preoperational children (4-6-J- years) main-
comprehension proceeds through a logical or-
tained primarily egocentric viewpoints, draw-
dering of three kinds of spatial information:
ing, modeling, or selecting the picture that
topological, protective, and metric (Euclidean)
represented their own view. These younger
space (Piaget & Inhelder, 1967; Piaget, In-
children persisted in this egocentric representa-
helder, & Szeminska, 1960).
tion, even when allowed to walk around the
Topological information includes an object's
proximity, separation, extent of continuity, model and view it from the doll's perspective.
With the onset of concrete operations (7-9
and degree of enclosure. The ability to com-
years), children coordinated perspectives, gen-
prehend topological space that arises during
the transition period from sensorimotor to erally reflecting a correct sense of in front
of/in back of, then followed by left and right.
preoperational cognition is strongly linked to
direct tactile and motor experience. Projective Not until the latter stages of concrete opera-
spatial comprehension that emerges during tions, however, did children correctly per-
the latter preoperational stage of cognitive de- form the task indicating that they could co-
velopment is based on the interrelationships ordinate perspectives that were independent
among objects in space as perceived from of their own egocentric viewpoint.
various perspectives. Shape and form informa- Subsequent research has replicated the ma-
tion (e.g., rectilinearity) are comprehended jor developmental trends found by Piaget and
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 267

Inhelder but has raised two questions. First, ings. Thus the doll was placed next to the
to what extent do the developmental differ- same object or one in the same part of the
ences reflect qualitative shifts in spatial cog- model, with little or no apparent recognition
nition as opposed to quantitative progress? of distance, before-behind, left-right, and so
Flavell, Botkin, Fry, Wright, and Jarvis forth. Children at this age could not logically
(1968) concluded that only egocentric and transpose more than one proximate relation-
nonegocentric responses could be differentiated ship at a time. For example, if the doll was
and that other changes were simply quantita- placed in a stream in Model A at the bottom
tive, whereas others have concluded that qual- of the model, nearly all children placed it in
itative shifts occur, including the orderly com- the stream in Model B but at many different
prehension of object interposition, object ori- locations. During Stage II (4-6 years), two
entation, and left/right relationships (Coie, substages were reported. Children 4 and S
Costanzo, & Farnill, 1973; Laurendeau & Pi- years old located the doll relative to their
nard, 1970; Nigl & Fishbein, 1974). Second, own position and disregarded rotation but
procedural variations can improve younger used more than one feature at a time to locate
children's comprehension of another's per- the doll. During the latter part of Stage II
spective. The use of easily differentiated ob- (5-6 years), representation gradually ap-
jects or requiring children to mentally rotate peared that accounted for rotation. Left-right
the model may reduce egocentric responding and before-after relationships, order, and dis-
in preoperational children (Borke, 1975; Fish- tance emerged gradually as the result of a
bein, Lewis, & Keiffer, 1972; Huttenlocher & laborious trial-and-error process. At Stage III
Presson, 1973; Masangkay et al., 1974). (6-7 years), model rotation no longer affected
Younger children can also adopt perspec- the child's judgment, and the doll was placed
tives other than their own in interpreting accurately.
aerial photographs (Blaut, McCleary, & Blaut, Several investigators have replicated these
1970; Blaut Si Stea, 1974). Four-year-olds findings, particularly initial egocentrism and
located their homes and traced routes between inability to coordinate more than one spatial
points on aerial photographs. An important referent simultaneously. This is followed by
difference between these studies and Piaget's rotation comprehension but continued diffi-
is that subjects here could successfully per- culty with multiple referents coordination,
form the tasks required primarily by object with final emergence of multiple, referent co-
identification (i.e., "that is a road," etc.). In ordination abilities (De Lisi, Locker, &
the previously described studies, comprehen- Youniss, 1976; Laurendeau & Pinard, 1970;
sion of relative object location, not descriptive Pufall & Shaw, 1973). Controversy remains,
content, was required. however, about the specific ages for each
In a second study on frame of reference, stage. It is also unclear whether mental age
Piaget and Inhelder (1967) asked children to (as distinguished from IQ) accounts for some
place objects on a scale model. To locate a of the age differences reported. Unfortunately,
doll on the landscape model, the child was there are no data on this question. One im-
presented a model (Model A) on which the portant issue in many of the described de-
experimenter placed a doll. Separated from velopmental studies is the unknown influence
Model A by a screen was a second identical of language comprehension on children's task
model (Model B), rotated 180°. To accu- performance. For example, younger children
rately locate the doll on Model B, children will behave less egocentrically in spatial model-
could not use their own reference position ing tasks when told to mentally rotate a model
(egocentric orientation) but had to rely on versus when told to adopt another's spatial
either parts of the model itself (fixed location) perspective.
or some abstract coordinate system of cardinal An additional limitation in all of the de-
directions. scribed studies is their reliance on small-scale
At ages 3-4, doll position was determined models that, as discussed earlier, may affect
primarily by relative proximity of surround- results, particularly with children. Recently,
268 GARY W. EVANS

researchers have examined the frames of ref- confusing target sightings as if no imagined
erence children use to orient in larger scale location shifts had occurred. Fifth graders cor-
models. In one study 3-, 4-, and 10-year-olds rectly determined ordinal distance relation-
were led to a table on their right as they en- ships among targets but failed to accurately
tered an otherwise empty room and were then coordinate this information with specific angu-
blindfolded. Children were then led around lar data. Thus fifth graders made relatively
the room, with half of them ending their walk accurate gross directional determinations but
at the opposite side of the room from the failed to fine tune their responses. First grad-
original entry and half returning to the origi- ers frequently did not discern basic directional
nal entry point. In addition, for half of them, information.
the table was moved to the opposite side of Summary of developmental frame of refer-
the room. The blindfold was removed, and the ence research. Research on frame of reference
child asked to return to the spot at which the information used to orient in modeled and
blindfold had been fastened. Three-year-olds realistic settings generally supports the devel-
either responded egocentrically, turning to opmental sequence posited by Piaget and sub-
their right regardless of change in bodily posi- sequently elaborated on by Hart and Moore.
tion or table placement or depended on a fixed Initially, young children rely heavily on ego-
frame of reference provided by the table posi- centric cues to orient in space. This is fol-
tion. The 4-year-olds used table position pre- lowed by the use of fixed objects in space,
dominantly to orient, whereas the 10-year- first singly and eventually coordinating multi-
olds relied on a coordinated frame of refer- ple objects' interrelationships to the observer.
ence (e.g., the room itself), correctly .lo- Finally, comprehension of space as a coordi-
cating the original blindfolding point irre- nate system, independent of the object's or
spective of their relative body position or the person's position within that space occurs. Con-
location of the table (Acredolo, 1976). In troversy remains regarding the precise age
subsequent research Acredolo (1977) has ranges of these changes, the influence of differ-
shown that egocentric responding in 3-, and entiated landmarks on the process, and whether
4-year-olds under similar conditions is reduced sequential, qualitative differences among non-
by providing landmark cues in the room. egocentric errors can be discerned.
A second developmental research program Frames of reference and orientation behav-
on orientation examined the abilities of first- ior in the elderly. Orientation abilities of the
grade, fifth-grade, and college students to elderly have not been examined in great de-
sight target items through a sighting tube tail. Looft and Charles (1971) and Rubin,
when the targets are in the same room but not Attewell, Tierney, and Tumolo (1973) found
visible (Hardwick et al., 1976). The intersec- the elderly less accurate than young adults in
tion of three different viewing sights on each perspective-taking tasks like the Piagetian
target was used to measure accuracy. First three-mountain problem but did not examine
graders performed significantly poorer on the the types of errors made. Schultz and Hoyer
task but still had relatively good knowledge (1976) found that the elderly made more
of the familiar environment (school library). nonegocentric errors than young adults but
There were greater age differences in two vari- the same amount of egocentric errors in a
ations of the task in which subjects sighted perspective-taking task. They did not exam-
all the targets from one viewing station while ine the kinds of nonegocentric mistakes made.
imagining that they were sighting the targets More recently Ohta, Walsh, and Krauss (Note
from each other viewing station (perspective 3) examined error rates, reaction times, and
taking) or that the room had rotated relative types of errors made by elderly and young
to their fixed position in space (mental rota- adults in a spatial memory task. Subjects ex-
tion). Only adults accurately performed these amined a small-scale model of three buildings
tasks, and qualitatively different errors were and then judged the accuracy of various
noticed among the first and fifth graders. The slides that represented either the actual view
first graders made egocentric errors, frequently of the model, an egocentric view, interposi-
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 269

