Anda di halaman 1dari 30

YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 42:1–30 (1999)

Evolution of Coalitionary Killing


RICHARD W. WRANGHAM
Department of Anthropology, Peabody Museum Harvard University,
Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138

KEY WORDS chimpanzee; lethal raiding; warfare; assessment

ABSTRACT Warfare has traditionally been considered unique to hu-


mans. It has, therefore, often been explained as deriving from features that
are unique to humans, such as the possession of weapons or the adoption of a
patriarchal ideology. Mounting evidence suggests, however, that coalitional
killing of adults in neighboring groups also occurs regularly in other species,
including wolves and chimpanzees. This implies that selection can favor
components of intergroup aggression important to human warfare, including
lethal raiding. Here I present the principal adaptive hypothesis for explaining
the species distribution of intergroup coalitional killing. This is the ‘‘imbalance-
of-power hypothesis,’’ which suggests that coalitional killing is the expression
of a drive for dominance over neighbors. Two conditions are proposed to be
both necessary and sufficient to account for coalitional killing of neighbors: (1)
a state of intergroup hostility; (2) sufficient imbalances of power between
parties that one party can attack the other with impunity. Under these
conditions, it is suggested, selection favors the tendency to hunt and kill rivals
when the costs are sufficiently low. The imbalance-of-power hypothesis has
been criticized on a variety of empirical and theoretical grounds which are
discussed. To be further tested, studies of the proximate determinants of
aggression are needed. However, current evidence supports the hypothesis
that selection has favored a hunt-and-kill propensity in chimpanzees and
humans, and that coalitional killing has a long history in the evolution of both
species. Yrbk Phys Anthropol 42:1–30, 1999. r 1999 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Coalitionary Killing among Chimpanzees and Other Species .............................................. 4


Species distribution of coalitionary killing ......................................................................... 5
The lethal-raiding problem ................................................................................................. 5
Lethal raiding by chimpanzees ........................................................................................... 5
Territorial defense ............................................................................................................ 6
Border patrols .................................................................................................................. 7
Deep incursions ................................................................................................................ 7
Coalitionary attacks ......................................................................................................... 8
Coalitionary kills .............................................................................................................. 8
The Imbalance-of-Power Hypothesis .................................................................................... 11
Explaining chimpanzee violence ....................................................................................... 11
The significance of power imbalances ............................................................................... 12
Origins of power imbalances ............................................................................................. 12
Group territoriality and the benefits of lethal raiding ..................................................... 14
Sex differences in territoriality and aggressiveness ........................................................ 16
Bonobos: exceptions that support the rule? ...................................................................... 17
The imbalance-of-power hypothesis and the evolution of human warfare ...................... 18
Challenges to the Imbalance-of-Power Hypothesis ............................................................. 20

r 1999 WILEY-LISS, INC.


2 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

Uncertainty in the chimpanzee data ................................................................................ 20


The claim that biology is irrelevant for human warfare .................................................. 21
Implications of the Imbalance-of-Power Hypothesis ........................................................... 22
Chimpanzee and human psychology ................................................................................. 22
The complexity of war ........................................................................................................ 23
The relation between lethal raiding and hunting ............................................................ 24
Morality .............................................................................................................................. 25
Conclusion ............................................................................................................................. 26
Acknowledgments ................................................................................................................. 26
Literature Cited .................................................................................................................... 27

Two related but distinct hypotheses have terms of predation by carnivores and tapho-
proposed that warfare has its origins in nomic processes, and the supposed bone and
pre-human violence. The first is no longer horn weapons were better explained as frag-
supported. This was the so-called ‘‘killer ments produced by carnivores chewing bone
ape’’ hypothesis, which stated that warfare (Brain, 1981; Cartmill, 1993). The killer ape
springs from an aggressive instinct that hypothesis fell into general disrepute in the
began among australopithecines and contin- 1970s, even though the notion that warfare
ued into humans (Dart 1953; Ardrey, 1961, evolved out of complex hunting patterns has
1966; Lorenz, 1966; Tiger, 1969). Raymond not completely died (Morris, 1977; Ferrill,
Dart based this idea on South African homi- 1985).
nid fossils, which he interpreted with in- In the 1980’s a second, unrelated, set of
creasing pessimism after the Second World ideas arose, which I collectively call the
War until eventually concluding that Austra- chimpanzee violence hypothesis (CVH). Like
lopithecus africanus not only hunted other the killer ape hypothesis, the CVH proposes
mammals but also killed adult conspecifics that human warfare is built on pre-human
(Dart, 1953; Dart and Craig, 1959; Cartmill, tendencies. In contrast to the killer ape
1993). At the time of these ideas, intraspe- hypothesis, however, the CVH does not posit
cific killing was considered to be absent in a prior history of hunting, nor an aggressive
other wild mammals (including chimpan- instinct. These and other differences make
zees Pan troglodytes) (Lorenz, 1966). There- the killer ape hypothesis irrelevant to the
fore, killing by australopithecines was con- CVH (Table 1). The remainder of this paper
sidered part of a uniquely hominid suite of is concerned with the CVH, and not with the
characteristics. Lorenz (1966) lent ethologi- killer ape hypothesis with which it has
cal authority to Dart’s ideas by suggesting sometimes been confused (Sussman, 1997).
how killing could evolve. He proposed, for The CVH proposes that selection has fa-
example, that the use of weapons, such as vored a tendency among adult males to
pebble tools, could overcome natural inhibi- assess the costs and benefits of violence, and
tions against killing conspecifics. Lorenz to attack rivals when the probable net ben-
(1966) thus followed Dart in arguing that efits are sufficiently high. It suggests that
human warfare had evolved from australopi- this tendency occurs as a result of similar
thecine aggressive instincts. conditions in the lives of chimpanzee and
The killer ape hypothesis provoked vigor- human ancestors, including a fission-fusion
ous attacks (e.g., Ashley Montagu, 1968). system of grouping, and intergroup hostility.
Much criticism was directed at theoretical It also raises the question of whether lethal
components, such as the claim that humans raiding had a common origin in the ancestor
have an innate aggressive drive that needs of chimpanzees and humans around 5–6
periodic expression. It was empirical evi- mya, or whether it evolved later and indepen-
dence that felled it, however. Most impor- dently in each line.
tantly, the fossils suggestive of intraspecific The CVH was stimulated by observations
violence were convincingly reinterpreted in of male chimpanzees collaborating to kill or
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 3
TABLE 1. Comparison of the killer ape and for at least 5 million years. This idea of an
chimpanzee violence hypotheses
ancient origin of warfare is supported by the
Chimpanzee rarity of coalitionary lethal violence toward
Killer ape violence
hypothesis hypothesis adult conspecifics in other primates, and by
evidence that subsequent to the split with
Lethal violence Important in Important in
human evolu- human evolu- gorillas Gorilla gorilla (Pilbeam, 1996), chim-
tionary history tionary history panzees and humans share a common ances-
Significance of Critical pre- Possible conse-
hunting cursor to intra- quence of tor around 5–6 mya.
specific vio- intraspecific In the first part of this paper, the evidence
lence violence; not a for coalitionary killing by chimpanzees and
necessary pre-
cursor the nature of their intergroup aggression
Mechanism of Instinct Strategic assess- are examined. The principal adaptive expla-
aggression ment
Putative reason Inadvertent Assessment that nation linking chimpanzee and human vio-
for intraspe- breakdown of costs of elimi- lence is then reviewed. This is the imbalance-
cific violence natural inhibi- nating rival of-power hypothesis, which states that
tions are low
Chimpanzees Nonviolent Strategically vio- coalitionary kills occur because of two fac-
considered to lent tors: intergroup hostility, and large power
be
Killing among Assumed to be Known to occur asymmetries between rival parties. After
animals other absent considering separately the costs and ben-
than hominids
Reliance on fossil Critical Relevant, not
efits of lethal raiding among chimpanzees,
evidence critical how the imbalance-of-power hypothesis also
First ancestor of Australopithe- Unknown applies to bonobos (Pan paniscus) and to
humans sup- cines
posed to have humans is assessed.
coalitionary In the third part, objections and problems
violence
Evolutionary Include group Group selection are considered. Arguments are discussed
mechanisms selection appears that deny the relevance of biological argu-
favoring vio- unnecessary ments for understanding human warfare,
lence
including the following claims: warfare is
wholly cultural (e.g., Keeley, 1996); modern
war is too complex for individual aggression
brutally wound other adults (Goodall et al.,
to be important (Hinde, 1993); and, nothing
1979). Most such attacks were directed to-
ward members of neighboring communities, useful can be learned by studying species
in patterns reminiscent of human war raids other than humans, because humans are
(Goodall, 1986). As a result, various authors already known to be violent (Leach, 1968;
raised the possibility of functional parallels Gould, 1996).
and/or evolutionary continuities linking Criticisms directed specifically at the im-
chimpanzee violence and human warfare balance-of-power hypothesis are also dis-
(Trudeau et al., 1981; Otterbein, 1985, 1997; cussed. These include concerns about the
Goodall, 1986; Alexander, 1987, 1989; Wrang- validity of the chimpanzee data (e.g., Power,
ham, 1987, 1999b; Ghiglieri, 1988; van Hooff, 1991; Sussman, 1997) or about the interpre-
1990; Hamburg, 1991; Knauft, 1991; Man- tation of human data (e.g., Knauft, 1991;
son and Wrangham, 1991; Boehm, 1992; van Sponsel, 1996), and claims that data on
der Dennen, 1995; Wrangham and Peterson, bonobos undermine the use of chimpanzees
1996; Boesch and Boesch, 1999). For ex- as a reference species for early hominid
ample, Otterbein (1997, p 253) noted that ancestry (Zihlman, 1997; Stanford, 1998a).
similarities between chimpanzee communi- They also include accusations of genetic
ties and human bands suggest that ‘‘early determinism (e.g., Regal, 1998; Sussman,
man . . . is likely to have been organized into 1997) or societal bias (e.g., Sussman, 1997).
localized groups of related males, groups These are important considerations, but
that engaged in intergroup conflict.’’ If so, they do not invalidate the comparative ap-
Otterbein concluded, warfare has been con- proach. Therefore this article ends with a
tinuous in human and pre-human ancestry brief discussion of the implications of the
4 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

imbalance-of-power hypothesis for human species never form coalitionary alliances in


psychology, warfare, and morality. any context. But coalitions are not a suffi-
cient condition for coalitionary killing. Thus
COALITIONARY KILLING AMONG
many primates form coalitions without any
CHIMPANZEES AND OTHER SPECIES
evidence of adult-killing [e.g., Cercopithecus
Species distribution of coalitionary killing aethiops (Cheney et al., 1988)] or with fatal
Contrary to initial assumptions (Lorenz, fighting known only from dyadic interac-
1966), research in recent decades has re- tions [e.g., Cebus capucinus] (Miller, 1998).
vealed that intraspecific killing occurs in a Indeed, the only nonprimate mammal for
variety of species, commonly following pat- which coalitionary violence is known to be
terns explicable by natural selection theory. commonly responsible for adult deaths is
For example, among primates infanticide is the wolf Canis lupus. In at least three sites,
widely reported, typically committed by non- adults are known to kill other adults at high
relatives (Hausfater and Hrdy, 1984; Palom- rates [Denali (Alaska), Isle Royale (Michi-
bit, 1999). Among spiders, killing of adults gan), and Minnesota] (Mech et al., 1998).
occurs predictably when resources of high For example in Denali, 39–65% of adult
value are at stake (Austad, 1983). Among mortality was due to intraspecific killing,
ants, large imbalances of power increase the based on 22 intraspecific killings recorded
probability of lethal intercolony aggression from 17–20 packs (Mech et al., 1998). This is
(Hölldobler, 1981; Adams, 1990). Obvious the least disturbed study site of wolves. In
parallels can be found among humans (Daly northeastern Minnesota, 43% of wolves not
and Wilson, 1988; Bueno de Mesquita, 1981, killed by humans were killed by other wolves.
1985; Bueno de Mesquita and Lalman, 1992). Killings tended to occur in buffer zones
To some extent, therefore, patterns of hu- (where territories met), which wolves mostly
man killing appear to follow the ordinary avoided (Mech, 1994). These data suggested
patterns of lethal aggression found in other to Mech et al. (1998) that intraspecific kill-
species. ing is a normal consequence of wolf territori-
Not so ordinary, however, is the way that ality.
human killing occurs. Among humans most Occasional coalitional killing of adult con-
killing occurs in warfare, where the predomi- specifics in neighboring groups has also been
nant style of violence is coalitionary. In most recorded among other social carnivores [li-
animals, by contrast, even where aggression ons Panthera leo, spotted hyenas Crocuta
occurs at high rates, lethal violence is dyadic crocuta, cheetahs Acinonyx jubatus (Kruuk,
(one versus one) rather than coalitionary 1972; Caro and Collins, 1986; Goodall, 1986;
(many vs. one, or many vs. many). During Packer et al., 1988; Grinnell et al., 1995)]
rut-fighting among male pronghorn ante- and at least one group-territorial bird [Tas-
lope (Antilocapra americana), for example, manian native hen Gallinula mortierii (Put-
12% of 82 fights over mating rights to es- land and Goldizen, 1998; A. Goldizen per-
trous females led to the death of one or both sonal communication)]. However, the
males (Byers, 1997). Likewise, in different frequency of killing has not been reported
populations of red deer Cervus elaphus, 13– for these species. Outside of mammals, only
29% of adult male mortality came from social insects are known to kill conspecifics
rut-fighting (Clutton-Brock et al., 1982). regularly with coalitional aggression (van
Many similar examples occur. Deaths tend der Dennen, 1995).
to occur in intensely escalated contests in These data suggest that animals can be
which both opponents expose themselves to divided into three major categories. First
high risk of injury, typically because ‘‘a are species in which intraspecific killing of
major part of a contestant’s lifetime repro- adults is rare (e.g., less than 1% of all adult
ductive success is at stake’’ (Enquist and deaths). Most species fall into this category.
Leimar, 1990). But killing is never coalition- Second are those where killing occurs more
ary in these species. frequently [often 10% of more of deaths
The explanation for the widespread ab- (Enquist and Leimar 1990)], but entirely in
sence of coalitionary violence is trivial. Most dyadic interactions. In these species, fatal
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 5

fighting is dangerous. In the third category, Abundant evidence routinely attests to the
killing is also frequent, but differs by being blood-lust of the participants.
polyadic (coalitionary). Furthermore, as ar- Against the notion that men have a ready
gued below, fatal fighting need not be danger- appetite to attack their enemies, combat-
ous for the killers. ants in modern warfare are often reluctant
Ants dominate this last category, which to fight (Hinde, 1993; Ehrenreich, 1997;
contains probably less than 10 mammalian Grossman, 1999). This front-line lack of
species and perhaps no other vertebrates. aggressiveness is understandable because
Chimpanzees and humans (or at least, cer- in modern warfare, unlike intergroup aggres-
tain populations of these species) are the sion in primates, soldiers are organized hier-
only primates known to be frequent coalition- archically and are ordered into battle by
ary killers. A possible additional candidate their superiors, regardless of their personal
is the western red colobus monkey (Colobus motivation. Participation in raids among
badius), for which at least two and possibly pre-state societies, however, is normally vol-
four coalitionary kills were recorded by Sta- untary (Keeley, 1996). Thus, reluctance of
rin (1994). In these cases, coalitions of fe- soldiers under orders does not undermine
males attacked and killed males attempting the more widespread phenomenon of male
to enter their groups. eagerness for fighting.
It is likely that lethal raids also occur in
The lethal-raiding problem some of the carnivore species that engage in
In chimpanzees, humans, and some other intraspecific killing of adults. For example,
animals, coalitionary killing can occur in the lethal raiding is suggested by the report of
context of lethal raiding. Lethal raids are an Mech et al. (1998) that neighbors killed
unusual form of aggression because they do three members of a wolf pack, two others
not escalate from a conflict. Instead, parties disappeared unseen, and the defeated pack’s
of allied males collectively invade a neighbor- territory was taken over by the killers. There
ing territory, seek one or more vulnerable are also reports of spotted hyenas making
neighbors, apparently assess the probability incursions into neighboring territories to
of making a successful attack, conduct a attack neighbors (Goodall, 1986). Among
‘‘surprise’’ attack that leaves one or more invertebrates, patterns similar to lethal raid-
victims dead or dying, then return to their ing occur in a variety of ants (Hölldobler,
own territory. ‘‘Surprise’’ refers to the attack 1981; van der Dennen, 1995).
occurring without any initial conflict, with- Among species other than humans, how-
out escalation from a lower level, and with- ever, lethal raids have been most clearly
out the victim interacting with the oppo- reported in chimpanzees. Evidence of chim-
nents until the attack starts. panzee raiding has been fundamental for
Thus, lethal raids indicate an appetite for the development of the CVH. I therefore
hunting and killing rivals that is akin to review the chimpanzee data in detail.
predation. By contrast, most animal con-
Lethal raiding by chimpanzees
flicts escalate in a stepwise manner that
allows both opponents to assess each other Although lethal raiding among chimpan-
and to withdraw when the risks of losing zees has been described more clearly than
appear too high (Archer and Huntingford, for any other mammal, few cases have been
1994). The ‘‘appetite for lethal raiding’’ there- completely observed. Furthermore, all the
fore requires explanation in different terms detailed observations come from a single
from escalated conflicts. site, Gombe National Park in Tanzania
Among humans, lethal raids are wide- (Goodall, 1986). Of course, lethal raids are
spread in all forms of warfare. For example, expected to be rare, as they must be in any
Keeley (1996) regards small raids and am- long-lived, slowly reproducing species. Nev-
bushes as ‘‘the commonest form of combat ertheless, the concentration of observational
employed in primitive warfare’’ (see also evidence at Gombe has suggested to critics
Turney-High, 1949; van der Dennen, 1995; of the CVH that lethal raiding may have
Maschner and Reedy-Maschner, 1998). been induced in Gombe by unnatural condi-
6 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

TABLE 2. Territorial behavior in chimpanzees and bonobos1


P. t. schweinfurthii P. t. verus P. paniscus
Gombe Mahale Kibale Budongo Taı̈ Wamba Lomako
Territorial defense ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ⫹
Border patrols ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ? ⫹ ⫺ ⫺
Deep incursions ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ? ⫹ ⫺ ⫺
Coalitionary attacks ⫹ ⫹ ? ? ⫹ ⫺ ⫺
Coalitionary kills ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ⫹ ⫺ ⫺ ⫺
Border avoidance ⫹ ? ⫹ ? ⫹ ⫺ ⫺
Peaceful intercommunity association ⫺ ⫺ ⫺ ⫺ ⫺ ⫹ ⫹
1 All long-term studies are included except for Bossou, where the community is isolated by agricultural land from its nearest

chimpanzee neighbors. ‘‘Coalitionary kills’’ refers to adult victims only. ‘‘Coalitionary attacks’’ means non-lethal attacks by several
males on a single victim. Data are from: Gombe (Goodall, 1986); Mahale (Nishida, 1979, 1986; Nishida et al. 1985); Kibale (Chapman
and Wrangham, 1993); Budongo (V. Reynolds, personal communication); Taı̈ (Boesch and Boesch, 1999); Wamba (Kano, 1992;
Hashimoto et al., 1998); Lomako (White, 1996).

TABLE 3. Chimpanzees and bonobos: lethal violence by site1


Site Subspecies Adult deaths Infanticides Years of study
Gombe P. t. schweinfurthii 6 (3) 6 (3) 38
Mahale P. t. schweinfurthii 1 (6) 4 (6) 33
Kibale P. t. schweinfurthii 2 (0) 1 (0) 11
Budongo P. t. schweinfurthii 1 (1) 1 (1) 8
Bossou P. t. verus 0 (0) 0 (0) 22
Taı̈ P. t. verus 0 (0) 0 (0) 19
Wamba P. paniscus 0 (0) 0 (0) 24
Lomako P. paniscus 0 (0) 0 (0) 15
Total 10–20 12–22 170
1 Numbers include (for adult deaths) kills recorded on the basis of direct observation and/or fresh bodies as well as (in parentheses)

those from suspicious disappearances (see Table 5), or (for infanticides) kills observed directly or (in parentheses) inferred from context
(Arcadi and Wrangham 1999, updated for Mahale by Nishida (personal communication). ‘‘Years of study’’ is number of years from
beginning of continuous study until 1998. Bonobo studies have been intermittent.

tions such as reduced habitat, or provision- Six components of chimpanzee intergroup


ing, and therefore that lethal raiding is aggression are especially relevant to lethal
uncharacteristic of chimpanzees more gener- raiding: territorial defense (showing evi-
ally (Power, 1991). dence of hostile intergroup relationships),
There are six study sites in which habitu- border patrols, deep incursions, coalitionary
ation of chimpanzees is sufficiently good to attacks, coalitionary kills, and border avoid-
allow multi-hour observations of known indi- ance (Table 2).
viduals traveling throughout the home range
(Tables 2 and 3). These include four studies Territorial defense. This has been re-
of the eastern subspecies (P. t. schwein- ported in all studies in which intercommu-
furthii), none of the two central subspecies nity relationships have been described, based
(P. t. troglodytes and P. t. vellerosus), and two on some combination of: counter-calling be-
of the western chimpanzee (P. t. verus). In tween parties of neighboring males; rapid
one of the P. t. verus studies, at Bossou, the travel toward a site where an opposing party
study community is ‘‘semi-isolated’’ by loss has been detected; avoidance of opposing
of habitat, separated by several kilometers parties that were obviously larger; charging
from the home ranges of its closest neigh- displays directed toward an opposing party
bors (Sugiyama, 1989; Sugiyama et al., of males; or one party chasing another
1993). Consequently there is no possibility (Nishida, 1979; Goodall, 1986; Boesch and
of territorial behavior or intercommunity Boesch, 1999; V. Reynolds personal commu-
interaction from Bossou. This leaves five nication; Wrangham et al., in preparation).
studies of chimpanzees, varying in duration For example on nine occasions chimpanzees
from 8 to 38 years, that permit observation in Taı̈ have been seen in ‘‘back-and-forth
of intergroup interactions (Table 3). attacks’’ with neighbors, in which all males
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 7

rush toward the opponents, giving loud at- (Wrangham, 1975, Table 5.9). By contrast,
tack calls, and opponents may flee up to 400 in the 5 years following the extinction of the
m. In a further nine cases, males in the front Kahama community (1978–1982), border pa-
line of attack were supported by loud calls trols continued at a rate of 18 per individual
from females in a rear line (Boesch and per year (range 9–27; calculated from Good-
Boesch, 1999). all, 1986, Table 17.1).
Table 2 shows that border patrols have
Border patrols. These are an intrinsic been reported also from Mahale, Kibale, and
component of lethal raiding, because they Taı̈. Border patrols have not been described
put a party of individuals in a position to in detail from Mahale, but key elements of
stalk and to hunt a neighboring victim. border patrols have been reported—includ-
Border patrols are visits to a peripheral ing scouting, and silent and cautious travel,
sector of a home range by a party of males mainly by males, in border areas (Nishida,
that monitors the area. They are initiated 1979, 1990; Nishida et al., 1985). The Kibale
without any immediate contact with mem- evidence comes from border patrols seen at
bers of the neighboring community, and are Kanyawara, involving parties of males inter-
often undertaken with little or no feeding. mittently checking their territorial bound-
According to Goodall (1986) and Boesch and aries (Wrangham et al., in preparation). In
Boesch (1999), they include some or all of Taı̈, border patrols occurred in 29% of 129
the following features: (1) cautious and slow territorial actions, normally involving at
travel around or across the border, including least four males, and included all of the
long periods of gazing toward the neighbor- elements listed above (Boesch and Boesch,
ing home range; (2) nervousness shown to- 1999).
ward unexpected sounds; and (3) inspection
of signs of other chimpanzees, such as dis- Deep incursions. These were included as
carded food wadges, feces, nests, or aban- part of border patrols by Goodall (1986), but
doned termite-fishing tools. I distinguish them because they involve
Border patrols were first reported at deliberate travel into the neighboring terri-
Gombe in 1971 by JD Bygott, who was the tory rather than merely checking of the
first researcher to conduct regular all-day border area. Deep incursions are character-
observations of individual males (Bygott, ized by (1) substantial penetration into the
1979). Most patrols at Gombe were by males neighboring territory, e.g., for one kilometer
of the principal study group, the Kasekela or more; (2) silent and cautious travel dur-
community, but the Kahama males pa- ing periods of moving outwards from own
trolled also (Bygott, 1979; Wrangham, 1975). territory; and (3) noisy and vigorous dis-
Border patrols do not occur every time a plays on return to their own territory. Deep
party reaches the boundary area. In Gombe, incursions have been well described at
for example, patrols occurred during 28% of Gombe and Taı̈. Boesch and Boesch (1999)
134 boundary visits made by parties from found that ‘‘many patrols were probably
the Kasekela community from 1977 to 1982 aimed at finding and attacking strangers. . .
(calculated from Goodall, 1986, Table 17.1). . . . (they) were impressive by the intensity
Their frequency appears to vary as a func- with which the males searched for strang-
tion of relations between particular commu- ers, not only entering deep (into) their terri-
nities. Thus, during and after the 1974– tory, but once even heading backwards to
1977 period, during which the Kasekela find the neighbors.’’ Deep incursions lasted
community killed the males of the neighbor- up to 6 hours, and on 5 of 129 territorial
ing Kahama community, Kasekela border interactions in Taı̈, led to attacks. All incur-
patrols were disproportionately directed to- sions were led by males.
wards the Kahama territory (Goodall, 1986).
The recorded frequency of border patrols by Coalitionary attacks. These are interac-
Kasekela males was highest in 1972–1973, tions in which observers assess that the
when 13 border patrols occurred in 58 days intent of those in the aggressive party is to
of observation, i.e., a rate of 82 per year hurt or to kill one or more victims. They can
8 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