tion shifts, 180° rotation of each building, or grouped objects together that were near one
a right/left reversal. The elderly's correct another in the model. Intergroup arrangement,
slide judgments were slower, and overall er- as opposed to intragroup arrangement, ap-
ror rates were higher than the younger adults'; peared particularly distorted and uncoordi-
however, the elderly made the same proportion nated. Finally, at 7-10 years a more complete
of error types as the young adults. coordination of projective and Euclidean spa-
Representation research in children. A sec- tial properties emerged. Left-right and be-
ond line of inquiry on developmental aspects hind-before positions and relative distance
of environmental cognition has examined the were conserved and accurately modeled, cul-
properties of children's mental representations minating in a final Stage IV in which propor-
of spatial layouts. This research has focused tional scale reduction was accomplished.
on the accuracy and complexity of children's In another study Piaget et al. (1960) had
memory for spatial information in actual and children model known routes in a sandbox
modeled settings. Siegel and White (1975), with components provided to represent build-
drawing from the Piagetian perspective on ings, rivers, and so forth. Children under 4
spatial cognition, have posited a develop- years could not do the task but responded to
mental model of spatial representation. First, questions as they were taken for an actual
the young child notices and remembers land- walk. There was a clear egocentric perspective
marks. Once landmarks are established, route in orientation; direction reversals, for ex-
learning occurs within their context in a ample, created confusion. From 4-7 years
point-to-point fashion. Third, landmarks and there was a strong reliance on landmarks as
routes are organized into small clusters that they appeared in serial order in locomotion
have good internal organization but that are along the route. Position and distance were
poorly coordinated with one another. Finally, not accurately represented; instead, landmark
survey representation is achieved wherein position in sequence was a critical orientation
routes are coordinated within an overall frame cue. When children drew routes, Piaget noted
of reference. A similar distinction was made a similar influence of actual motoric represen-
earlier by Shemyakin (1962) between route tation in recall. Each portion of the journey
and survey knowledge. He found that 6-8- was constructed in seriation with various sub-
year-olds' maps of familiar settings frequently sections joined only loosely and with consid-
reflected familiar routes of locomotion, whereas erable error. Thus the route was represented
older children's were more holistic, accu- as a collection of links between landmark
rately coordinating landmarks and paths in pairs or small groups. The influence of motoric
space. experience in younger children's (4-7 years)
Empirical research on the development of spatial representations has also been demon-
spatial representations has relied on models, strated in distance judgment tasks. Preopera-
sketch maps, and behavior in actual settings. tional children are strongly affected by func-
Several early studies by Piaget et al. (1960) tional distance, as opposed to actual visual
illustrate the use of small-scale models to distance. Younger children are more likely to
study spatial representation abilities in chil- distort their distance judgments between ob-
dren. In the first study children were pre- jects when barriers exist between the points
sented diagrammatic layouts of a model land- (Kosslyn, Pick, & Fariello, 1974) or when
scape and asked to either draw pictures or the points are linked by indirect (looping)
duplicate it with another model. Children 4 versus direct pathways (Anooshian & Wilson,
years and under did not comprehend the spa- 1977). Older children's and adults' distance
tial arrangement and frequently did not even judgments rely on direct visual estimates and
choose the same components or the right less on the path one would have to take to get
number when duplicating the model. The 4-6- from one point to another.
year-olds correctly identified the model objects Children 7-9 years old in Piaget's experi-
but could not logically order more than one ment grouped objects in terms of a fixed ref-
spatial relationship simultaneously. Children erence system of landmarks but not in terms
270 GARY W. EVANS

of a whole, abstract coordinate system. Parts of the younger children. The kindergartners
of the plan were organized correctly, but the did not improve with practice over initially
relationships among parts or object clusters low levels of accuracy for the exact placement
were not fully coordinated. Finally, older chil- of buildings. Furthermore all of the children,
dren assumed a more holistic coordinated ref- although to a lesser extent than the kinder-
erential system. Object groups were intercon- gartners, performed more poorly in the un-
nected, with distance and relative position ac- bounded space of the larger room. Again, we
curate both within and between element see reasons to be cautious in using models for
clusters. Caution in interpreting the develop- measuring environmental cognition abilities in
mental data in both of these studies by Piaget children.
is warranted, given earlier discussion of diffi- Working at the interior scale, Day (1977)
culties with children's drawings and small- asked children (kindergarten and first, third,
scale model building. and fifth graders) to replace furniture in a
In an interesting extension of Piaget's model house they had walked through. Both
route-modeling experiment, Herman and Siegel the exact location of the furniture and the
(1978) walked kindergartners, second graders, relative positioning of furniture in rooms was
and fifth graders through a large model of a more accurate with increasing age, with the
town that contained eight distinct buildings. largest increments between third and fifth
Half of the children constructed the town from grade. Although the relatively low level of
memory after each of three walks, whereas exact object placement accuracy for the younger
the other half of the children constructed the children is consistent with the Piagetian per-
town only after their third walk through the spective, the continued improvement in rela-
model. After initial exposure to the model, tive object location accuracy over the four
fifth graders exhibited significantly greater grades is not in accord with the Piagetian
building placement accuracy (both correct position.
quadrant placement and exact location). The At least two model studies of children's
experience of walking through the .model also spatial representation knowledge have found
significantly increased both types of accuracy. no age differences. Siegel and Schadler (1977)
Furthermore, an interaction of age and con- asked children 61-70 months old to build a
struction experience suggested that with model of their classroom in a sandbox. No age
greater experience the age differences dimin- differences in either exact or relative object
ished significantly. placement were found. Stea and colleagues
The high level of Euclidean accuracy ob- (Blaut & Stea, 1974; Stea & Taphanel, 1974)
tained by the younger children with repeated in a series of studies instructed 3- and 4-year-
experience in this study is contradictory to olds to arrange toys on a large, novel land-
Piagetian theory and to much of the work scapelike graphic on the floor. Both age groups
discussed earlier. Herman and Siegel (1978) were equally adept at building realistic models
suspected that this high level of apparently and navigating certain routes with "cars."
Euclidean comprehension on the part of the Given the narrow age range in the Siegel and
younger children was due to a procedural Schadler study, the lack of age differences
artifact. In a second study the same layout therein is not surprising. The absence of age
was used in the middle of a large room (gym- differences in Stea's research is difficult to in-
nasium) in which the walls of the space were terpret because of the dependent measures
far enough away so that it was unlikely that used. It is not clear if judges' ratings of land-
the room itself could function as a bounded scape realism and route navigation perform-
space, providing topological cues external to ance reflect cognitive, developmental change.
the model itself. In the first study the model Three studies have tested children's knowl-
had been set up in the middle of a classroom edge of actual settings with spatial problem-
in which the walls of the room were close to solving tasks. Pick et al. (Note 2) asked 3-,
the edges of the model. The room change had 4-, and S-year-olds to select a rectangular
a substantial effect on the Euclidean accuracy shape most like their bedroom and to indicate
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 271

where the doors, closets, and windows were reversal task, but the 3-year-olds were less
located by placing models of these compo- accurate in the landmark reversal testing.
nents in the appropriate places. The same task Only the 6-year-olds completed the landmark
was also completed for the kitchen. Children inference task accurately. The age differences
were also asked how they would go from their in landmark reversal and inference tasks may
kitchen to their bedroom and to describe what reflect differences in language comprehension
they would see if they were able to see through more than spatial cognition. The 6-year-olds
their bathroom mirror. For the second phase were better than the S-year-olds in model
of the experiment, the children were asked building only when a more complex route had
what was behind two walls of their own room originally been taken through the rooms (zig-
and the kitchen while they were actually zag vs. a U), but otherwise 5- and 6-year-olds
present in their own home. had equally accurate models that were better
Groups did equally well in identifying the than the 3-year-olds' models.
shape of their own room and kitchen. The Scant research has examined the spatial
older group, however, averaged greater cor- representation abilities of adolescents, Ladd
rect object placement across the two rooms (1970), Lynch (1977), Maurer and Baxter
and gave more accurate descriptions of what (1Q72), and Moore (1973) have reported no
was beyond the walls of their room than the significant age effects in the neighborhood
younger children. Both groups did poorly on sketch maps of junior high adolescents. An-
the mirror imagination task with no age dif- drews (1973), however, found that high school
ferences. In addition to some of the problems seniors were more accurate in locating down-
in using scale models, these age-related data town Toronto landmarks on a schematic map
may partially reflect differences in the younger than were ninth graders. This age effect in-
and older children's ability to comprehend teracted with residential location, indicating
instructions. that only students who lived within seven
In a second experiment 3-, 4-, and 8-year- miles of Toronto exhibited these age trends.
old children were led on a walk through a hall- Thus a plausible explanation for these age
way in their school (Acredolo et al., 197S). differences is differential experience.
During the walk, the experimenter "acci- Two recent studies suggested deficiencies in
dently" dropped her keys in a hallway that the elderly's recall of the spatial layout of set-
was either bare of furniture or had a few tings. Weber, Brown, and Weldon (1978)
distinctive landmarks (different chairs). The asked elderly residents of a nursing home
landmarks were not near the key drop. The whether they recognized various photographs
child's task was to return to the location of the of their residence and what section of the
key drop after walking through the hall. Pre- home the photo was from. In comparison with
schoolers made greater errors than older chil- college students considerably less familiar with
dren when no landmarks were present, but the nursing home, the elderly were less ac-
when landmarks were present, no age differ- curate on both tasks. The identification data
ences were found, may reflect response bias differences, how-
Finally, Hazen, Lockman, and Pick (1978) ever, since no scenes not from the nursing
compared 3-6-year-olds' ability to navigate home were used. Although all of the elderly
in four or six interconnected rooms. Once they patients participating had been rated by nurs-
had learned a specified route and several land- ing staff as alert and mobile, the college stu-
marks in the rooms, the children retraced dents had been given a tour of the entire build-
their original route in reverse. They also were ing, whereas certain areas of the home may not
required to anticipate in serial order each have been frequented by the elderly patients.
landmark as they approached it, infer the lo- In a second study, the authors in fact found
cation of other landmarks not directly on the that the patients spent most of the day in
original route, and finally build a model of their own rooms. Walsh, Krauss, and Regnier
the rooms when the test walk was completed. (in press) asked elderly and young adults to
All ages performed equally well on the route draw maps of their neighborhoods. Indepen-
272 GARY W. EVANS