occur within the territory of either the ag- an adolescent male and nulliparous female
gressors or the victims, or in the boundary near the territorial border, but later re-
area. In Gombe, coalitionary attacks have treated when confronted by four adult males
included both interactions between the well- from the neighboring community, who chased
habituated Kasekela and Kahama communi- the aggressors for 700 m (M. Muller, per-
ties, as well as other communities. In late sonal communication).
1974, for example, a party of three Kahama Coalitionary attacks also occur within com-
males encountered a male and female found munities. For example, a bullying and insub-
to the south of their territory (the ‘‘Kalande’’ ordinate young adult male (Jilba) in Ma-
community). Two of the Kahama males hale’s M-group was attacked so severely by
grabbed and attacked the Kalande male, but six males and two females that it took him 3
he escaped without serious injury (Goodall, months of traveling alone before he recov-
1986). ered sufficiently to rejoin the community
In Mahale, cases included both attacks by (Nishida, 1994).
M-group toward K-group males, and vice
versa. For example, Nishida (1979) recorded Coalitionary kills. No cases have been
three K-group males chasing an M-group reported of dyadic violence leading to the
male for 200 m. Two of the pursuers gave up, death of an adult chimpanzee. However,
but the third caught the victim, forced him lethal coalitionary attacks on adults have
to the ground, bit his thigh, stamped on him, been reported from all four study sites of the
chased him as he tried to escape, but then eastern subspecies (Table 4). Table 4 lists
suddenly gave up. known and inferred cases. The largest sam-
In Taı̈, Boesch and Boesch (1999) reported ple comes from Gombe and includes five
a category of attack which they called ‘‘com- observed brutal attacks followed by the dis-
mando,’’ in which a party of males (with or appearance of the victim (four male, one
without females) penetrated into the neigh- female), and one case of a fresh corpse of an
boring range and attacked one or more unidentified female, considered to have been
neighbors, i.e., a combination of a ‘‘deep killed by the Kahama males. Goodall (1986)
incursion’’ with a coalitionary attack. The summarizes the observations as follows: the
Taı̈ males sometimes ‘‘waited and listened attacks lasted at least 10 min each; the
silently for hours before they attacked. We victim was always held to the ground by one
twice saw the study community being victim or more of the assailants while others at-
of a commando attack, in one of which tacked; the victim was dragged in at least
Macho (an adult male) escaped with 19 two directions, eventually gave up resisting,
wounds’’ (Boesch and Boesch, 1999). A sec- and was essentially immobilized by the end
ond form of coalitionary attack was the of the attack.
‘‘lateral attack’’ (seen six times), in which a In Mahale, Nishida et al. (1985) recorded
party moved laterally while looking in the the deaths of all six adult males of K-group
direction of strangers, then approached, community between 1969 and 1980. In Nishi-
chased, and on at least one occasion caught da’s words, the observers ‘‘speculate that at
and attacked one of the opponents. This least some adult males, particularly So-
appeared to be a tactic for increasing the bongo and Kamemanfu, were killed by M-
imbalance of power by isolating a victim group’s chimpanzees. Severe fighting was
from the rest of the party (Boesch and occasionally witnessed between males of K-
Boesch, 1999). group and M-group in (their area of over-
In Kibale, no complete coalitionary at- lap). . . . M-group’s males were sometimes
tacks have been seen, but twice parties of seen to penetrate into the core area of K-
neighbors have silently charged toward iso- group’s range from 1974 onwards’’ (Nishida
lated males of the Kanyawara study commu- et al., 1985, p 288). The males who disap-
nity, then veered off on seeing observers, peared were all healthy, not senile. In one
suggesting that coalitionary attacks were case, M-group males were known to be very
averted by the presence of humans. In a near to K-group males; there were many
third case, five Kanyawara males attacked outbursts of calls; and the next day another
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 9
TABLE 4. All reported intraspecific kills of adult chimpanzees1
Aggressor’s Victim’s
Result Site Date Community Party Community Party Victim’s ID Ref.
Death Gombe 1974 Kasekela 7M, 1F Kahama 1M Godi Goodall (1986, p.
50)
Death Gombe 1974 Kasekela 3M, 1F Kahama 3M, 1F Dé Goodall (1986, p.
50)
Death Gombe 1975 Kasekela 5M Kahama 1M Goliath Goodall (1986, p.
50)
Death Gombe 1977 Kasekela 6M Kahama 1M Sniff Goodall (1986, p.
51)
Death Gombe 1975 Kasekela 4M Kahama 1F Madam Bee Goodall (1986, p.
51)
Death Mahale 1995 M-gp gang M-gp 1M Ntologi Nishida (1996)
Death Budongo 1998 Sonso gang Sonso 1M Zesta (⫹2 K. Fawcett (per-
injured) sonal commu-
nication)
Death Kibale 1992 Rurama gang Kanyawara ⱕ1M Ruwenzori KCP
Death Kibale 1998 Kanyawara gang Sebitole ⱖ1M Unknown KCP
Death Gombe 1972 Kahama ? Kalande? ⱖ1F 1F Wrangham
(1975)
Death Gombe 1977 Kasekela 5M Kahama ⱖ1M Charlie Goodall (1986, p.
50)
Death? Mahale 1996 M-gp ? M-gp 1M Jilba M. Huffman
(personal com-
munication),
Hofer et al.
(1998)
Death? Mahale 1970-83 M-gp ? K-gp ? Some males? Nishida et al.
(1985)
Death? Gombe 1981 Kalande ? Kasekela ? Humphrey Goodall (1986, p.
killed? 51)
Attack Gombe 1974 Kahama 3M Kalande 1M, 1F M attacked, Goodall (1986, p.
caught by 2, 49)
escaped
Attack Gombe 1980 Kalande ? Kasekela 1F Passion Goodall (1986, p.
(inferred) 51)
Attack Mahale 1974 K-group 3M M-group 1M 1 on 1 fight Goodall (1986, p.
51)
1 Parties for aggressors and victims show the number of adult males (M) and females (F). Letters in bold show the victim’s sex. KCP
(citation for Kibale deaths) is records of the Kibale Chimpanzee Project.

K-group male was missing (Kasonta) (T had been removed. Nine Kanyawara males
Nishida, personal communication). had been patrolling the border on the previ-
In Kibale, after 3 days during which males ous evening, all of whom were present the
from the Kanyawara and Rurama communi- next morning (17 hours later), and several of
ties had been counter-calling at each other whom beat on the victim’s body and dragged
in a hostile manner, a Kanyawara male it about (M. Muller, personal communica-
(Ruwenzori) was found freshly dead in the tion).
border area. His body, huddled face down at Like coalitionary attacks, coalitionary kills
the bottom of a slope around which the also occur within communities. At Budongo
vegetation had been beaten down, showed in 1998, an adult male was killed by other
clear evidence of a violent attack by chimpan- males of his own community (K. Fawcett,
zees. In an unrelated incident, the personal communication). Intracommunity
Kanyawara males were followed by observ- killing is also thought to have occurred in
ers to the fresh corpse of an individual from Mahale, where adult male Ntologi was found
a neighboring community (Sebitole) who had dead in the center of M-group’s territory
apparently been killed by chimpanzees the with numerous wounds on his body. His
previous evening. There were numerous death followed several coalitionary attacks
wounds on the front of his body, his trachea on him by his former subordinates—after
had been ripped through, and both testicles his defeat as alpha-male of M-group
10 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

TABLE 5. Chimpanzee coalitionary kills of adults1


Kill seen,
Aggressor Victim’s or fresh Suspicious
Site community community corpse disappearance Reference
Gombe Kasekela Kahama 5 2 Goodall (1986)
Gombe Kahama Kalande 1 Goodall (1986)
Gombe Kalande Kahama 1 Goodall (1986)
Kibale Rurama Kanyawara 1 KCP
Kibale Kanyawara Sebitole 1 KCP
Mahale M-group K-group 6? Nishida et al. (1985)
Mahale M-group M-group 1 1 T. Nishida (personal communication)
Budongo Sonso Sonso 1 K. Fawcett (personal communication)
Total 10 10?
1Intra-community aggression is indicated by the victim and aggressors being in the same community. KCP, records of the Kibale
Chimpanzee Project.

(Nishida, 1996; T. Nishida, personal commu- tion time. Thus, Figure 1 shows that in
nication). relation to observation time, the number of
The chimpanzee data are summarized in observed and suspected kills appears simi-
Table 5. Tables 4 and 5 also list suspicious lar in the four schweinfurthii study sites.
disappearances. These are cases where ob- The idea that as observation years accumu-
servers believed the most likely explanation late, more killing will be seen, is supported
for disappearances was that they were killed by the data on infanticides, which show a
by neighbors, because: (1) those who disap- similar trend (Fig. 1). In summary although
peared were healthy and not senescent; (2) the evidence needs to be substantiated by
other causes of death appeared improbable; continuing observation, current evidence is
and (3) the disappearances occurred at a that in all four populations of the eastern
time and place where there were patently subspecies, adults kill each other occasion-
hostile relationships with a neighboring com- ally through coalitionary violence. Figure 1
munity. suggests a rate of approximately 0.25 adults
Although the reported episodes of lethal killed per year.
coalitionary violence are still few, the kill- On the other hand, there is no evidence of
ings are noteworthy because they have been lethal intraspecific aggression toward either
reported from four sites and, in relation to adults or infants from either of the studies of
total observed adult deaths, they appear to the western subspecies, i.e., from Taı̈ or
be demographically significant. In Gombe, Bossou. Because the Bossou community has
data reported by Goodall (1986) indicate no neighbors and few males, low rates of
that for adult males in Kasekela and Ka- aggression are not surprising. However, as
hama, the proportion of adult male mortal- demonstrated by Figure 1, lethal coalition-
ity from intraspecific coalitionary aggres- ary aggression would be expected to have
sion was 30–40%. Although fewer kills have been seen at Taı̈: the study is now 21 years
been seen elsewhere, it seems likely that old and should therefore have produced evi-
this variation is partly a function of observa- dence of about five killings each of adults

Fig. 1. Chimpanzee intraspecific


killings by study site. S, P. t. schwein-
furthii; v, P. t. verus. See Tables 3 and 4
for data and sources.
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 11

and infants if this population conformed to The known kills at Gombe occurred be-
the schweinfurthii pattern. Although the tween 1973 and 1977, during a period of
sample sizes are small, the fact that Taı̈ intense hostility between two communities
chimpanzees show all components of lethal that had recently split from a single commu-
raiding but no coalitionary kills suggest that nity, and which were each dominated by two
the nature of aggressive relationships dif- alpha-males with mutually hostile relations
fers between Taı̈ and the eastern popula- (Goodall, 1986). Border patrols by Kasekela
tions (below). males were directed mostly toward the Ka-
Finally, border avoidance is expected if hama community during this period, which
individuals are aware that the border is ended with the extinction of the Kahama
physically dangerous. Low frequency of use community (Goodall, 1986). Thus, unusual
of border areas has been documented by demographic and social conditions applied
Boesch and Boesch (1999), who found that in to elicit this particular bout of lethal raiding.
Taı̈ 75% of time was spent in the central 35% Therefore, nonlethal raiding is a routine
of the range. When Gombe or Kibale component of the chimpanzee behavioral
(Kanyawara) chimpanzees do visit border repertoire. Coalitionary killing is less com-
mon. However it has been recorded in four
areas, they tend to do so in parties that are
out of five sites. This raises the question of
relatively large (Gombe) and contain a higher
why chimpanzees have an appetite for ago-
proportion of males than normal
nistic interactions with members of neighbor-
(Kanyawara) (Bauer, 1980; Chapman and
ing communities, and why they sometimes
Wrangham, 1993). Finally, high prey densi-
kill opponents.
ties in border areas have been reported for
the main prey species of chimpanzees, red THE IMBALANCE-OF-POWER
colobus (Colobus badius), both in Gombe HYPOTHESIS
(Stanford, 1998b) and Kibale [Ngogo (D. Explaining chimpanzee violence
Watts, personal communication)]. Similarly
Many reasons have been advanced to ac-
among wolves, prey densities are higher in
count for chimpanzee lethal raiding, includ-
border areas between territories, a result of
ing: male–male bonds, hostility toward out-
avoidance of those areas by wolves (Mech et
siders, cooperative group living, cooperative
al., 1998). Border avoidance by territory
hunting skills, power imbalances when par-
holders has not been reported in other non-
ties from neighboring communities meet,
human species, but is presumably wide- large and overlapping home ranges, high
spread in humans. cognitive ability, and innate killing potential
In summary, there are five study sites (reviewed by van der Dennen, 1995). The
(Gombe, Mahale, Taı̈, Kibale, Budongo) in only attempt at a cost-benefit analysis that
which chimpanzees have neighbors and explains the species distribution of lethal
where intercommunity interactions have raiding, however, is the imbalance-of-power
been at least partly described or observed hypothesis. This hypothesis was implied by
(Table 2). At the three best-documented sites Goodall (1986), then elaborated by Manson
(Gombe, Mahale, Taı̈) patterns of territorial and Wrangham (1991), Wrangham and Pe-
interaction appear similar: all of them show terson (1996), and Wrangham (1999b). The
territorial defense and border patrols by imbalance-of-power hypothesis proposes that
adult males, with violent coalitionary at- the function of unprovoked intercommunity
tacks on neighbors. All these patterns are aggression (i.e., deep incursions and coali-
similar to the data on wolves, which is the tionary attacks) is intercommunity domi-
only other nonhuman mammal with fission- nance. By wounding or killing members of
fusion grouping and group territories in the neighboring community, males from one
which intergroup interactions have been community increase their relative domi-
well described. These points suggest that all nance over the neighbors. According to the
the major elements of lethal raiding are imbalance-of-power hypothesis, the proxi-
routinely present in populations of chimpan- mate benefit is an increased probability of
zees. winning intercommunity dominance con-
12 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

tests (nonlethal battles); this tends to lead to when a party of three or more males encoun-
increased fitness of the killers through im- ters a lone victim (M Wilson et al., personal
proved access to resources such as food, communication), supporting the observa-
females, or safety. tional data from Gombe. These observations
The imbalance-of-power hypothesis con- make sense because to date, there appear to
trasts with proposals that chimpanzees are be no records of any aggressors receiving
exceptionally capable of conducting attacks, serious wounds.
or win particularly large rewards from inter- In light of such data, several authors have
group competition. proposed that it is the ability of a gang of
three or more males to overwhelm a lone
The significance of power imbalances victim, at low risk of injury to themselves,
Both within and between primate groups, which at least partly explains why chimpan-
contests tend to be won by the larger of two zees are so ready to attack (Goodall, 1986;
coalitions, though variables such as domi- Manson and Wrangham, 1991; Wrangham
nance rank and geographic location are also and Peterson, 1996; Boesch and Boesch,
important (Cheney, 1986; Chapais, 1995). 1999). The logic is that a victim can be held
Coalition size appears even more important down or otherwise disabled by two or more,
for interactions among chimpanzees from while another aggressor can impose damage
different communities. In the four longest at will. This idea that the low cost of lethal
studies of chimpanzees, the principal deter- aggression elicits lethal raiding is central to
minant of the nature of intercommunity the imbalance-of-power hypothesis (Manson
interactions is not the geographic location and Wrangham, 1991; Wrangham and Peter-
but the relative size and composition of son, 1996).
parties when they encounter each other.
Origins of power imbalances
This conclusion is based on direct observa-
tions at Gombe, Kibale, Mahale, and Taı̈ All chimpanzee populations have fission-
(Bygott, 1979; Nishida, 1979; Goodall, 1986; fusion grouping patterns, with individuals
Boesch and Boesch, 1999; Wrangham et al., sometimes alone and sometimes in parties
in preparation), as well as playback experi- (Fig. 2), and adult males more gregarious
ments at Kibale (M Wilson et al., personal than mothers (Wrangham, 1999a). Demo-
communication). For example, Boesch and graphic, social and ecological variables influ-
Boesch (1999) found that small parties of ence party size (Boesch, 1996). For example,
males (1–3) mainly checked for the presence party size increases both with the number of
of strangers by drumming and listening to females having sexual swellings, and with
the response (67% of 18 occasions). Middle- the amount of fruit in the habitat (Nishida,
sized parties (4–6 males) tended to make 1979; Goodall, 1986; Boesch, 1996; Wrang-
incursions into the neighboring territory ham, 1999a). Parties appear to be con-
more often (37% of 76 observations). Large strained by fruit availability as a result of
parties (7–9 males) tended to attack the scramble competition, with larger parties
strangers (63% of 30 observations). More formed more when fruit is sufficiently abun-
generally, at all sites, the probability that a dant to allow gregariousness (Chapman et
party will advance, exchange displays, or al., 1995).
retreat appears to be well predicted by Neighboring communities can experience
whether it is larger than, equal to, or smaller markedly different levels of fruit supply, a
than the opposing party (Boesch and Boe- result of differences in fruit-tree density, or
sch, 1999). Relative party size is also a in fruiting success (Chapman et al., 1997).
critical variable among lions (McComb et al., This means that, on occasion, neighboring
1994; Grinnell et al., 1995). communities may contain parties of differ-
The evidence therefore suggests that chim- ent mean size. The community in which
panzee parties are bolder when they contain parties are able to be larger can then make
relatively more males. In addition, playback low-risk raids to attack neighbors. This socio-
experiments at Kibale support the hypoth- ecological connection has been observed in
esis that males are more likely to attack Mahale, when the M-group community
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 13

attacked by a coalition at low risk to the


aggressors. This means that populations (or
seasons) with fewer encounters between soli-
taries and large groups should have fewer
violent interactions. Data on wolves at De-
nali (Alaska) offer a test of this prediction.
The ratio of the number of parties contain-
ing three or more wolves to the number of
solitary individuals was higher during win-
ter (5.2) than summer (0.1). This means that
solitaries were much more likely to encoun-
ter a large party during winter than sum-
mer. As expected, winter was also the season
when intraspecific kills were more likely (a
sevenfold increase in probability, from 22
Fig. 2. Party size distribution among chimpanzees dated kills) (data calculated from Figs 5.4
and bonobos. Data sources: eastern chimpanzee P. t. and 5.7, Mech et al., 1998).
schweinfurthii: Kibale, Kanyawara community 1994– If a similar effect applies to chimpanzee
1996. Western chimpanzee P. t. verus: Taı̈ (Boesch,
1996); Mt. Assirir (Tutin et al., 1983). Bonobo P. panis- populations, and if the fact that Taı̈ has had
cus; Wamba (Kuroda, 1979); Lomako (White, 1988). All a low kill rate is meaningful (rather than
populations show substantial variation in party size
over time and between communities. Comparable data
stochastic), Taı̈ should have larger, less fis-
for Gombe and Mahale were not found. sioned parties than at Gombe, Mahale, or
Kibale. Preliminary data suggest this predic-
tion is qualitatively correct, because Boesch
would make seasonal forays into the terri- (1996) found that among six chimpanzee
tory of the K-group community, supplanting populations, the mean party size was high-
K-group parties and sometimes attacking est at Taı̈ (8.3, compared to a mean of
them (Nishida, 1979). 5.2 ⫾ 0.8 at the other five sites, including
Why are chimpanzees (compared to other Gombe, Mahale, and Kibale). The percent-
species) particularly vulnerable to the pres- age of lone individuals was also lowest in Taı̈
sures of scramble competition that lead to a (4%, compared to 14% ⫾ 3% for the three
fission-fusion, rather than a stable-troop populations with data, Boesch, 1996, Table
system of grouping? Wrangham et al. (1996) 8.2). These data thus indicate consistently
argued that the important characteristic of larger parties in Taı̈ than elsewhere, compat-
chimpanzees is that even when fruits are ible with evidence that Taı̈ is a relatively
scarce, individuals continue to search for productive habitat (Boesch and Boesch,
them. Consistent with this hypothesis, chim- 1999).
panzees (unlike gorillas) are restricted to Further data will test whether party size
areas that contain year round fruits, and and the frequency of high-intensity aggres-
spend significantly greater proportion of sion do indeed co-vary among sites, and how
their feeding time eating ripe fruits than do often chimpanzee populations tend to have
sympatric frugivorous monkeys (Wrangham small parties and high rates of aggression.
et al., 1998). This strategy of constant fruit Two points suggest that the high frequency
search is presumably forced on chimpanzees of intense aggression seen at Gombe and
by species-specific digestive adaptations, suggested by the other eastern chimpanzee
such as the rate of food passage through the populations may be unusual for the species
gut and the ability to ferment long-chain as a whole. First, skeletal trauma indicative
carbohydrates (Milton, 1987). Whatever its of intraspecific aggression has been found at
origins, it exposes them to relatively intense higher rates in a sample of chimpanzee
scramble competition. crania from Gombe than from elsewhere
The imbalance-of-power hypothesis states (Jurmain, 1997). Second, the four P. t. sch-
that violence is facilitated by vulnerability, weinfurthii study sites (Gombe, Mahale,
because lone individuals can be vigorously Kibale, and Budongo) are all located at the
14 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

extreme east of the species geographical occurs in chimpanzees. Others are (1) that
range, where dry seasons are relatively long. the benefits of raiding rise so steeply with
These eastern populations may therefore be increased party size that it pays individuals
living under relatively harsh conditions of to participate for selfish reasons or (2) that
food availability compared to more western chimpanzees have evolved exceptional coop-
sites. erative abilities in contexts other than le-
In summary, chimpanzees are vulnerable thal raiding (C. van Schaik, personal commu-
to particularly intense scramble competi- nication). This remains an important
tion, apparently because of their digestive problem, as it does for much of human
adaptations to ripe fruit. This competition behavior (Boehm, 1999).
forces them to travel alone or in small On the other hand, the occurrence of
parties when fruits are scarce. Patchy fruit territoriality among chimpanzee communi-
distribution can mean that one community ties, and of occasional imbalances of power
has abundant supplies, while its neighbors between parties from neighboring communi-
have few. Demographic differences between ties, are easily explained. First, current
communities (i.e., differences in the number theory suggests that home ranges are eco-
of adult males) may also mean that parties nomically defensible if individuals can eas-
in one community can be dominant over ily cross their home range in a day. Chimpan-
those in the neighboring territory. Such fac- zees can do so (Lowen and Dunbar, 1994; see
tors can account for differences between also van Schaik, 1996). Second, a system of
populations or communities in the number fission-fusion grouping can explain why lone
of males in parties, and hence for differences individuals occasionally encounter larger
in their vulnerability to attack by coalitions coalitions, and are therefore vulnerable to
of neighbors. attack.
But neither long day ranges nor fission-
Group territoriality and the benefits fusion grouping can explain lethal raiding,
of lethal raiding
in which individuals seek opportunities to
Understanding the selective advantage of attack (as opposed to responding to inva-
aggression is more complicated for intercom- sion, escalated contest for resources, etc.).
munity than interindividual relationships, As Goodall noted, for example, there have
because any fitness benefits gained by a rise been three major invasions at Gombe and
in intercommunity dominance are shared Mahale. ‘‘Kasekela males took over Kahama
among individuals within the community. range, Kalande males pushed deep into
This might be expected to favor free-riders Kasakela range, and M-group moved into
(individuals who would benefit from lethal K-group range. During all these invasions
raiding without taking part), which would adult males (and some females) were killed
lead to a suboptimal level of collective action or disappeared. Even if it is argued that the
(van Schaik, 1996; Nunn, 1999). In fact, Kasekela males were merely trying to re-
however, there is no evidence of defection claim an area to which they previously had
among raiding chimpanzees (Goodall, 1986; free access, the assertion does not explain
Wilson et al., personal communication) [(or, the northward thrust of the Kalande commu-
for that matter, among lions in similar inter- nity or the takeover by the M-group at
community contexts (Grinnell et al., 1985)]. Mahale’’ (Goodall, 1986, p. 528).
How lethal raiding escapes the free-rider Two kinds of hypothesis have been pre-
problem is not understood. One possibility is sented to account for such incursions, proxi-
that intercommunity conflict has been so mate competition and dominance drive.
intense that selection has occurred at the First, aggression may be proximately elic-
between-group level (Boehm, 1999). How- ited by resource competition, such as for
ever, this is unlikely because it would re- mates, food, or land (Manson and Wrang-
quire very frequent group extinctions with ham, 1991). This hypothesis is strongly sup-
few survivors. Another is that free-riders are ported by some observations, such as the
policed by others in the community (Boehm, incursions by Mahale’s M-group into the
1999), but no evidence suggests that this K-group range. These occurred in a seasonal
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 15