dent judges rated the elderly's maps as more Familiarity


disorganized, simple, and inaccurate. The el-
derly's maps were also smaller in size and Several researchers have examined the ef-
moderately correlated with the use of facili- fects of setting familiarity on cognitive maps.
ties in the area and self-ratings of mobility. This research has focused primarily on two
Elderly who were more mobile and who used issues: the kinds of elements individuals rely
a broader range of facilities in their neighbor- on to learn new environments and changes in
hood drew larger maps. No sex differences or accuracy that occur with increased environ-
educational level effects were found. In both mental familiarity.
of these studies with the elderly, mobility Physical elements. There are two com-
provides a partial explanation for age effects. peting models of how people learn a new set-
Summary of spatial representation research. ting. One position argues that individuals ini-
Children's knowledge of spatial information tially rely on paths and districts to orient in
generally fits the developmental sequence a novel environment. Later, when they are
posited by Piaget and elaborated on by Siegel more familiar with the setting, they rely pri-
and White. Initially, route knowledge de- marily on landmarks for orientation (Apple-
yard, 1970, 1976; Lynch, 1960). Conversely,
velops and involves at least two aspects: di-
rection choice at landmarks and knowledge of both Hart and Moore (1973) and Siegel and
landmark sequence. A strong, motoric, experi- White (197S) theorized that environmental
ential bias is present in preschoolers learning learning is primarily landmark based, with
of geographic spatial information. Thus the path structures elaborated subsequently within
dependence on model tasks in several stud- original anchor, landmark points.
ies is bothersome. At the next stage of de- Appleyard (1970, 1976) compared the
velopment, knowledge of the relative location sketch maps of adults who had lived in an
urban area for less than 6 months, for 6
of objects within small proximate clusters is
months to 1 year, for 1 to S years, and for
apparent, but intercluster, relative position
is poor. Accurate intercluster representation greater than S years. People who had lived in
gradually emerges with the development of an the city for 1 year or less produced maps
characterized by greater path usage (sequen-
overall coordinate system of spatial referents
that is independent of landmark locations or tial dominant map). More long-term residents
the observer's position in the setting. There drew maps emphasizing schematic boundaries
is little difference in the accuracy of the ordi- and landmarks (spatial dominant map). In
addition, Devlin (1976) found that newcomers
nal relationships among objects in space for
to an area (2 weeks) used nearly the same
different aged children, but marked differ- pathways 6 weeks later in sketch maps but
ences appear in the ability to exactly locate showed greater variability in landmarks. Early
points in space. Although the converging lines paths seemed to establish initial structures
of evidence for this developmental sequence that were then elaborated on with continued
are strong, a caveat is in order. All of the setting experience. Thus both of these studies
developmental, spatial representation studies are consistent with Lynch's and Appleyard's
except one have relied on scale models, and hypothesis that path structures are most
several included instructions that may be critical as early learning cues in the physical
difficult for younger children to understand. environment. Unfortunately, Appleyard's clas-
Little data are available on the cognitive sification scheme of sequential or spatial domi-
nant maps includes other criteria in addition
mapping abilities of adolescents and the el-
to route or landmark predominance and does
derly. Adolescent trends suggest that few
not directly isolate the two important fea-
changes occur between the ages of approxi- tures. Devlin's data clearly support the pri-
mately 12 and 17 years in mapping accuracy. macy of path structures in early learning but
The elderly may have some deficits in environ- may not generalize well, since her setting was
mental cognition, but the influence of mo- a small town with few distinct, easily visible
bility needs to be considered more carefully. landmarks.
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 273

Evans, Marrero, and Butler (in press) as- mate groups were correct, but the relative
sessed changes in adults' sketch maps of their positions of the clusters were not correct.
residential environment over a 1-year-period. Familiar map areas appeared to be more ab-
Subjects from two independent samples • re- stractly coordinated, with both high intra-
called significantly more paths and nodes af- and intercluster accuracy, Similar results were
ter 1 years residence but recalled the same obtained by Banerjee (1971) and Tzamir
number of landmarks. The landmarks recalled (Note 4), who experimentally manipulated
were nearly identical to those recalled during familiarity by exposure to a model.
the first week. Furthermore, path systems were Two studies directly measured changes in
elaborated within the initial landmark struc- topological, projective, and Euclidean accuracy
ture with most of the increases in paths re- over time. Siegel and Schadler (1977) exam-
flecting alternative routes between already ined the effect of 6 months experience on 5-
established landmarks. In addition, Heft and 6-year olds' model reconstructions of their
(1979) reported that adults relied more on classroom. Independent of maturation, experi-
landmarks to learn a route through a novel ence significantly enhanced Euclidean accu-
path network the first time they walked racy (exact object placement) but had no
through it than when they had traversed the impact on topological or projective accuracy
path system several times. (relative object placement within an object
Accuracy. The environmental learning se- cluster, relative cluster placement within the
quences posited by Hart and Moore (1973) model), Evans, Marrero, and Butler (in press)
and Siegel and White (197S) may be extended also found that for adults, intracluster rela-
to shifts in accuracy with experience that re- tive landmark placement and intercluster
semble the ontogenesis of spatial cognition placement did not improve over a 1-year pe-
(Piaget & Inhelder, 1967). Preliminary con- riod but that exact landmark placement did.
tact with an environment provides comprehen- Euclidean accuracy was measured by com-
sion of the relative positions of items in space paring the nonmetric MDS solutions for in-
with respect to the body. Thus the relative dividuals' maps at the beginning and end of
location of objects close in space can be cor- the year, respectively, with the MDS solution
rectly processed, provided no perspective for the real maps in each setting. Golledge,
shifts are demanded. Projective accuracy fol- Rivizzigno, and Spector (1976) also used
lows with items comprehended in terms of MDS techniques, comparing subjects' esti-
their relative positions with respect to various mates of interpoint distances. They found that
fixed points in space. Finally, Euclidean com- object location was better in old residents (6
prehension emerges wherein space is encoded months to 1 year) than in newcomers (less
as a unit in which items are located with re- than a few weeks).
spect to their position in two- or three-dimen- At least two studies, however, found no ef-
sional space. fects of environmental experience on accu-
There are a few findings in the environ- racy. Ladd (1970) reported that the accuracy
mental cognition literature generally consistent of adolescents' sketch maps of their neighbor-
with this Piagetian trend. Appleyard (1970, hood was unrelated to length of residence
1976) found that newcomers to a city (less based on judge's ratings of accuracy. Length
than 6 months) were more apt to reverse the of residence was grossly dichotomized, how-
location of different city zones than were ever, into less than or greater than 3 years
long-term residents. Both newcomers and long- residence, which may have been too broad a
term residents, however, placed roads and time period. The high levels of map accuracy
landmarks within zones equally accurately. found from 6 months to 1 years residence by
Moore (1974) had independent judges sort several researchers (Appleyard, 1976; Evans,
students' maps of familiar and unfamiliar city Marrero, & Butler, in press; Golledge et al.,
areas. Area familiarity was determined by 1976) plus Ladd's data suggest that accuracy
self-report. For unfamiliar map areas the rela- may improve and then reach asymptote within
tive positions of objects within small, proxi- roughly a 1-year period. In the second study,
274 GARY W. EVANS

Acredoio et al., (1975) found that children substantially more photographs of their own
accurately recalled where a particular recent borough than did nonresidents. An exception
event had occurred equally well in familiar and to this pattern was Manhattan, for which there
unfamiliar school hallways. Accuracy was mea- were uniformly high recognition scores. Thus
sured as the distance between where an object while subjects were familiar with their immedi-
had previously been dropped by the experi- ate home area, Manhattan is considerably
menter and the child's relocation of that event. well-known as well. This is probably because
A possible explanation for Acredoio et al.'s many people who live in surrounding boroughs
(197S) failure to find familiarity effects is work, shop, and enjoy recreational activities in
that children in their study acquired less fa- Manhattan. In a second study, Milgram and
miliarity with the "familiar" setting, a known Jodelet (1976) asked residents of Paris to
hallway, than the children in the Siegel and draw maps of their city, identify photographs,
Schadler (1977) study, who were probed and rate the 10 most familiar and 10 least
about their classroom, or Evans, Marrero, and familiar districts of the city. Recognition data
Butler's (in press) subjects, who were asked matched the map data very well, particularly
about their residential neighborhood. The for frequently recalled items. The elements of
operationalization of familiarity in the Acre- Paris most frequently recalled and recognized
doio study as amount of exposure illuminates were located in the heart of Paris (e.g.,
an interesting problem in the study of environ- PEtoile, Notre Dame). Familiarity ratings also
mental familiarity. One could live in an area corresponded with these data, as the center
for several years but spend less time exploring districts of the city were rated as most fa-
it than a newcomer. Several recent studies sug- miliar by all subjects. The latter finding is
gest that environmental cognition is strongly somewhat consistent with the explanation of-
affected by actual usage patterns. fered previously for the high recognition ac-
Patterns oj setting use. The relative loca- curacy found for Manhattan scenes in the New
tions of home, work, schools, and shopping York City study. City knowledge also varied
areas strongly affect the extent of individuals' as a function of residential location. Wealthier
knowledge of the immediate geographic en- Parisians who lived in the western sector of
vironment (Horton & Reynolds, 1971). Simi- Paris rated lower-class areas on the eastern
larly, cognitive representations of downtown sector as least familiar and knew less about
urban areas differ in their overall distribution the eastern sector. The opposite occurred for
of detail as a roughly linear function of dis- poorer Parisians' ratings and knowledge.
tance from where individuals work or live to Therefore, the extent of actual setting con-
the downtown (Lynch, 1977; Saarinan, Note tact as well as overall length of residence may
S). Finally, Holahan (1978) demonstrated affect human knowledge of settings. Future
that students draw more complete, detailed research on environmental learning should
maps of campus areas they actually use more specifically examine the contributions of spe-
often. Displacement of the perceived center of cific setting experience and overall length of
campus was also skewed toward a student's residence. The two variables clearly correlate,
usual entry point onto campus. but they are not necessarily synonymous. Fur-
There is also evidence that recognition mem- thermore, the effects of environmental contact
ory for components of settings is related to may interact with other individual charac-
actual experience as well as to general residen- teristics. Kozlowski and Byrant (1977) found
tial history. Banerjee (1971) reported that that people who had a better sense of direction
the longer a person lived in Boston, the greater (self-ratings validated against actual orienta-
the number of photographs of the city they tion and map tasks) showed greater learning
could correctly identify and locate. Milgram et of a novel environment than did individuals
al. (1972) asked residents of various boroughs with a poorer sense of direction. Subjects were
of New York City to identify color slides of led through an unfamiliar tunnel five different
randomly chosen pictures of the various times. After the initial incidental trial and
boroughs. Residents of each borough recognized Trials 2, 3, and 5, they were asked to draw an
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 275

arrow on a small diagram of the tunnel en- could argue that as the number of paths be-
trance from the starting point to the end. The tween a given set of points in space increases,
picture indicated only the first leg of the tun- there is increased restriction on the possible lo-
nel and did not include several remaining cations of items in space. Given a large enough
changes in direction. They also estimated the set of points, if the relative position of items
relative direction and distance from the tunnel is accurate, then each item can only exist in
entrance to the end point. Persons with better one particular locus in space.
sense of direction significantly improved their Finally, two additional familiarity issues
performance with repeated exposures to the warrant research. First, the operationalization
tunnel, whereas those with poorer sense of of environmental familiarity. Many researchers
direction did not. have equated familiarity with time periods
Summary. Research on environmental fa- (months, years), ignoring experiential differ-
miliarity has focused on two major issues: ences in setting exposure across different per-
the kinds of elements individuals rely on to sons. Second, most of the familiarity research
learn new environments and changes in ac- to date has examined large, cross-sectional dif-
curacy that occur with increased setting ex- ferences in time. There is need for more fine-
posure. Competing hypotheses have proposed grained, longitudinal analysis that examines
that landmarks or paths serve as primary, ini- the same individual's environmental learning
tial navigational cues in environmental learn- over smaller periods of time.
ing. Whether people learn to orient in a new
setting primarily by landmarks or by pathways
Gender
may depend on the physical structure of the
setting. Familiarity research should more Most research on sex differences in spatial
carefully examine the interplay between cognition has found few differences until ado-
physical structures and learning. lescence, with a slight male advantage emerg-
Several studies have consistently found that ing (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Nearly all of
more changes occur in Euclidean accuracy this research, however, has used two dimen-
over time than in the accuracy of relative ob- sional spatial tasks (paper-and-pencil tests)
ject location or clusters of objects in space. and has not investigated three-dimensional,
Both the changes in accuracy and the kinds of spatial comprehension at real scale. Most cog-
elements used in learning environments re- nitive mapping research has found no sex dif-
semble the developmental changes noted ear- ferences in environmental knowledge based on
lier in orientation cues and spatial representa- sketch maps (Francescato & Mebane, 1973;
tion. The importance of landmarks in initial Maurer & Baxter, 1972; Orleans & Schmidt,
environmental learning as fixed frames of 1972), recall of roadside information (Carr &
reference and shifts in accuracy from good Schissler, 1969), or in locating objects in real
intracluster but poor intercluster accuracy to space with a sighting tube (Hardwick et al.,
more precise Euclidean accuracy with experi- 1976). Furthermore, Kozlowski and Bryant
ence are both similar to the developmental (1977) have shown that sex is unrelated to
trends discussed in the previous section. self-ratings of sense of direction that were a
Whether these age or experience trends re- potent predictor of performance in maze learn-
flect qualitative shifts in spatial cognition or ing, locating the direction of familiar buildings
quantitative, scalar changes is a point of con- on a schematic diagram, and accuracy in map
troversy. Several studies suggest that memory drawing.
for exact object location in the environment Orleans and Schmidt (1972), however,
improves with experience but that the rela- found that males constructed sketch maps us-
tive position of objects in space is accurately ing base map coordinates that were provided,
comprehended with little experience. The op- whereas women used their own home as a
erational distinctions among these Piagetian fixed referent system and largely ignored the
stages may reflect scalar differences instead abstract coordinates provided. Appleyard
of distinct stages of spatial cognition. One (1976) also found that men drew slightly
276 GARY W. EVANS

more accurate and extensive city maps than Pradham (1967) asked 10-15-year-olds in
did women, which he attributed to greater Nepal and the United States to draw maps of
travel and exposure in the city. Similar dif-their route from home to school. They found
ferences among the sketch maps of adolescent that the Nepalese maps were generally less
functional and less cartographic in form, rely-
girls and boys have been related to setting ex-
ing more on a pictorial mode that depicted line
posure. In cultures in which girls' home ranges
were restricted (Argentina, Mexico), their drawings of objects (house, child, school)
neighborhood sketch maps were smaller and without connecting routes. They suggested
less accurate than the boys'. For cultures inthat these differences were because of dif-
which all adolecents were given similar home ferential experience with maps. Blaut et al.
range privileges (Australia, Poland), no sex (1970) reported that 6-year-olds in the United
differences in mapping were apparent (Lynch, States and Puerto Rico were equally adept at
1977). Hart (1979) also found a strong posi-interpreting aerial photographs, including mea-
tive relationship between the accuracy and sures of object identification and navigational
extent of children's sketch maps and the ex- problem solving. Finally, Maurer and Baxter
tent of their home range. In the New England (1972) compared neighborhood maps of
town he studied, girls' ranges were more re- lower-class black, Chicano, and white children
stricted and their map smaller and less ac- living in the same neighborhood. Several
curate than those of boys. ethnic differences emerged, including greater
Siegel and colleagues (Herman & Siegel, neighborhood extent in white children's maps,
1978; Siegel & Schadler, 1977) in their de- use of more human-made structures by white
velopmental research also found some sex dif-and Chicano children and more natural fea-
ferences in environmental cognition. In the tures by black children. (The total number of
model classroom study, kindergarten boys elements used was similar.) Black children
drew their home first more often and covered
were more accurate than girls in both relative
and exact object placement in the model. Whena larger percentage of their maps with it. Fi-
children worked on models in a small, clearlynally, significantly more white children drew
bounded space, as opposed to a large, open a larger map of the entire city of Houston
when asked to do so. The authors suggest that
space, sex differences were absent. Second- and
fifth-grade boys, however, performed better some of the unique aspects of the white chil-
dren's data were due to the fact that they had
than girls in reconstructing a model in a large,
unbounded space. The relatively low overall significantly more friends farther from their
home and more frequently attended school
error rate in the simpler, model reconstruction
task may have obscured sex differences in outside the immediate neighborhood.
spatial cognition abilities. In the described studies it is difficult to
Summary. Although there are some trends separate class from cultural variables. Sev-
suggesting male superiority in spatial cognition
eral studies suggest class differences in en-
for small-scale stimuli, the preponderance ofvironmental cognition that may result from
evidence from real-scale spatial tasks indi- differential setting exposure. Orleans (1973)
discovered that an upper-class, professional
cates few sex differences in environmental cog-
group drew much broader, more accurate maps
nition. Furthermore, when sex differences have
been noted, they can often be explained by of Los Angeles than both middle- and lower-
differences in the extent of neighborhood class respondents, whose maps were restricted
exposure. and accurate only for their immediate environ-
ment. Orleans argued that these differences
emanated from the greater breadth of social
Class and Culture
contacts that the upper class had. He found
Much of the research in this area has fo- that the upper-class subjects were significantly
cused on class differences in environmental more likely to have close friends outside of
cognition, although a few cross-cultural in- their immediate neighborhood than were sub-
vestigations have been conducted. Dart and jects in the other groups. Appleyard (1976)
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 277

found the opposite result in his research in travel mode effects comparing bus and auto-
Ciudad Guayana, Venezuela. Lower-class mobile users, which in turn was correlated to
people had more complex maps that indicated class. Bus riders generally saw more of the
more detailed knowledge of the city than did city, especially off-the-road features, but had
the upper class, although the latter group a poorer sense of the overall configuration of
more accurately coordinated the interrelation- the city. Similar data have been reported by
ships among distinct districts of the city. Beck and Wood (1976).
Appleyard suggested that the greater knowl- Summary. Class and cultural differences
edge of the lower class was explained by their in environmental cognition may reflect differ-
daily travel experiences. The wealthy lived ent cognitive styles. Alternatively, they may
near the major places of employment and simply be explained by environmental experi-
rarely traveled to lower-class areas of the city. ence, particularly as affected by extent of set-
The poor traveled across town each day to the ting exposure and travel mode. Furthermore,
factories, offices, and homes of the wealthy in individuals in some cultures and/or classes
which they worked, thus gaining wider city have had little map exposure, which appears
exposure. to affect sketch map production. Thus at
Francescato and Mebane (1973) reported present it is difficult to demonstrate cultural
that lower-class residents of Milan and Rome, or class differences per se in environmental
Italy, drew maps that covered less area and cognition. Research in this area must pay
had fewer elements (especially districts) than closer attention to variables such as map ex-
middle-class residents. They also found that a perience, travel mode, and extent of home
greater number of lower-class individuals re- range.
fused to draw maps of their city. Magana
(1978), who examined class differences in
Physical Setting Components
Guadalajara, Mexico, also found that lower-
class people were less likely to draw maps of Unfortunately, insufficient attention has
their city and had a higher percentage of un- been given to the impact of physical compo-
scorable maps (less than four elements) than nents of settings on environmental cognition
upper-class residents. Furthermore, those
(Wohlwill, 1976). Furthermore, the interac-
lower-class maps that were scorable were less tion of the components of physical settings
accurate and had fewer elements than did the
and personal variables has been neglected.
maps of the upper class. A free-listing tech- Existing research has focused primarily on
nique, however, yielded no class differences in
two variables: environmental structure and
number of elements or willingness to do the
landmarks.
task. Cultural differences in environmental Environmental structure. Urban planners
cognition may be due to fundamental differ- have stressed the importance of city structure
ences in cognitive style. Appleyard (1976) in environmental cognition (Appleyard, 1976;
suggests that analytical styles are more com- Lynch, 1960), with particular emphasis on
mon in developed societies, whereas people in
regular, well-defined path systems. Some em-
underdeveloped societies use more relational
pirical data support these concerns. De Jonge
modes of thinking. The analytic mode ab-
(1962) found that residents of cities with
stracts bits of information from settings and is
more regular (i.e., linear, parallel, and perpen-
stimulus centered, whereas the relational mode dicular configuration) street grid patterns
operates more concretely, focusing on general drew more complete, accurate city maps.
impressions of the global setting, without ap-
Tzamir (Note 4) has extended this earlier
preciation of the abstract relationships among work by varying the similarity of pathway
various parts. Unfortunately, no data are cur- distances and pathway angles of intersect in a
rently available on this issue. scale model. Subjects viewed videotapes made
In addition to the amount of experience, by a camera that moved through the model,
the type of experience in the setting may be simulating a drive around the city, and were
important. Appleyard (1976) found significant then asked to draw the model from memory.
278 GARY W. EVANS