rhythm coincident with fruit shortages in weakness of a dominant due to aging, wound-
M-group’s range and abundant fruits in ing, loss of allies or loss of confidence in-
K-group’s range (Nishida, 1979). Again, in creases the rate of attack by a subordinate
certain circumstances raiding might help (Bygott, 1979; de Waal, 1982; Goodall, 1986;
males to recruit young females: this possibil- Nishida, 1994). Selection is expected to fa-
ity is suggested by evidence at Gombe that vor the effort to rise in dominance because
severe attacks on the mothers of nulliparous dominant individuals (or groups) tend to
females in neighboring communities are have high fitness, and accordingly, individu-
sometimes followed by the young females als opportunistically take advantage of any
joining the aggressor’s community (Goodall, perception of changes in power asymmetry
1986). (Chapais, 1995).
Alternatively, aggression may be elicited Sometimes, admittedly, the expectation
merely by the opportunity to reduce the that higher dominance leads to higher fit-
coalitionary power of the neighbors (Manson ness is not met. Thus, in around half of the
and Wrangham, 1991; Wrangham and Peter- studies between dominance and reproduc-
son, 1996). According to this ‘‘dominance tive success within primate groups, there
drive’’ hypothesis, no resources need be in was no relationship. However, in the other
short supply at the time of the raid. Instead, half, dominants had higher fitness than
unprovoked aggression is favored by the subordinates (Harcourt, 1987; de Ruiter and
opportunity to attack ‘‘economically,’’ that is, van Hooff, 1993; Ellis, 1995). This means
at low personal risk. If raiding leads to the that even though increased dominance does
wounding or death of a neighboring male, not always lead to higher fitness, it pays on
the neighboring community’s competitive average.
ability is substantially reduced. For ex- Therefore, according to the dominance-
ample, if the neighboring community has 10 drive hypothesis, a necessary and sufficient
males, its fighting power is reduced by 10%. condition for intercommunity aggression is
This reduction lasts for a considerable time, a perception that an opponent is sufficiently
because the system of male philopatry means vulnerable to warrant the aggressor(s) at-
that a dead male can be replaced only via tacking at low risk to themselves.
births within the community, which is a slow The dominance drive hypothesis appears
process. The aggressors’ probability of win- useful for explaining why carnivores share
ning future intercommunity contests lethal coalitionary violence with chimpan-
(battles, not raids) will therefore be signifi- zees. As expected by both the proximate
cantly increased by killing a neighboring competition and dominance drive hypoth-
male. The increase in relative fighting power eses, fission-fusion grouping and intergroup
can be expected to enable a community to hostility occur in these species (Table 6). The
enlarge its territory, as suggested by prelimi- proximate competition hypothesis also pre-
nary evidence of a correlation between the dicts, however, that the type of food supplies,
number of males and territory size at Gombe mating system and/or coalitionary bond
(Stanford, 1998b). Over the long term, there- should be similar in allowing benefits to be
fore, if fitness is correlated with territory gained from raiding or killing neighbors.
size, successful raiding is expected to in- However, the four species of carnivores in
crease the raiders’ fitness. which lethal coalitionary violence has been
This ‘‘between-community dominance recorded show various combinations of mat-
drive’’ hypothesis for explaining aggression ing systems and coalitionary bonds, all differ-
between groups is therefore similar to the ent from those found in chimpanzees (Table
‘‘within-community dominance drive’’ hy- 6). For example, intergroup transfer is in
pothesis, which contributes to explaining some species primarily by males, in others
patterns of aggression between individuals primarily by females. Therefore, lethal vio-
within dominance hierarchies (Popp and lence cannot be uniformly explained as re-
DeVore, 1979; Chapais, 1995). Within chim- sulting from competition over females. Be-
panzee communities, for example, male ag- cause the carnivore species in Table 6 vary
gression occurs predictably over status; in the type of benefits to be gained by
16 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

TABLE 6. Intergroup aggression in fission-fusion species with group territoriality1


Spotted
Chimpanzee Human Wolf Lion hyena Cheetah
Battles Y Y Y Y Y ?
Kill adults Y Y Y Y Y Y
Lethal raid Y Y Y Y? Y? ?
Food supplies Dispersed Variable Clumped Clumped Clumped Dispersed/
clumped
Coalitionary Males Males Pair ⫹ Helpers Females; Males Females Males
bonds among
1 ‘‘Kill adults’’ is shown separately from ‘‘Lethal raid’’ because, in hyenas and lions, it is not clear if killing of neighbors occurs with
lethal raids, or merely when invaders are discovered and killed by residents. ‘‘Dispersed’’ food supplies imply that individual food-
patches are not defensible, whereas ‘‘clumped’’ foods can be individually defended (e.g., carcasses). Coalitionary bonds are bonds in
which adults support each other in aggression against others. Sources for mating system and coalitionary bonds are Kruuk (1972)
(spotted hyenas), Grinnell et al. (1995), and McComb et al. (1994) for lions, and Mech et al. (1998) for wolves. For other sources, see
text.

intergroup dominance, the dominance drive high rates, and there is no evidence of a sex
hypothesis explains similarities in their ten- difference in aggressiveness (Mech et al.,
dency to use lethal violence more easily than 1998). Why, therefore, are males the princi-
the proximate competition hypothesis. pal perpetrators of aggression in chimpan-
The proximate competition and domi- zees and humans?
nance drive hypotheses are closely related, Traditional explanations are that males
because in both cases, the ultimate benefits are more expendable, or that males have
of dominance are increased success in re- more to gain simply because they have higher
source competition. The proximate competi- variance in fitness than females do (re-
tion hypothesis is favored if raiding is elic- viewed by van der Dennen, 1995). However,
ited by the presence of stealable resources, such general explanations do not account for
or if benefits accrue immediately after a species variation in the intensity of female
raid. On the other hand, unprovoked deep participation. Nor does the degree of sexual
incursions and attacks on males without dimorphism in body size, because among
any obvious reward are better explained by nonhuman primates, sexual dimorphism in
the dominance drive hypothesis. To differen- body size is not correlated with female in-
tiate these hypotheses more clearly, data are volvement in intergroup aggression (Man-
needed on the proximate stimuli that elicit son and Wrangham, 1991).
aggression. Since current information sug- Male bonding, which is especially pro-
gests that chimpanzee raids are often initi- nounced among chimpanzees and humans,
ated without the raiders perceiving mates or has often been proposed to be an important
food sources, the dominance drive hypoth- influence (reviewed by van der Dennen,
esis appears relevant to explaining the tim- 1995). This idea is supported by the fact that
ing and direction of lethal raiding. both in humans and nonhuman primates,
populations with more patrilocal residence
Sex differences in territoriality
(or male philopatry) have relatively greater
and aggressiveness
tendency for aggressors to be male (Adams,
Among chimpanzees, males have to date 1983; Manson and Wrangham, 1991).
been the only observed killers and aggres- The ultimate origins of male bonding are
sors in intergroup interactions, and males still debated. In chimpanzees, males are
are also more likely than females to be more gregarious than mothers, possibly be-
victims (Table 4). Among humans, warriors cause, as a result of carrying and waiting for
are also overwhelming male (Adams, 1983). infants, mothers travel slowly (Wrangham,
This contrasts with spotted hyenas, where 1999a). The relative mobility and gregarious-
females are more aggressive than males ness of males means that they can use allies
(Kruuk, 1972; Frank, 1986; East and Hofer, to dominate access to their home ranges,
1991; Henschel and Skinner, 1991); and excluding other males and thereby forcing
with wolves, where both sexes are killed at male philopatry. As a result, a system evolves
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 17

in which it pays to eradicate males from the community range borders. Among bono-
neighboring communities (Wrangham, bos, by contrast, interactions are seen at
1999a,b). range borders without the components of
According to this logic, therefore, male lethal raiding.
bonding has two effects. First, it contributes Second, relatively peaceful intercommu-
to the development of male philopatry and nity relationships in bonobos may be an
the benefits of excluding nongroup males, incidental result of a reduction in the level of
thereby raising the stakes in territorial en- within-community violence compared to
counters. Second, it makes available allies chimpanzees. For example, fewer violent
that enable a larger party to dominate a behavioral interactions of all kinds occur
smaller party. among bonobos: no sexual coercion, no infan-
None of this means, however, that male ticide, no brutal fights among males or fe-
participation is a necessary condition for the males competing for dominance, no male
evolution of lethal violence in territorial beatings of females (Wrangham and Peter-
interactions. As wolves, spotted hyenas and son, 1996; Furuichi et al., 1998). The same is
ants show, coalitionary territoriality can be true in captivity (de Waal and Lanting,
carried out by both sexes, or even primarily 1997; Stanford, 1998a). Collateral evidence
by females. The comparative evidence, there- comes from a survey of cranial and postcra-
fore, suggests that lethal raiding in chimpan- nial skeletal trauma that concluded that in
zees and humans cannot be attributed to the chimpanzees (and gorillas), but not in bono-
fact that bonds among adults are primarily bos, there was evidence of serious risk from
among males. interindividual aggression (Jurmain, 1997)
and from the generally less robust and less
Bonobos: exceptions sexually dimorphic morphology of bonobos
that support the rule? than chimpanzees (Zihlman and Cramer,
Intercommunity relations among bonobos 1978; Shea, 1984). Pending further data,
sometimes involve fights between large par- bonobo males consistently appear to be less
ties, but as a species they appear to be violent than chimpanzees.
substantially less hostile to each other than Accordingly, a possible hypothesis is that
are chimpanzees (reviewed by Wrangham selection may have favored a generally less
and Peterson, 1996; Stanford, 1998a). First, aggressive male temperament in bonobos,
bonobos have never been seen to engage in as opposed to a loss of motivation specifically
lethal raiding, nor indeed in any compo- for lethal raiding. The reasons why male
nents of such behavior (Table 2). Second, bonobos are generally less aggressive than
they can include markedly peaceful interac- male chimpanzees could derive from the
tions, in which individuals from neighboring dominance of males by powerful female–
communities rest, travel, copulate, play, and female coalitions, or the greater importance
groom together (Idani, 1991; White, 1996). of mothers than other males as allies for
In contrast, peaceful interactions involving individual males, or other social dynamics
males of neighboring communities have not occurring within communities (Kano, 1992;
been seen among chimpanzees. Parish, 1996; Wrangham and Peterson, 1996;
In view of the anatomical and phyloge- de Waal and Lanting, 1997). The important
netic similarities between chimpanzees and point is that the reduced tendency for lethal
bonobos these differences are remarkable. raiding would be viewed as an incidental
Three kinds of explanation suggest them- consequence of a more general reduction in
selves. male aggression. This hypothesis is chal-
First, the facts may be misleading; more lenged, however, by species that have aggres-
prolonged observation may reveal lethal sive intergroup interactions despite having
raiding in bonobos (Stanford, 1998a). How- peaceful relationships within groups (e.g.,
ever, this eventuality appears unlikely be- female lions). Furthermore, the tendency to
cause at Gombe, Mahale, Taı̈, and Kibale engage in lethal raiding seems unlikely to be
components of lethal raiding were seen selectively neutral, considering its poten-
shortly after individuals were observed near tially large effects on dominance relation-
18 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