For this task they were provided lists of path walked through. They found that the addition
and node names that had been on the model. of landmarks to the environment significantly
Incorrect or absent path linkages or basic improved the recall of 3- and 4-year-olds but
topological distortions of path configurations not of 8-year-olds. Acredolo (1977) also ex-
were scored as structural distortions. Con- amined the effect of landmarks on children's
sistent with Lynch's hypothesis, models with frames of reference in making orientation
the least variability in path distances and judgments. In this study, 3-, 4-, and 5-year-
angles of intersect had the fewest structural olds were tested in a room that had either no
errors. One problem with de Jonge's research landmarks or a few landmarks. Without land-
is that other variables such as complexity may marks, 3- and 4-year-olds used place respond-
have covaried with street grid pattern regu- ing (object frame of reference) significantly
larity in cities. Tzamir's study, however, care- less than did the 5-year-old. Landmarks did
fully controlled for other structural param- not affect the behavior of the 5-year-olds.
eters, systematically varying grid structure Landmarks decreased egocentrism and in-
regularity only. creased place responding, however, in the
Zannaras (1976) also explored the effects of younger children. Landmarks may have facili-
city structure on cognitive representations of tated orientation by (a) denoting changes in
the environment, She asked subjects to trace bodily position in the room or (b) differen-
routes from the outskirts to the central core tiating the targets. To investigate these pos-
of three cities on either models or maps. Each sibilities, Acredolo replicated the no-landmark
city represented one of the three major urban condition of the experiment with 3- and 4-
organization patterns: concentric zone, sector year-olcls, In addition, she reminded the chil-
zone, and mixed concentric/sector zone. The dren as they changed position in the room,
major dependent variable was ratings of the that they were now in fact standing in a d i f -
relative importance of environmental features ferent place. This reminder significantly helped
in way finding. Traffic features (railroad cross- the 4-year-old children but not the 3-year-
ings, traffic lights) were most important in the olds.
concentric zone city structure, and land-use Thus for younger children, at least, there
cues (institutional use, buildings) were most is evidence that landmarks facilitate cognitive
predominant for the other zonal organizations. representation of the physical environment.
Thus both the configuration of street grid Older children may not gain as much advan-
patterns and overall city zonal structure can tage from landmarks because they no longer
affect cognitive mapping. Regular street grid depend primarily on topological relationships
patterns apparently facilitate environmental for self-location and orientation in space.
clarity. Furthermore, people may use different Younger children, however, appear to rely
orientation cues to navigate in concentrically more heavily on topological relationships (e.g.,
arranged cities than in cities with sector relative position) and are thus helped more by
structure. accurately placed landmarks. Nevertheless, fa-
Landmarks. Recent empirical work veri- miliarity research with adults, as previously
fies the importance of landmarks in cognitive discussed, indicates the importance of land-
mapping of the physical environment. Siegel marks in learning new settings (Evans, Mar-
and Schadler (1977) asked young children (5- rero, & Butler, in press).
6 years) to construct three-dimensional models Approaching the problem of cognitive map-
of their classrooms. For half of the subjects, ping from a planner's perspective, Appleyard
accurately placed landmarks had been placed (1969) asked the question, What attributes of
in the model. This manipulation significantly buildings enhance their recall? Buildings of
enhanced the accuracy of the model con- high use and/or important symbolic signifi-
struction. cance, buildings with high size contrast to
Acredolo et al. (1975) asked children to re- surroundings, and buildings with sharp, singu-
call where a particular event had occurred in lar contours and bright surfaces were recalled
an interior space that the child had previously more frequently than others. These findings
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 279

were replicated by Pezdek and Evans (1979) streets were shown on a screen 90° to the left.
in a model reconstruction task for both build- At each intersection subjects had to choose
ing recall and recognition memory, provided whether to continue straight ahead or make
that no semantic labels were on the model a left turn and proceed down the side street
buildings. When written labels were pro- that was then simulated. The subject's task
vided (e.g., library), there was no relationship was to reach and enter a freeway entrance.
between the physical features of the buildings The correct solution of the problem entailed
and memory. Additional experiments demon- continued travel on the main road, that is, no
strated that the effects of semantic labeling on branching off to the left. Thus the correct
the relationship between building features and solution was unfortunately confounded with
memory were due to the different coding continued progress on the road and no explora-
strategies that were used when labels were tory behavior. Subjects' behavior was moni-
provided. Pezdek and Evans also found that tored in three conditions: no cues removed
when these labels were available, relocation from slides, high-rise buildings removed, high-
memory for buildings was significantly better rise buildings and freeway structures removed
than when no labels were provided. (ramps, bridges, abutments). Analyses re-
Building location may also affect recall. vealed no differences for the first two condi-
Buildings proximate to important road inter- tions, but in the third condition (both cues
sections or visible from primary traffic arteri- removed) subjects made substantially more
als in the real world (Appleyard, 1970, 1976; incorrect branching off decisions. Based on
Heft, 1979) or positioned close to a road in these findings, the author tentatively concluded
a scale model (Herman & Siegel, 1978) are that proximate cues were more important than
more frequently recalled or more accurately distant high-rise buildings in making path
relocated, respectively. choice decisions in a novel environment. In-
Other efforts have focused on what aspects direct support for this claim was evident in
of settings are remembered or used when the type of cues subjects mentioned in a free-
traveling. Appleyard, Lynch, and Myer (1964) response format, as they were "traveling"
asked subjects to make rapid sketches depict- along the road. For Conditions 1 and 2, the
ing what they perceived as they traveled over most frequently mentioned cues included con-
designated road sectors. Subjects also recalled crete freeway structures, freeway ramps,
features along the route. Of the respondents, changes in road grade, and signs. Both Carr
20%-25% drew the road ahead, IQ°/0-15% and Schissler's (1969) fixation data and Apple-
drew objects overhead and large objects at yard et al.'s (1964) sketch data also suggest
the edge of the road, 5%-lQ% drew distant that distant landmarks are not important in
landmarks, guide rails and other road edge adults' roadside cognition. On the other hand,
details, and 2$%-S% drew signs, layouts, Allen et al. (1978) found, like Jones, that
traffic, and hills. Carr and Schissler (1969) proximate landmarks facilitate environmental
extended this work by examining travelers' cognition in route travel.
eye movements and free recall over the same In conclusion, some data suggest that land-
route. In comparison with Appleyard et al.'s marks are most likely to facilitate environ-
sketch data, Carr and Schissler reported con- mental comprehension in preschool children.
siderably less attention to the road itself. In Furthermore, proximate landmarks facilitate
addition, the amount of time an object was adult learning in novel settings The memora-
looked at was highly correlated (.61) to item bility of landmarks may be enhanced by cer-
recall. Furthermore, the degree of correlation tain physical features such as size, shape, and
among what different subjects actually looked functional uniqueness.
at was relatively high (.41). Summary. More regularly structured set-
In another study subjects viewed sequen- tings with ordered pathways that meet at per-
tial slides that depicted a drive down a novel pendicular angles are more readily compre-
but typical urban street in Los Angeles (Jones, hended. The importance of landmarks, particu-
1972). At appropriate intervals five cross larly for younger children in environmental
280 GARY W. EVANS

cognition, has been shown in several studies. At the urban scale, landmarks that are dis-
Individuals more than 6 years of age are helped tinctive in size, color, or form, are functionally
by proximate landmarks when learning a new unique, or are frequently used are more easily
setting. More empirical research is needed on remembered (Appleyard, 1970, 1976). Land-
how specific aspects of the configuration, com- marks are also important in learning novel
position, and location of elements contribute environments (Evans, Marrero, & Butler, in
to environmental legibility. Insufficient atten- press; Heft, 1979). Landmarks in interior
tion has also been given to other setting vari- spaces help orientation, particularly for pre-
ables, including boundary clarity and complex- school children (Acredolo, 1977; Acredolo et
ity. Finally, researchers have generally ignored al., 1975). Furthermore, building designs with
the potential influence of social meaning and greater visual differentiation among various
symbolism in environmental cognition. How subsections and with more regular floor plans
we feel about a place and its historical and (e.g., interior hallways and stairs parallel on
cultural significance may influence cognitive all floors) are more easily remembered by
processing (Appleyard, 1979; Moore, 1979). adults (Weisman, 1979). Color coding of
building interiors also enhances legibility, In-
Practical Applications dividuals who learned the interior of an un-
familiar building that had been color coded
Architecture and Planning performed better on actual way-finding tasks
in the building, floor plan recall and recogni-
Lynch's (1960) concept of legibility has tion tasks, and target sighting tasks using
had a profound influence on the fields of plan- a surveyors transit than did persons who
ning and architecture. Appleyard and his col- learned the building interior without the color
leagues, for example, planned the major new coding (Evans, Fellows, Zorn, & Doty, in
city of Ciudad Guyana, Venezuela, based on press).
legibility principles (Appleyard, 1976). Legi-
Planners and other design professionals have
bility is "the ease with which its [the city's]
implicitly assumed that physical settings that
parts can be recognized and can be organized
facilitate the formation of good cognitive maps
into a coherent pattern" (Lynch, 1960, pp.
are preferable (Appleyard, 1976; Lynch,
2-3). Lynch proposed various systematic
1960). Unfortunately, few investigators have
changes in physical city structure that would
examined the relationship between environ-
enhance urban form, particularly the develop-
mental legibility and preference or feelings of
ment of regular city street patterns. As previ-
personal satisfaction, competence, and so forth.
ously discussed, several studies have found that
A notable exception is research by S. Kaplan
path structures with parallel streets and per-
(1973a, 1975). He found that setting features
pendicular intersections are more readily com-
that enhance map formation also increase
prehended (de Jonge, 1962; Tzamir, Note 4).
preference. Two key concepts are coherence
Lynch also hypothesized that distinct, easily
and moderate uncertainty. Structural features
visible landmarks and clearly bounded city
that provide coherence include continuous
districts would enhance legibility. Two limita-
texture gradients, thematic color or graphic
tions of improving urban legibility are first,
patterns, and variable but identifiable physical
the danger of making environments so routine
forms; moderate uncertainty is provided by
or boring that prediction and exploration be-
variety, moderate complexity, moderate spa-
come trivial (S. Kaplan, 1973a, 1973b). Sec-
ciousness, and occasional structural irregu-
ond, an overemphasis on physical aspects of
larities.
settings per se may ignore the symbolic mean-
ing of spaces to various people. Both Apple-
yard (1979) and Moore (1979) have noted Education
the paucity of research on how individuals'
interpretive, symbolic labeling of geographic Two researchers have developed preliminary
places might affect their cognitive representa- educational programs to enhance children's
tions of settings. cognitive-mapping abilities. Pick (Note 1)
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 281

found that preschoolers who received a short, from maps. Significant individual difference in
map-training curriculum performed slightly map-reading performance and geographic
better than controls in an unrelated task in knowledge revealed that better map readers
which they had to locate where a particular use more active learning strategies. Good map
event had occurred in interior hallways near readers more accurately determine what in-
their classroom (Acredolo et al., I97S) and formation they already know, focusing atten-
made fewer errors in locating objects on maps. tion on unlearned information.
Training consisted of asking children to indi- With the exception of a few studies (R.
cate where and in what order various objects Kaplan, 1976; Kozlowski & Bryant, 1977;
were located on models or photographic and Thorndyke & Stasz, 1980), cognitive mapping
map representations of the models. R. Kaplan has been treated exclusively as a dependent
and colleagues (Devlin, 1973; R. Kaplan, variable. Researchers need to examine cog-
1976) used picture maps, contour maps, and nitive mapping abilities as an independent
aerial photography, integrated with game for- variable. Does greater knowledge of a place
mats, to prepare junior high school students make a difference in self-esteem, competence,
for subsequent exploration of a novel, natural or the likelihood that one will explore a set-
area. Games included finding a hidden loca- ting? Furthermore, are people with orienta-
tion through a series of questions with a part- tion problems adversely affected by their de-
ner, determining which paths were absent from ficiency? Both the visually handicapped and
one's own map by verbal exchange with a recently de-institutionalized groups such as
game partner who had complementary missing psychiatric patients may have reduced self-
paths, and general probes about the location confidence and may avoid exploring their en-
of specific elements and features of the setting. virons because of fear of getting lost. Mobility
In general, children playing these games ex- training for the blind, for example, explicitly
hibited more confidence exploring actual set- trains mapping techniques to enhance both
tings and found the experience more positive self-confidence and orientation skills (U.S.
and less frightening than did controls. Finally, Department of the Interior, Note 6).
computer-assisted map displays may provide
important techniques for enhancing geographic
knowledge. Collins, Adams, and Pew (1978) Conclusions
developed an interactive, computer map dis-
play technique that significantly increased Human beings know a considerable amount
about the content and location of information
high school students' knowledge of world
in real-world settings they experience. Yet we
geography.
know little about how this information is
Cartographers have also become interested
processed. Environmental psychologists study-
in the implications of environmental cogni-
ing this question have relied primarily on
tion research for map design (Robinson &
questionable methodologies such as hand draw-
Petchenik, 1976). Bronzaft, Dobrow, and
ings and small-scale models. In addition, theo-
O'Hanlon (1976), for example, examined the
retical development has been minimal, with
effectiveness of New York City subway maps
largely descriptive taxonomies of the geo-
by assigning recent city residents actual trip
graphic features contained in cognitive maps.
segments of varying difficulty on the subways.
Cognitive psychologists, on the other hand,
Almost all subjects felt insecure traveling and
have ignored the study of real-world spatial
made numerous navigational errors including
memory. They have developed more rigorous
choosing a nonoperative train, transferring at methodologies and sophisticated models of cog-
incorrect stations, and confusing similarly nitive representation but used simplistic, out-
named subway stops. The data strongly indi- of-context stimuli. Very recently these two
cated that present subway maps and other approaches have begun to merge with more
graphic aids are inadequate. Thorndyke and rigorous studies of how information in actual
Stasz (1980) have recently investigated the geographic settings is processed. More thor-
strategies people use to acquire knowledge ough psychometric examination of predomi-
282 GARY W. EVANS

nant cognitive-mapping methodologies is criti- of objects within small, proximate clusters


cal. High-priority issues include comparisons follows but without good intercluster accuracy.
among different accuracy rating scales, analy- Next, children can comprehend both intra-
sis of model and drawing procedures, with and intercluster spatial relationships, and then
particular attention given to comparisons they acquire knowledge of the exact location
among processing in actual settings and in of objects in space. There is insufficient data
modeled or photographic simulations of the on the elderly to draw conclusions about their
same settings. We also need to more care- environmental cognition abilities. Two impor-
fully consider procedural issues that may in- tant limitations of these conclusions are (a)
fluence data production. Recent applications are these stages actually qualitatively distinct,
of chronometric paradigms, scaling algorithms, or do they simply reflect quantitative refine-
and quantifiable orientation tasks in situ are ments in precision, and (b) to what extent do
preliminary steps in the right direction. the developmental differences reflect proce-
Many conceptual questions remain unre- dural artifacts? Comprehension of instructions,
solved. First, we cannot determine at this and reliance on drawings or small-scale mod-
time whether cognitive representations of els may obscure developmental differences in
realistic, large-scale settings consist primarily environmental cognition. These procedural
of propositional, analogical, or some combina- cautions apply especially to the spatial repre-
tion of information. Cognitive maps seem to sentation studies. Several actual way-finding
function as schemata on at least two levels. studies replicate the developmental frames of
Generically we approach settings with gen- reference sequence posited by Piaget. More
eral, prototypic expectations about the loca- research is needed on the effects of procedural
tion and relative spatial positioning of certain variables in developmental studies of cogni-
parts of settings. We assume, for example, tive mapping.
that streets are parallel and intersect at right Amount of experience in a setting is an im-
angles. We also selectively represent particular, portant variable in environmental cognition
familiar settings. Our general knowledge of research. Many of the gender and cross-cul-
even highly familiar settings is incomplete and tural differences found in environmental cog-
uneven. Once we form an initial, highly sche- nition research can be more parsimoniously
matic model of a place, subsequent geographic explained by the extent of actual setting ex-
knowledge is largely assimilated within the ploration permitted to various subgroups of
framework of that initial structure. On the populations. Although it is clear that we gain
other hand, comparative judgments about knowledge of a place with continued exposure,
relative distance or spatial interposition of we cannot precisely describe these increments
well-known loci may operate in a manner in knowledge. Conflicting data suggest two hy-
largely analogous to how that information is potheses on the physical cues people use to
actually configured in real space. learn to orient in a new setting. Some studies
Developmental research on environmental suggest initial reliance on landmarks as orien-
cognition provides some support for Piaget's tation aids with subsequent path learning em-
analysis of an orderly stage acquisition of bedded within the initial landmark network.
knowledge about topological, projective, and Other researchers have found the opposite se-
Euclidean spatial properties. Frame of refer- quence. At least two logical possibilities for
ence data show that young children use ego- resolving the contradictory data are apparent.
centric cues to orient in space, followed by re- First, the type of orientation cues used to
liance on relative position to one and then to learn a setting may depend on the physical
multiple fixed points in space. Finally, com- configuration of the physical environment it-
prehension of space as a coordinate system self. Certain physical cues in a setting may
emerges. Spatial representation research sug- direct our spatial learning strategies by the
gests the following related sequence: Initial opportunities they provide. For example, visu-
learning is strongly tied to direct motoric ex- ally distinctive buildings placed near major
perience; knowledge of the relative location path intersections may lend themselves to a
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 283

landmark-dominant learning strategy. A sec- complex, confusing. Further, how do other