ships between groups and the time and however, remains to be proven. In Lomako,
effort spent on raids. I therefore conclude male bonobos spend increased time alone
that the low tendency for lethal raiding in during periods of fruit scarcity (White, 1998).
bonobos is not merely an incidental conse- White (1998) suggests that these males
quence of the benefits of within-community choose to travel alone in order to track the
peacefulness. increasingly dispersed female parties, but
The third kind of explanation is that according to the imbalance-of-power hypoth-
among bonobos, important components of esis, it should be dangerous for them to do
lethal raiding has been specifically selected so. However, if solitary travel is a social
against. Under what circumstances could option rather than a strategy dictated by
this occur? According to the imbalance-of- ecological pressures, it may be possible for
power hypothesis, lethal raiding is favored bonobos to restrict their solitary periods to
by a combination of coalitionary territorial- times and locations when they can assess
ity and imbalances of power sufficient to that they are safe. More quantitative data
allow one party to kill victims of the rival will be needed to test such ideas.
community with impunity. Since lethal raid- One of the only other species of primate in
ing is absent even though coalitionary terri- which lethal raiding might be expected from
toriality occurs among bonobos, the imbal- the imbalance-of-power hypothesis are spi-
ance-of-power hypothesis predicts that der monkeys Ateles spp., because spider
compared to chimpanzees, bonobos must monkeys have a fission-fusion grouping sys-
experience greatly reduced power imbal- tem like chimpanzees: individuals some-
ances between rival parties. times travel alone and sometimes in larger
In general, variance in bonobo party size parties. Furthermore, males are more gre-
is less than among chimpanzees, even when garious than mothers, and they form coali-
average party size is similar (Chapman et tionary bonds with each other against neigh-
al., 1994). This is as expected from the boring groups (Chapman et al., 1995). On
imbalance-of-power hypothesis. However, the other hand, spider monkeys are wholly
the more critical question is how often bono- arboreal, which may reduce their ability to
bos are forced to travel alone, because indi- use coalitionary aggression. Among baboons
viduals can be killed (at minimal cost to the Papio anubis, for example, coalitions of low-
aggressors) only when they are found alone ranking males are effective in defeating a
by a rival party. The two principal bonobo single higher-ranking male on the ground,
study sites both indicate that lone travel is but not in trees (Smuts, 1986). Further data
rarely forced by ecological pressures. In on the effect of arboreality on power asymme-
Wamba, bonobos usually range as one or two tries between coalitions and solitaries is
large mixed parties averaging more than ten therefore desirable.
individuals (Hashimoto et al., 1998). In Lo-
The imbalance-of-power hypothesis
mako, where feeding competition appears
and the evolution of human warfare
more intense than in Wamba, females re-
main in multi-female parties even during Peace is the normal human condition, in
the seasons when fruit is least available the sense that most human groups, for most
(White, 1998). Such observations suggest of the time, are not at war (Ferguson, 1989;
that compared to chimpanzees, the intensity Sponsel, 1996). Nevertheless, ethnographic
of feeding competition is substantially re- and historical records clearly show that war-
duced among bonobos (Chapman et al., 1994; fare is a frequent practice (Keeley, 1996;
Wrangham et al., 1996; White, 1998). As a Manson and Wrangham, 1991; van der Den-
result, extreme imbalances of power appear nen, 1995). Increasingly, archaeological data
unlikely to occur between parties meeting suggest that violence has often been a statis-
from neighboring communities. tically important source of death, and it is
Whether the apparent difference in the sometimes possible to infer that the violence
intensity of feeding competition between was coalitionary (Keeley, 1996; Larsen,
chimpanzees and bonobos is sufficient to 1997). In small-scale societies, the common-
satisfy the imbalance-of-power hypothesis, est form of war interaction is a raid (e.g.,
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 19

Turney-High, 1949; Keeley, 1996; Maschner ers’ ability both to defend a range and to
and Reedy-Maschner, 1998). Even if hu- form alliances.
mans are routinely peaceful, therefore, war Both intergroup hostility and a fission-
needs to be explained. Because warfare in- fusion grouping system are universal in
cludes a variety of types of interaction (such contemporary human populations, whether
as raids and battles), it will require multiple tribal or nation-state (Rodseth et al., 1991).
explanations. Whether these features were characteristic
The myriad hypotheses proposed to ex- of humans in prehistory is unknown. How-
plain why humans practice raids and other ever, with the exception of brain size, human
forms of warfare fall into three general morphology has changed relatively little dur-
classes. Maladaptive hypotheses suggest ing the last 1.9 million years (Wolpoff, 1998),
that warfare results from an originally adap- suggesting that the essential ecology of hu-
tive aggressive tendency that, as a result of man prehistory may been rather stable prior
subsequent developments such as the inven- to agriculture. The essence of theories about
tion of weapons, became disadvantageous fission-fusion grouping in chimpanzees is
even to the winners (e.g., Lorenz, 1966). that fission is a response to high costs of
They suffer from the theoretical problem scramble competition (Chapman et al., 1995);
that if warring tendency is indeed disadvan- scramble competition is expected to be more
tageous, it has such large effects that it intense in species that depend on rare, high-
should be selected against rapidly. However, quality foods (Janson and Goldsmith, 1995),
it seems unlikely that winners fare badly. and humans appear adapted to high-quality
Neutral hypotheses are currently more foods (Milton, 1987; Leonard and Robertson,
popular, though they also suffer from the 1997). Following this line of argument, fis-
selective-disadvantage problem. They sug- sion-fusion grouping is expected to have
been characteristic of human evolutionary
gest that warfare should be regarded as
history.
deriving merely from a capacity, or poten-
Based on the ubiquity of xenophobia and
tial, resulting from our cognitive creativity.
ingroup-outgroup bias in contemporary
Neutral hypotheses consider warfare to be
populations, intergroup hostility is normally
elicited by environmental and social stimuli
assumed to have been routine in human
that have no evolutionary significance (e.g.,
prehistory. The likelihood of intergroup hos-
Bock, 1980; Keeley, 1996; Gould, 1996). They
tility in prehistory is supported also by its
are often based on the (erroneous) premise prevalence among nonhuman primates (Che-
that behaviors that vary among populations ney, 1986). The form of hostility can be
cannot be explained in terms of natural inferred as being territorial, because among
selection without assuming genetic differ- primates, territories tend to be found in
ences between populations (see Discussion). species with long day ranges in relation to
The imbalance-of-power hypothesis exem- home ranges, and are predicted to occur
plifies a third kind of hypothesis that views more easily where groups are split into
warfare as adaptive and rooted in genetic multiple parties (Mitani and Rodman, 1979;
predispositions. It suggests that raiding de- Lowen and Dunbar, 1994). The long day
rives from the advantages of gaining inter- ranges of contemporary forager men [e.g., 9
group dominance and an ability to assess km (Bailey, 1991)] and the probability of
power imbalances in an environment of inter- fission-fusion foraging suggest that territori-
group hostility and power imbalances be- ality would have been possible where home
tween parties from neighboring communi- ranges were not immense.
ties. As in chimpanzees, it raises the question The imbalance-of-power hypothesis is thus
of why territories are defended by males compatible with conventional views of hu-
rather than females. By analogy with the man prehistory. It can in theory be chal-
argument for chimpanzees, male rather than lenged by evidence that recent prehistoric
female territoriality derives from the high ancestors foraged in stable parties, or had
cost of travel experienced by mothers ways of reducing power imbalances between
(Wrangham, 1999a). This cost reduces moth- rival parties, or had little intergroup hostil-
20 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

ity, though such evidence would in practice the forest has been disturbed by encroach-
be difficult to obtain. ment or logging (Kibale, Ngogo, and
On cladistic grounds, various authors have Kanyawara).
hypothesized that lethal raiding in humans However, the relative lack of observations
and chimpanzees shared a common origin of violence in the early years of chimpanzee
around 5–6 mya, and has been present studies cited by Power (1991) is easily under-
continuously in the subsequent evolution of stood without reference to the effects of
each species (Wrangham, 1987; Ghiglieri, disturbance. Where there was no provision-
1988; Wrangham and Peterson, 1996; Otter- ing, early observations were relatively few
bein, 1997). This hypothesis is currently compared to later years, and they were
untestable. A key issue for human ancestry mostly of poorly habituated individuals, more
is whether australopithecine ancestors of concerned about humans than each other
humans foraged in temporary parties (i.e., [e.g., contrast early observations by Ghi-
with fission-fusion) or in stable groups. An- glieri (1984), Kibale, Ngogo community, Isa-
swers to such questions are needed before birye-Basuta (1989), Kibale, Kanyawara
we can be confident whether lethal raiding community, Reynolds and Reynolds (1965),
in chimpanzees and humans represents a Budongo, and Sugiyama (1973), Budongo]
synapomorphy or a homoplasy. with those based on well-habituated indi-
CHALLENGES TO THE viduals by Watts (1999) (Kibale, Ngogo com-
IMBALANCE-OF-POWER HYPOTHESIS munity), Wrangham et al. (1992) (Kibale,
Kanyawara community), and Newton-Fisher
Uncertainty in the chimpanzee data (1997) (Budongo). With increased observa-
The evidence of lethal raiding in chimpan- tion of habituated individuals, studies of the
zees comes from few cases and a small Kanyawara, Ngogo, and Budongo communi-
number of populations, some of which have ties conform to the essential Gombe-Mahale
experienced significant anthropogenic influ- model of dominance-motivated and strategi-
ences. This has led to doubts about the cally violent males. Power (1991) appears
importance of lethal raiding as a species not to have appreciated the difficulty of
trait among chimpanzees (Power, 1991; Suss- observing dominance behavior and violence
man, 1997). among poorly habituated and little known
For example, Power (1991) accepts that individuals.
the descriptions of chimpanzees in Gombe, Provisioning complicates the issue be-
Mahale, and Kibale as violent and status- cause it confounds increased observability
striving are accurate, but regards the behav- with a concentrated food resource that is
iors as nonadaptive consequences of exces- liable to promote aggression. In the case of
sive ecological stress. A central concern for Gombe, Goodall’s introduction of banana
Power (1991) is why there was a shift in the feeding in 1962 led to the chimpanzees
perception of chimpanzee society from peace- becoming habituated quickly, so individuals
ful to violent, beginning in the 1970s based were watched at close quarters during the
on observations after the first 5 years of first decade. Once the chimpanzees were
Goodall’s study (1960–1965). She argues that habituated, they were observed almost en-
the 1960s view of chimpanzees living in a tirely in the banana-feeding area (ca. 50 ⫻ 50
peaceful society was the ‘‘correct’’ one, and m, approximately 1/5,000th of their terri-
that subsequent observations of violence tory of 12 sq km or more), and not followed
reflect a social environment stressed by vari- toward territorial boundaries. Much aggres-
ous kinds of human-induced disturbance. In sion during intense banana-feeding years of
support, she cites the fact that intense ag- 1965–1969 was clearly directed toward ob-
gression was seen rarely in early chimpan- taining bananas (Wrangham, 1974). During
zee studies, especially at Gombe, Budongo, 1969, banana feeding was reduced, and fewer
and Kibale (Ngogo community). She notes aggressive incidents occurred in the banana-
that in studies where violence has been feeding area (Wrangham, 1974).
reported, either humans have provided food Power (1991) argued that a particularly
for the chimpanzees (Gombe, Mahale), or important feature of the banana-feeding sys-
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 21

tem was that, after 1965, chimpanzees were not easily viewed as ‘‘distinct.’’ The series of
frustrated by their lack of control. She char- attacks that began in January 1974 oc-
acterized the observation period 1960–1965 curred almost a decade after the start of the
as ‘‘naturalistic’’ (implying undisturbed by problematic banana-feeding-system, and
humans) because the bananas were given took place several kilometers from the feed-
freely, i.e., they were never withheld. From ing station. Power (1991) implies that this
1965 onward, in attempts to reduce the long delay can be accommodated by the
banana-induced aggression of the 1962– hypothesis of a permanent behavioral/psy-
1965 era, bananas were made available in chological reorganization, but clearly the
metal boxes equipped with doors that were behavior must also be viewed in the context
controlled in various ways by observers. It of ongoing social tensions (see Goodall, 1986
was this system that Power argued caused for an account of the relationships between
chimpanzees to express their potential for the alpha-males of the Kasekela and Ka-
aggression, because it frustrated them. hama communities that may have helped
In the absence of controlled experiments, precipitate the aggression).
no hypothesis can be rejected. Two points The incidence of aggression in the banana-
relevant to the imbalance-of-power hypoth- feeding-area was closely related to the num-
esis can be made, however. First, even if the ber of bananas that chimpanzees obtained.
frustration-aggression hypothesis is correct Party size increased in the feeding area
in explaining why the Kasekela community compared to the natural habitat, but within
attacked the Kahama community, it does minutes of the chimpanzees leaving the
not explain why coalitional lethal aggres- feeding area the expected party size was
sion was elicited relatively easily in these restored (Wrangham, 1994). These and simi-
chimpanzees, or why it occurs in males, lar results show that there were indeed
whereas it has not been seen in any other short-term influences related to the availabil-
species faced with similarly frustrating con- ity of bananas, but no long-term influences
texts. [For example, baboons obtained ba- have been detected (Wrangham, 1974; Good-
nanas regularly at the banana-feeding-area, all, 1986). The accumulation of data from
though observers tried to prevent them other, nonprovisioned sites continues to chal-
(Wrangham, 1974). There has been no hint lenge the view that chimpanzees are natu-
of any behavior resembling lethal raiding in rally averse to violence.
these baboons, despite intense study in sub-
The claim that biology is irrelevant
sequent years]. Thus, as Power (1991) her-
for human warfare
self says, whether or not feeding frustration
contributed to the social tensions at Gombe, Some critics reject evolutionary explana-
there remains a problem to be explained. tions of warfare out of hand, based on the
Why are male chimpanzees easily prompted misconception that the only behavioral pat-
to adopt intense coalitionary violence as a terns explicable by biology are ‘‘instincts,’’
solution to social problems? i.e., behaviors that are obligatory and/or
Second, the frustration-aggression hypoth- invariable. According to this logic, since
esis has much against it. The idea is that warfare is not ‘‘instinctual,’’ biological adap-
‘‘frustration causes a distinct behavioral tations cannot explain the propensity for
change in the condition of an organism’’ war (Keeley, 1996; Sussman, 1997; Regal,
(Power, 1991, p 3). This implies that under 1998). This error seems remarkable, be-
natural conditions, chimpanzees are not cause behavioral ecologists have long
naturally frustrated, which is clearly not stressed that psychological adaptations are
true. For example, intense aggressive compe- expected to respond in a contingent way to
tition occurs regularly in all study sites over appropriate contexts (e.g., Hrdy, 1990;
prized foods, such as meat (Goodall, 1986; Barkow et al., 1995; Krebs and Davies,
Boesch and Boesch, 1989). On the other 1997). In the words of Otterbein (1997,
hand, whatever behavioral change occurred p 272), ‘‘Man is neither, by nature, peaceful
among the post-1965 Kasekela chimpanzees nor warlike. Some conditions lead to war,
(compared to chimpanzees at other sites) is others do not.’’
22 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