ond logical possibility for resolving the com- physical elements like boundaries and districts
peting learning hypotheses is that perhaps affect knowledge of real-world settings?
some people are landmark dependent in their As psychologists, our focus has largely been
learning strategies, whereas others are path on information processing within the head,
dependent. Previously successful orientation forgetting that variables related to the physi-
experiences in unfamiliar settings could selec- cal structure of stimuli affect cognitive proces-
tively reinforce an individualized orientation sing. The study of environmental cognition is
strategy for learning to orient in new settings. a relatively new area of psychology that pro-
Several researchers have concluded that vides potentially important conceptual links
systematic changes in accuracy occur with in- among environmental psychology, cognitive
creasing environmental familiarity. More psychology, urban planning, and geography.
changes occur in Euclidean accuracy over time Research on environmental cognition should
than in the accuracy of relative object loca- provide further impetus for the study of in-
tion. Although these accuracy trends in en- formation processing under more realistic,
vironmental learning seem stable, these Piage- ecologically valid conditions.
tian-like distinctions may reflect quantitative,
scalar differences in spatial accuracy. Better
exact location accuracy of objects in space Reference Notes
with increasing familiarity may be due to 1. Pick, H. L. Mapping children-mapping space.
greater restrictions on the possible locations of Paper presented at the meeting of the American
items in space with more knowledge of alter- Psychological Association, Honolulu, Hawaii, Sep-
native path linkages in a given set of points tember 1972.
in space. 2. Pick, H. L., Acredolo, L., & Gronscth, M. Chil-
dren's knowledge of the spatial layout of their
A final familiarity issue worthy of further /tomes. Paper presented at the meeting of the
investigation is the issue of how familiarity is Society for Research in Child Development, Phila-
operationalized. We need to examine more delphia, Pa., March 1973.
carefully the relationships between actual set- 3. Ohta, R., Walsh, D., & Krauss, I. Spatial perspec-
ting contact and the acquisition of knowledge. tive-taking ability in young and elderly adults.
Paper presented at the American Psychological
It is the actual amount and kind of setting Association, San Francisco, September 1977.
exploration that really matters and not simply 4. Tzamir, Y. The impact of spatial regularity and
how long we have lived in a place. irregularity on cognitive mapping (Tech. Rep.)
We know little about how physical variables Haifa, Israel: Technion-Israel Institute of Tech-
affect environmental cognition. Landmarks ap- nology, Center for Urban and Regional Studies,
December 197S.
pear to be important in learning new environ-
5. Saarinan, T. Perception of environment (Commis-
ments and as aids for young children's orien- sion on College Geography, Resource Paper No.
tation in large-scale spaces. There is some evi- S). Washington, D.C.: Association of American
dence that more regularly structured path Geographers, 1969.
networks (90° intersects) are more easily 6. U.S. Department of the Interior. Experimental
comprehended. Several issues remain unex- maps of D.C. area for visually handicapped. News
release, Washington, D.C., January 26, 1979.
plored. How does the symbolic meaning of
places affect our knowledge of them, and does
historical or cultural significance affect our References
knowledge of a setting? Although it is intui-
tively reasonable to suspect that emotion Acredolo, L. P. Frames of reference used by chil-
affects cognition, with few exceptions psycholo- dren for orientation in unfamiliar spaces. In G.
Moore & R. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental
gists have largely compartmentalized thinking knowing. Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutchinson
and feeling into separate topics of inquiry. & Ross, 1976.
What is the relationship between legibility and Acredolo, L. P. Developmental changes in the abil-
ity to coordinate perspectives of a large-scale en-
preference? If a setting is too simple, too vironment. Developmental Psychology, 1977, 13,
easily comprehended, it may be boring, if too 1-8.
284 GARY W. EVANS

Acredolo, L. P., Pick, H. L., & Olsen, M. Environ- Collins, A., Adams, M. J,, & Pew, R. W. Effective-
mental differentiation and familiarity as deter- ness of an interactive map display in tutoring
minants of children's memory for spatial location. geography. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Developmental Psychology, 1975, 11, 495-501. 1978, 70, 1-7.
Allen, G., Siegel, A., & Rosinski, R. The role of per- Dart, F. E., & Pradham, P. L. The cross-cultural
ceptual context in structuring spatial knowledge. teaching of science. Science, 1967, 155, 649-656.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Day, J. D. Veridical and inferential memory for the
Learning and Memory, 1978, 4, 617-630. spatial layout of a small house. Unpublished mas-
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. F. Human associative ter's thesis, University of Illinois at Urbana-Cham-
memory. New York: V. H. Winston, 1973. paign, 1977.
Andrews, H. F. Home range and urban knowledge of de Jonge, D. Images of urban areas: Their structure
school-age children. Environment and Behavior, and psychological foundations. Journal of the
1973, 5, 73-86. American Institute of Planners, 1962, 28, 266-276.
Anooshian, L. J., & Wilson, K. L. Distance distortion De Lisi, R., Locker, R., & Youniss, J. Anticipatory
in memory for spatial locations. Child Develop- imagery and spatial operations. Developmental
ment, 1977, 48, 1704-1707. Psychology, 1976, 12, 298-310.
Appleyard, D. A. Why buildings are known. En- Devlin, A. S. Some factors in enhancing knowledge
vironment and Behavior, 1969, 1, 131-156. of a natural area. In W. Preiser (Ed.), Environ-
Appleyard, D. A. Styles and methods of structuring mental design research (Vol. 2). Stroudsburg, Pa.:
a city. Environment and Behavior, 1970, 2, 100- Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, 1973.
116. Devlin, A. S. The small town cognitive map: Adjust-
Appleyard, D. Planning a pluralistic city. Cambridge, ing to a new environment. In G. T. Moore & R. G.
Mass.: MIT Press, 1976. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental knowing. Strouds-
Appleyard, D. The environment as a social symbol. burg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, 1976.
Journal of the American Planning Association, Dirks, J., & Neisscr, U. Memory for objects in real
1979, 45, 143-153. scenes: The development of recognition and re-
Appleyard, D., Lynch, K., & Myer, J. R. The view call. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
from the road. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1977, 23, 315-328.
1964. Downs, R., & Stea, D. Image and environment. Chi-
Baird, J. Studies of the cognitive representation of cago: Aldine, 1973.
spatial relations. Journal of Experimental Psychol- Evans, G. W., Fellows, J., Zorn, M., & Doty, K.
ogy: General, 1979, 108, 90-106. Cognitive mapping and architecture. Journal of
Banerjee, T. K. Urban experience and the develop- Applied Psychology, in press.
ment of city image: A study in environmental per- Evans, G. W., Marrero, D., & Butler, P. Environ-
ception and learning (Doctoral dissertation, Massa- mental learning and cognitive mapping. Environ-
chusetts Institute of Technology, 1971). Disserta- ment and Behavior, in press.
tion Abstracts International, 1971, 32, 335. Evans, G. W., & Pezdek, K. Cognitive mapping:
Beck, R. J., & Wood, D. Cognitive transformation Knowledge of real-world distance and location in-
of information from urban geographic fields to formation. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
mental maps. Environment and Behavior, 1976, 8, Human Learning and Memory, 1980, 6, 13-24.
199-238. Fishbein, H. D., Lewis, S., & Keiffer, K. Children's
Blaut, J. M., McCleary, G., & Blaut, A. Environ- understanding of spatial relations. Developmental
mental mapping in young children. Environment Psychology, 1972, 7, 21-33.
and Behavior, 1970, 2, 335-349. Flavell, J., Botkin, P., Fry, C., Wright, J., & Jarvis,
Blaut, J. M., & Stea, D. Mapping at the age of three. P. The development of role-taking and communi-
Journal of Geography, 1974, 73, 5-9. cation skills in children. New York: Wiley, 1968.
Borke, H. Piaget's mountains revisited: Changes in Francescato, D., & Mebane, W. How citizens view
the egocentric landscape. Developmental Psychol- two great cities: Milan and Rome. In R. Downs
ogy, 1975, 11, 240-243. £ D. Stea (Eds.), Image and environment. Chi-
Bronzaft, A., Dobrow, S., & O'Hanlon, T. Spatial cago: Aldine, 1973.
orientation in a subway system. Environment and Golledge, R. G. Methods and methodological issues
Behavior, 1976, S, 575-594. in environmental cognition research. In G. Moore
Byrne, R. Memory for urban geography. Quarterly & R. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental knowing.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1979, 31, 147- Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross,
154. 1976.
Carr, S., & Schissler, D. The city as a trip: Percep- Golledge, R. G. Multidimensional analysis in the
tual selection and memory in the view from the study of environmental behavior and environmental
road. Environment and Behavior, 1969, 1, 7-36. design. In I. Altman & J. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human
Coie, J. D., Costanzo, P. R., & Farnill, D. Specific behavior and environment (Vol. 2). New York:
transitions in the development of spatial perspec- Plenum Press, 1977.
tive-taking ability. Developmental Psychology, Golledge, R. G., Rivizzigno, V. L., & Spector, A.
1973, 9, 167-177. Learning about a city: Analysis by multidimen-
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 285

sional scaling. In R. G. Golledge & G. Rushton Kaplan, S. Cognitive maps in perception and thought.
(Eds.), Spatial choice and spatial behavior. Colum- In R. Downs & D. Stea (Eds.), Image and en-
bus: Ohio State Press, 1976. vironment. Chicago: Aldine, 1973. (b)
Goodnow, J. Children drawing. Cambridge, Mass.: Kaplan, S. An informal model for the prediction of
Harvard University Press, 1977. preference. In E. H. Zube, R. Brush, & ]. Fabos
Gould, P., & White, H. Menial maps. Baltimore, Md.: (Eds.), Landscape assessment: Values, perceptions
Penguin, 1974. and resources. Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutchin-
Gulick, J. Images of an Arab city. Journal of the son & Ross, 1975.
American Institute of Planners, 1963, 29, 179-197. Kaplan, S. Adaptation, structure, and knowledge,
Hardwick, D. A., Mclntyre, C. W., & Pick, H. L. In G. Moore & R. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental
The content and manipulation of cognitive maps knowing. Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutchinson
in children and adults. Monographs of the & Ross, 1976.
Society for Research in Child Development, 1976, Kosslyn, S. M. Information representation in visual
47(3, Serial No. 166). images. Cognitive Psychology, 197S, 7, 341-370.
Hart, R. Children's experience of place. New York: Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., & Reiser, B. J. Visual
Irvington, 1979. images preserve metric spatial information: Evi-
dence from studies of image scanning. Journal of
Hart, R. A., & Moore, G. T. The development of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
spatial cognition: A review. In R. Downs & D.
Performance, 1978, 4, 47-60.
Stea (Eds.), Image and environment. Chicago:
Kosslyn, S. M., Heldmeyer, K. H., & Locklear, E. P.
Aldine, 1973.
Children's drawings as data about internal repre-
Hazen, N., Lockman, J., & Pick, H. The development sentations. Journal of Experimental Child Psy-
of children's representations of large-scale environ- chology, 1977, 23, 191-211.
ment. Child Development, 1978, 49, 623-636. Kosslyn, S. M., Pick, H., & Fariello, G. Cognitive
Heft, H. The role of environmental features in route- maps in children and men. Child Development,
learning: Two exploratory studies of way-finding. 1974, 45, 707-716.
Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Behav- Kosslyn, S. M., & Pomerantz, J. P. Imagery, propo-
ior, 1979, 3, 172-185. sitions, and the form of internal representations.
Herman, J., & Siegel, A. The development of spatial Cognitive Psychology, 1977, 9, 52-76.
representations of large-scale environments. Jour- Kozlowski, L. T., & Bryant, K. J. Sense of direction,
nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1978, 26, spatial orientation and cognitive maps. Journal of
389-406. Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Holahan, C. Environment and behavior. New York: Performance, 1977, 3, 590-S98.
Plenum Press, 1978. Ladd, F. Black youths view their environment:
Horton, F,, & Reynolds, D. Effects of urban spatial Neighborhood maps. Environment and Behavior,
structure on individual behavior. Economic Geog- 1970, 2, 74-99.
raphy, 1971, 47, 36-48. Laurendeau, M., & Pinard, A. The development of
Howard, I. P., & Templeton, VV. B. Human spatial the concept of space in the child. New York: In-
orientation. New York: Wiley, 1966. ternational Universities Press, 1970.
Howard, R. B., Chase, S. D., & Rothman, M. An Lea, G. Chronometric analysis of the method of loci.
analysis of four measures of cognitive maps. In Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Per-
W. Preiser (Ed.), Environmental design re- ception and Performance, 197S, 1, 95-104.
search (Vol. 1). Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutch- Looft, W., & Charles, D. Egocentrism and social in-
inson & Ross, 1973. teraction in young and old adults. Aging and Hu-
Huttenlocher, J,, & Presson, C. Mental rotation and man Development, 1971, 2, 21-28.
the perspective problem. Cognitive Psychology, Lowenthal, D. (Ed.), Environmental perception and
1913,4, 277-299. behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
Ittelson, VV. Environmental perception and contem- 1967.
porary perceptual theory. In W. Ittelson (Ed.), Lynch, K. The image of the city. Cambridge, Mass.:
Environment and cognition. New York: Seminar, MIT Press, 1960.
1973. Lynch, K. (Ed.). Growing up in cities. Cambridge,
Jones, M. M. Urban path-choosing behavior: A Mass.: MIT Press, 1977.
study of environmental cues. In W. Mitchell (Ed.), Maccoby, E., & Jacklin, C. The psychology of sex
Environmental design: Research and practice. Los differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press,
Angeles: University of California Press, 1972. 1974.
Kaplan, R. Way-finding in the natural environment. MacKay, D. B. The effect of spatial stimuli on the
In G. T. Moore & R. G. Golledge (Eds,), Environ- estimation of cognitive maps. Geographical Anal-
mental knowing. Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, ysis, 1976, 8, 439-452.
Hutchinson & Ross, 1976. Magana, J. R. An empirical and interdisciplinary
Kaplan, S. Cognitive maps, human needs, and the test of a theory of urban perception (Doctoral
designed environment. In W. Preiser (Ed.), En- dissertation, University of California, Irvine,
vironmental design research (Vol. 1). Stroudsburg, 1978). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1978,
Pa.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, 1973. (a) 39, 1460B. (University Microfilms No. 78-15,840)
286 GARY W. EVANS

Magana, J. R., Evans, G. W., & Romney, A. K. Pufall, P., & Shaw, R. Analysis of the development
Scaling techniques in the analysis of environmental of children's spatial reference systems. Cognitive
cognition data. Professional Geographer, in press. Psychology, 1973, S, 151-175.
Handler, J. M., & Parker, R. E. Memory for de- Pylyshyn, Z. W. What the mind's eye tells the mind's
scriptive and spatial information in complex pic- brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological
tures. Journal oj Experimental Psychology: Hu- Bulletin, 1973, 80, 1-24.
man Learning and Memory, 1976, 2, 38—48. Robinson, A. H., & Petchenik, B. B. The nature of
Masangkay, Z., et al. The early development of in- maps. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1976.
ferences about the visual percepts of others. Child Rothwell, D, Cognitive mapping of the home en-
Development, 1974, 45, 357-366. vironment (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Maurer, R., & Baxter, J. C. Images of the neighbor- British Columbia, Canada, 1974). Dissertation Ab-
hood and city among black, anglo, and Mexican- stracts International, 1976, 36, 4758A. (Available
American children. Environment and Behavior, on microfiche from the National Library of Can-
1912,4, 351-388. ada, Ottawa, Ontario)
Milgram, S., Greenwald, J., Kessler, S., McKenna, Rubin, K., Attewell, P., Tierney, M., & Tumolo, P.
W., & Waters, J, A psychological map of New Development of spatial egocentrism and conserva-
York City. American Scientist, 1972, 60, 194-200. tion across the life span. Developmental Psychol-
Milgram, S., & Jodelet, D. Psychological maps of ogy, 1973, 9, 432.
Paris. In H. Proshansky, W. Ittelson, & L. Rivlin Schultz, N., & Hoyer, W. Feedback effects on spatial
(Eds.), Environmental psychology (2nd ed.). New egocentrism in old age. Journal of Gerontology,
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1976. 1976, 31, 72-75.
Moore, G. T. Developmental differences in environ- Shemyakin, F. \T. General problems of orientation
mental cognition. In VV. Preiser (Ed.), Environ- in space and space representations. In B. G. Ana-
mental design research. Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, nyev (Ed.), Psychological science in the USSR
Hutchinson & Ross, 1973. (Vol. 1; Tech. Rep. No. 11466). U.S. Office of
Moore, G. T. The development of environmental Technical Reports, Arlington, Va.: 1962. (NTIS
knowing: An overview of an interactional-con- No. TT62-11083)
structivist theory and some data on within-indi- Shepard, R. N. Form, formation, and transformation
vidual development variations. In D. Canter & T. of internal representation. In R. L. Solso (Ed.),
Lee (Eds.), Psychology and the built environment. Information processing and cognition: The Loyola
New York: Halstead Press, 1974. Symposium. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1975.
Moore, G. T. Knowing about environmental know- Siegel, A. W., Herman, J., Allen, G., & Kirasic, K.
ing: The current state of theory and research on The development of cognitive maps of large and
environmental cognition. Environment and Behav- small scale spaces. Child Development, 1979, SO,
ior, 1979, 11, 33-70. 582-585.
Moore, G. T., & Golledge, R. G. (Eds.). Environ- Siegel, A. W., & Schadler, M. Young children's cog-
mental knowing. Stroudsburg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutch- nitive maps of their classroom. Child Develop-
inson & Ross, 1976. ment, 1977, 48, 388-394.
Neisser, U. Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Siegel, A. W., & White, S. H. The development of
Freeman, 1976. spatial representations of large-scale environments.
Nigl, A. J., & Fishbein, H. D. Perception and con- In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child devel-
ception in coordination of perspectives. Develop- opment and behavior (Vol. 10). New York: Aca-
mental Psychology, 1974, 10, 858-866. demic Press, 1975.
Norman, D., & Rumelhart, D. Explorations in cog- Stea, D. Environmental perception and cognition:
nition. San Francisco: Freeman, 1975. Toward a model for mental maps. In G. Coates
Orleans, P. Differential cognition of urban residents: & K. Moffett (Eds.), Response to environment.
Effects of social scale on mapping. In R. Downs Raleigh: North Carolina State University Press,
& D. Stea (Eds.), Image and environment. Chi- 1969.
cago: Aldine, 1973. Stea, D., & Taphanel, S. Theory and experiment on
Orleans, P., & Schmidt, S. Mapping the city: En- the relation between environmental modeling (toy
vironmental cognition of urban residents. In W. play) and environmental cognition. In D. Canter
Mitchell (Ed.), Environmental design: Research & T. Lee (Eds.), Psychology and the built en-
and practice. Los Angeles: University of California vironment. New York: Halstead Press, 1974.
Press, 1972. Stevens, A., & Coupe, P. Distortions in judged spatial
Pezdek, K., & Evans, G. W. Visual and verbal mem- relations. Cognitive Psychology, 1978, 10, 422-437.
ory for objects and their spatial location. Journal Thorndyke, P., & Stasz, C. Individual differences
of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and in procedures for knowledge acquisition from maps.
Memory, 1979, 5, 360-373. Cognitive Psychology, 1980, 12, 137-175.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. The child's conception oj Tolman, E. C. Cognitive maps in rats and men.
space. New York: Norton, 1967. Psychological Review, 1948, 55, 189-208.
Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. The child's Trowbridge, C. C. On fundamental methods of ori-
conception of geometry. New York: Basic Books, entation and imagery maps. Science, 1913, 38, 888-
1960. 897.
ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION 287

Walsh, D., Krauss, I., & Regnier, V. Spatial ability, Wilton, R. Knowledge of spatial relations: The speci-
environmental knowledge, and environmental use: fication of the information used in making in-
The elderly. In L. Liben, A. Patterson, & N. New- ferences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
combe (Eds.), Spatial representation and behavior chology, 1979, 31, 133-146.
across the life span. New York: Academic Press, Wohhvill, J. F. Searching for the environment in
in press. environmental cognition research: A commentary
Weber, R., Brown, L., & Weldon, J. Cognitive maps on research strategy. In G. T. Moore & R. G.
of environmental knowledge and preference in Gotledgc (Eds.), Environmental knowing. Strouds-
nursing home patients. Experimental Aging Re- burg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, 1976.
search, 1978, 4, 157-174. Zannaras, G. The relation between cognitive struc-
Weisman, J. Way-finding in the built environment: ture and urban form. In G. T. Moore & R. G.
A study in architectural legibility (Doctoral dis- Golledge (Eds.), Environmental knowing. Strouds-
sertation, University of Michigan, 1979). Disserta- burg, Pa.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, 1976.
tion Abstracts International, 1979, 40, SOSA. (Uni-
versity Microfilms No. 79-16,843) Received April 13, 1979 •

Anda mungkin juga menyukai