The imbalance-of-power hypothesis is en- are the places to seek answers about popula-
tirely compatible with the observations that tion variations.
many people live wholly peaceful lives, that The assumption that evolutionary analy-
some cultures have periods of peace lasting sis implies genetic determinism seems oddly
for several generations, and that some chim- old-fashioned in an era when we are begin-
panzee populations have no lethal raiding ning to understand the nuances of psycho-
for long periods. Indeed, such variation is to neuroendocrinological adaptations of differ-
be expected. The imbalance-of-power hypoth- ent species and sexes. To Keegan (1993, p 3),
esis conforms well to some theories of peace, war reaches into ‘‘the most secret places of
such as the idea that nonviolence is an the human heart, places where self dissolves
adaptive response by societies to violence by rational purpose, where pride reigns, where
stronger neighbors (Dentan, 1992). Admit- emotion is paramount, where instinct is
tedly, males are expected by this hypothesis king.’’ If psychology can describe those se-
to take advantage of power over neighbors, cret places, it is the task of evolutionary
especially when unfettered by social or cul- anthropology to explain how they arose.
tural constraints. They are also expected to IMPLICATIONS OF THE
probe for weaknesses in perceived oppo- IMBALANCE-OF-POWER HYPOTHESIS
nents, and to be willing to fight in a wide
variety of circumstances where elevated sta- Chimpanzee and human psychology
tus is predictable or perceived opponents Different versions of the CVH pay varied
will be wounded or destroyed at low cost. attention to such factors as cognitive ability,
But the essence of the imbalance-of-power weapons, brain size, male-bonding, territori-
hypothesis, like other behavioral hypoth- ality, sexual dimorphism, and imbalances of
eses for large-brained mammals, is that power (van der Dennen, 1995). They are
expression of the behavior depends on con- united, however, in providing adaptive ra-
text. Whether or not an individual employs tionales for chimpanzee and human vio-
violence is expected to depend on the proxi- lence, and therefore in proposing that lethal
mate stimuli, about which we still know raiding has a substantial evolutionary his-
little. What leads individuals to classify tory, possibly since our split from a common
others as ‘‘opponents’’? How do social and ancestor with chimpanzees.
ideological pressures affect the ease with The implication is that there has been
which men, or women, respond to incite- selection for a male psyche that, in certain
ments to violence? How is ‘‘dominance’’ per- circumstances, seeks opportunities to carry
ceived? How are risks perceived, for in- out low-cost attacks on unsuspecting neigh-
stance, when Ego is embedded within a bors. The psychological mechanisms that
hierarchy of alliances? How do institutional would make such a complex function pos-
war relationships influence individual neu- sible have not been studied, but a partial list
roendocrinology, and vice versa? Such ques- might include: the experience of a victory
tions are critical for understanding who thrill, an enjoyment of the chase, a tendency
becomes violent, and when. for easy dehumanization [or ‘‘dechimpiza-
In the current context, the imbalance-of- tion,’’ (Goodall, 1986), i.e., treating nongroup
power hypothesis suggests that selection members as equivalent to prey], and deindi-
has favored certain emotional predisposi- viduation (subordination of own goals to the
tions in males that cause aggressive behav- group), ready coalition formation, and sophis-
ior to be elicited relatively easily under ticated assessment of power differentials.
certain circumstances. The challenge of de- Sex differences can be expected in at least
fining the eliciting circumstances is the prov- some of these traits, unless developmental
ince of disciplines that probe local varia- constraints interfere. Some features of a
tions, including not only biological lethal-raiding psychology are not easily pre-
anthropology, but also social psychology, be- dicted, such as the mechanisms by which
havioral ecology, social anthropology, cul- expected costs and benefits are assessed.
tural ecology, or social ecology. Those disci- A sharp alternative to the CVH is the
plines, together with biological anthropology, standard social science model (SSSM), that
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 23

human males have no inherent propensity human groups incorporate more levels of
to take advantage of power differentials. social dynamics (Hinde, 1993). At each such
Instead, according to the SSSM, humans level, ‘‘level-specific properties’’ influence and
merely have a capacity for violence, and are influenced by adjacent levels. For ex-
since ‘‘the range of possible cultural results ample, group processes and institutional
is not explicable by natural selection’’ (Bock, influences modify the motivational ideals of
1980; p. 76; cf. Gould, 1996), evolutionary individuals. In modern nation-states, the
history is claimed to be irrelevant. This line military-industrial-scientific complex tends
of thinking has several problems. It does not to precipitate and maintain war, e.g., by
account for the species distribution of coali- producing increasingly sophisticated weap-
tional aggression. It treats biology and cul- onry. War as an institution can in theory be
tures as alternatives, rather than as mutu- maintained by the inertia of subinstitutions,
ally interacting influences. It does not such as the belief that capitalism needs
account for the predictability of human ag- militarism for its continued growth (Hinde,
gressive patterns, and it is easily subsumed 1993). Dynamics like these mean that a
under adaptive theories of violence, which propensity for lethal raiding cannot be trans-
can account both for the fact that individu- lated directly into an explanation of the
als choose to manipulate others (whether complexities of human warfare.
through ideology or other ways) and for the Is a propensity for lethal raiding of the
fact that they are so easily manipulated. chimpanzee type at all relevant to human
warfare? Among people living in small politi-
The complexity of war cally independent groups, lethal raiding ap-
Even in the complex human world, some pears strikingly similar to the patterns
of the processes that regulate aggression among chimpanzees. In both cases, small
among large groups are analogous to those parties of males aim to make undetected
that occur at the individual or face-to-face incursions into the ranges of neighbors, at-
level (Hinde, 1993). Both at the large group tack unsuspecting victims, and retreat with-
and the individual level, for example, per- out being drawn into a battle (Turney-High,
sonal relations between leaders of opposing 1949; Chagnon, 1992; Keeley, 1996). Al-
groups can play an important role, with a though the psychological processes remain
threat to the interests or values of the actor undescribed, the imbalance-of-power hypoth-
being capable of instigating aggression. Pla- esis might suggest that selection has fa-
catory signals or actions (e.g., donations) are vored various complex traits, such as a
used to deter aggression. Aggressiveness tendency to classify others as in-group or
can be augmented by a greater asymmetry out-group, to regard members of out-groups
of power, or reduced by a probability of as potential prey, to be alert to (or search for)
punishment. Hinde (1993) found more than power asymmetries between in-group and
20 such analogies, of varying significance out-group parties, and to be ruthless in
and distinctness, linking the behavioral in- attacking out-group parties when the per-
teractions among individuals and large ceived power asymmetry is sufficiently great.
groups. This suggests that, in some ways, A list of traits such as these can in theory
the logic of aggressive interaction among describe an evolutionarily selected ‘‘propen-
individuals can be applied to large groups. sity for lethal raiding.’’
Nevertheless, so many cultural and lin- Such traits appear obviously relevant to
guistic novelties complicate warfare that the some aspects of intergroup relations (e.g.,
connection to biology can appear tenuous at the planning and execution of military en-
best. Among factors such as the number of gagements). Among humans, the complexity
military and strategic options available, the of society means that individual propensi-
ability to discuss options and manipulate ties sometimes have less direct impact on
others, the adoption of cultural goals, and social outcomes than among chimpanzees.
the unpredictable potential for shifting alli- Nevertheless, the imbalance-of-power hy-
ances, a particularly important trait distin- pothesis may explain why culturally derived
guishing humans from chimpanzees is that information is used in certain ways. For
24 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

example, as a result of cultural beliefs or humans and chimpanzees than in other


social pressure, individuals can either primates. Furthermore, the behaviors shown
broaden or contract their concept of where by chimpanzees toward mammalian prey
an in-group/out-group boundary falls, or of are partly similar to those they show toward
how important it is. Idealogues can per- conspecific victims, including quiet stalking
suade their followers that sufficient power during a hunt, intense arousal during the
asymmetry exists to make attacks on an attack phase (pilo-erection, intimidation dis-
outgroup worthwhile. Culture can thus ma- plays), and ambivalence toward the victim.
nipulate the information an individual uses In contrast, the behaviors shown by special-
to assess whether an attack is desirable. ized carnivores toward their prey are not
The imbalance-of-power hypothesis can like those directed toward conspecifics. For
be reconciled with the power of culture; example, social carnivores do not show signs
therefore, if human males have a tendency of excitement when killing prey, and tend to
to search for, and take advantage of, power use a killing bite (van der Dennen, 1995).
asymmetries sufficient to enable them to Such observations suggested to Eibl-
safely kill rivals, while social pressures Eibesfeldt (1975) and Goodall et al. (1979)
modify the concept of ‘‘rival,’’ ‘‘ally,’’ and that among chimpanzees similar motiva-
‘‘sufficient power asymmetry.’’ It accordingly tional factors may be involved in intraspe-
suggests an explanation for why human cific killing and hunting. Eibl-Eibesfeldt
males become dangerous when they obtain, (1975) specifically proposed that, ‘‘Motiva-
or believe they have, large power advan- tionally, hunting behavior in chimpanzees
tages over others. (Whether, in novel circum- has probably been derived from intraspecific
stances, they use such power adaptively is aggression’’ (translated and quoted by van
an open question.) It also suggests the impor- der Dennen, 1995, p 192). The essential logic
tance of systems that reduce power asymme- is that if hunting had arisen independently,
try, such as intergroup alliances through it should be expected to show more similari-
trade, marriage or treaty. ties to the patterns displayed by social carni-
When large power asymmetries do not vores. Van Hooff (1990) agreed, suggesting
occur, relationships between groups are of- that if selection favored the ability to hunt
ten peaceful, as expected from the imbalance- and kill conspecifics, the psychological
of-power hypothesis (Knauft, 1991; Bueno mechanisms that evolved would be easily
de Mesquita and Lalman, 1992; van der co-opted toward obtaining meat.
Dennen, 1995). Even when there is a bal- Note that these ideas are opposite to the
ance of power, however, lethal battles and killer ape hypothesis. The killer ape hypoth-
wars can occur among humans, in contrast esis suggested that intraspecific violence
to the pattern among chimpanzees (Singer, evolved from hunting, whereas Eibl-Eibes-
1989, Boehm, 1992). Coalitionary aggres- feldt (1975), Goodall (1986), and van Hooff
sion occurring between opponents with bal- (1990) proposed that hunting evolved from
anced power requires other kinds of explana- intraspecific violence. As van der Dennen
tion than the imbalance-of-power hypothesis, (1995) notes, the relationship between intra-
such as the cultural exaggeration of motivat- specific killing and hunting probably now
ing forces or the development of self-decep- involves multiple directions. For example, in
tive assessment strategies (Boehm, 1992; some human populations hunting may pro-
van der Dennen, 1995; Wrangham, 1999b). vide practice for warfare (Otterbein, 1997).
Disentangling these relationships will there-
The relation between lethal raiding
fore not be easy.
and hunting
Nevertheless the idea that violence begat
Both lethal raiding and hunting are car- hunting is useful because it suggests a new
ried out primarily by adult males acting in way to solve a puzzle about bonobos, namely,
coordinated groups: both involve otherwise that bonobos show no evidence of monkey
unusual actions such as searching for large hunting. Thus, no monkey hunting or mon-
prey, stalking, chasing, seizing, wounding, key eating has been recorded at the long-
and killing; both are more elaborated in term bonobo sites (Wamba and Lomako), or
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 25

in the shorter-term studies of Yalosidi, Lake suggests that compared to chimpanzees,


Tumba, or Lilungu, even though these stud- bonobos have a weaker motivation to hunt
ies have provided sufficient data to record, monkeys.
for example, termite-eating in four of the As a third possibility, therefore, Wrang-
sites (Thompson, 1997). Bonobos do eat meat ham and Peterson (1996) noted that the lack
occasionally, however, which they obtain in- of monkey hunting among bonobos might be
dividually by seizing young antelope. The explained as a consequence of their low
lack of monkey hunting by bonobos is strik- interest in intraspecific killing. For ex-
ing given that they not only prey on terres- ample, if bonobos evolved from a chimpanzee-
trial mammals, but also complete with each like ancestor, they began with a tendency for
other to eat meat, and sometimes interact lethal raiding which was lost or inhibited
socially with monkeys, in grooming and when they acquired relatively stable par-
play. On three occasions, they have even ties. Accordingly, the evolution of inhibitions
kidnapped young monkeys during play, but against lethal raiding may have inadver-
not eaten them (reviewed by Wrangham and tently caused monkey hunting to be inhib-
Peterson, 1996). ited also, if the two patterns are indeed
Stanford (1998b) suggested that the rea- motivationally related (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1975;
son why male bonobos hunt rarely is that van Hooff, 1990). This proposal implies that
hunting has a low pay-off, because they tend monkey hunting is motivationally more simi-
to lose meat to females. Against this, low- lar to lethal raiding, and relatively distinct
ranking male chimpanzees often lose meat from the killing of terrestrial ungulates.
to high-ranking males, but still hunt fre- Important similarities between monkey
quently (Goodall, 1986). Furthermore it is hunting and lethal raiding could include the
monkey hunting, rather than meat eating, necessity for coordination and planning and
that appears to be lacking in bonobos, not the ability to assess an adequate power
only among males but also among females. imbalance between predators and prey.
Stanford’s proposal is therefore not sup- In summary, it is admittedly speculative
ported. to propose that monkey hunting has been
Another possible explanation for the lack lost in bonobos as a result of selection against
of observations of monkey hunting by bono- propensities relevant to lethal raiding. How-
bos is stochastic. Hunting traditions might ever, this idea appears to explain the facts
vary among bonobo populations. If so, mon- better than alternative hypotheses. It sug-
key hunting may be observed in the future, gests a correlation between group hunting
in populations that have not yet been stud- and lethal raiding in humans, chimpanzees
ied. However, there is no evidence that any and bonobos that challenges traditional
chimpanzee population fails to hunt mon- thinking, and draws attention to the need
keys, provided monkeys are present. Chim- for further data.
panzees prey on monkeys in at least 12 sites,
Morality
including all the long-term sites [Gombe,
Mahale, Taı̈, Kibale, and Budongo (Goodall, This paper suggests that violent propensi-
1986)] as well as seven lesser-known popula- ties of a particular kind have been positively
tions [Chambura, (B. Fahey, personal com- selected among male chimpanzees and hu-
munication; Kahuzi-Biega, DRC (Basasose mans. Biologically, this is unsurprising. Like-
and Yamagiwa, 1997), Lopé, Gabon (Tutin wise, propensities for particular types of
and Fernandez, 1993), Mt. Assirik, Senegal altruistic and cooperative behavior have
(McGrew et al., 1979), Outamba-Kilimi, Si- probably also evolved through selection, and
erra Leone (Alp, 1993), Sapo, Liberia (Ander- are neither more nor less important biologi-
son et al., 1983), Tongo, DRC (A. Lanjouw, cally than violence.
personal communication). There is, there- But anthropologists’ views on violence tend
fore, a strong contrast between the wide- to be interpreted politically. For example
spread occurrence of monkey hunting in Otterbein (1997) labeled anthropologists as
chimpanzees and its absence in bonobos. ‘‘Hawks’’ or ‘‘Doves’’ according to whether
Contrary to the stochastic hypothesis, this they consider evolutionary biology relevant
26 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

or irrelevant to warfare (Otterbein, 1997). understand about the evolutionary origins


Although Doves (e.g., Power, 1991; Suss- and persistence of intergroup violence, the
man, 1997) suggest that Hawks (e.g., Ghi- better we can predict and avert it.
glieri, 1984; Goodall, 1986; Wrangham and
Peterson, 1996) are culturally biassed in CONCLUSION
thinking that male chimpanzees strive ag- Despite some important unsolved prob-
gressively for status and use violence adap- lems, chimpanzee lethal raiding appears
tively in intergroup interactions, there is no generally well explained by the imbalance-of-
evidence for a positive correlation between power hypothesis, which states that success-
anthropological Hawkishness (in the Otter- ful attacks on rivals are favored because
bein sense) and political beliefs. Indeed, they increase the dominance status of the
some notable ‘‘anthropological Hawks’’ have aggressors. A combination of three points
been prominent in the search for peace. For likewise suggests that selection has favored
example, Hamburg (1991) argued for the unprovoked intergroup violence in human
importance of biological similarities in chim- males: the prevalence of human war raiding,
panzee and human violence, and in the same the similarities of chimpanzee and human
spirit co-chaired a multi-year effort to re- lethal raiding, and the ability of the imbal-
duce the frequency and intensity of interna- ance-of-power hypothesis to explain the
tional violence (Carnegie Commission, 1997). mammalian distribution of lethal violence.
There is no moral high ground to be held by Until an alternative model exists, chimpan-
virtue of being an anthropological Dove. zees and humans are, therefore, best re-
Admittedly, any theory of violence has garded as species in which a dominance
moral implications, because biological analy- drive by male groups has been positively
ses can be misused. But no theory, however selected.
benign or malevolent or whether based on If this conclusion has merit, anthropology
biology, psychology, or culture, is immune to has given inadequate consideration to coali-
co-option by ideologues and propagandists. tionary violence as a force in human evolu-
While German military philosophy was tion. As anthropologists, we have a duty to
backed by Darwinism in the First World acknowledge the horrors of our evolutionary
War, French military philosophy was backed past, partly for the sake of truth, and partly
by Bergson’s theory of creative evolution to consider how such behavior can be avoided
(Tuchman, 1962). On either side, opposing in the future. By combining primatological,
theories of evolution were used to bolster the paleontological and behavioral-ecological ev-
waging of war. Military organizations can be idence, anthropologists can provide espe-
expected to deceive themselves and their cially rich tests of evolutionary hypotheses.
followers using any available materials These will offer a solid base from which
(Wrangham, 1999b). evolutionary anthropology can work with
I see no better course than to follow Dar- other disciplines to understand cultural
win (1871, p 405): ‘‘. . .we are not here con- variation and the proximate stimuli that
cerned with hopes or fears, only with the elicit violence.
truth as far as our reason permits us to
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
discover it.’’ Lethal violence appears strik-
ingly frequent among chimpanzees and hu- I thank Clark Larsen and the American
mans, and appears explicable by relatively Association of Physical Anthropologists for
simple adaptive rules. Current evidence sug- inviting me to speak at the AAPA annual
gests it has been a major selective pressure meeting in Salt Lake City, April 1998, which
for significant periods of chimpanzee and prompted this paper. Irven DeVore, Robert
human evolution. Until lethal violence is Hinde, Bill McGrew, and Karen Strier gener-
shown to be a strange new phenomenon, we ously offered extended comments. For help-
should consider it sufficiently ancient to ful critiques I am grateful also to Chris-
have influenced the temperaments of both topher Boehm, Christophe Boesch, Nancy
species, particularly of males, in ways that DeVore, Martin Muller, Vernon Reynolds,
should be taken seriously. The more we Carel van Schaik, and Michael Wilson. Chris-
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 27

tophe Boesch, Katie Fawcett, Martin Muller, Boesch C. 1996. Social grouping in Taı̈ chimpanzees. In:
McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T, editors. Great
Toshisada Nishida, Vernon Reynolds, and ape societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Michael Wilson kindly gave access to unpub- p 101–113.
lished material, Michael Huffman aided with Boesch C, Boesch H. 1989. Hunting behavior of wild
chimpanzees in the Taı̈ National Park. Am J Phys
references, and Johan van der Dennen im- Anthropol 78:547–573.
proved the article by providing his impor- Boesch C, Boesch H. 1999. The chimpanzees of the Taı̈
tant book. Forest: Behavioral ecology and evolution. New York:
Oxford University Press, in press.
Brain CK. 1981. The hunters or the hunted? An introduc-
LITERATURE CITED tion to African cave taphonomy. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press, p 1–365.
Adams DB. 1983. Why there are so few women warriors. Bueno de Mesquita B. 1981. The war trap. New Haven,
Behav Sci Res 18:196–212. CT: Yale University Press. p 1–223.
Adams ES. 1990. Boundary disputes in the territorial Bueno de Mesquita B. 1985. The war trap revisited. A
ant Azteca trigona: effects of asymmetries in colony revised expected utility model. Am Polit Sci Rev
size. Anim Behav 39:321–328. 79:156–177.
Alexander RD. 1987. The biology of moral systems. Bueno de Mesquita B, Lalman D. 1992. War and reason:
Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. p. 1–301. Domestic and international imperatives. New Haven,
Alexander RD. 1989. Evolution of the human psyche. In: CT: Yale University Press. p 1–322.
Mellars P, Stringer C, editors. The human revolution: Byers J. 1997. American pronghorn: Social adaptations
Behavioral and biological perspectives on the origins and the ghosts of predators past. Chicago: University
of modern humans. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univer- of Chicago Press. p 1–300.
sity Press. p 455–513. Bygott JD. 1979. Agonistic behavior, dominance, and
Alp R. 1993. Meat-eating and ant-dipping by wild chim- social structure in wild chimpanzees of the Gombe
panzees in Sierra Leone. Primates 34:463–468. National Park. In: Hamburg DA, McCown ER, edi-
Anderson JR, Williamson EA, Carter J. 1983. Chimpan- tors. The great apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/
zees of Sapo Forest, Liberia: density, nests, tools, and Cummings. p 405–428.
meat-eating. Primates 24:594–601.
Carnegie Commission on Preventing Deadly Conflict.
Arcadi AC, Wrangham RW. 1999. Infanticide in chimpan-
1997. Preventing deadly conflict: Final report. Wash-
zees: review of cases and a new within-group observa-
ington DC: Carnegie Commission on Preventing
tion from the Kanyawara study group in Kibale
Deadly Conflict. p 1–257.
National Park. Primates 40:337–351.
Caro TM, Collins DA. 1986. Male cheetahs of the
Archer J, and Huntingford F. 1994. Game theory models
and escalation of animal fights. In: Potegal M, Knut- Serengeti. Nat Geogr Res 2:75–86.
son JF, editors. The dynamics of aggression. Hillsdale, Cartmill M. 1993. A view to a death in the morning:
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. p 3–31. Hunting and nature through history. Cambridge, MA:
Ardrey R. 1961. African genesis: A personal investiga- Harvard University Press. p 1–331.
tion into the animal origins and nature of man. New Chagnon NA. 1992. Yanomamö: The last days of Eden.
York: Atheneum. p 1–330. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. p 1–309.
Ardrey R. 1966. The territorial imperative. New York: Chapais B. 1995. Alliances as a means of competition in
Atheneum. p 1–416. primates: evolutionary, developmental and cognitive
Ashley Montagu MF. 1968. Man and aggression. Lon- aspects. Yearbk Phys Anthropol 38:115–136.
don: Oxford University Press. p 1–178. Chapman CA, Wrangham RW. 1993. Range use of the
Austad S. 1983. A game theoretical interpretation of forest chimpanzees of Kibale: implications for the
male combat in the bowl and doily spider (Frontinella understanding of chimpanzee social organization. Am
pyramitela). Anim Behav 31:59–73. J Primatol 31:263–273.
Bailey RC. 1991. The behavioral ecology of Efé pygmy Chapman CA, White FJ, Wrangham RW. 1994. Party
men in the Ituri Forest, Zaı̈re. Ann Arbor, MI: Univer- size in chimpanzees and bonobos: A reevaluation of
sity of Michigan Press. p 1–143. theory based on two similarly forested sites. In:
Barkow J, Cosmides L, Tooby J. 1995. The adapted Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, editors.
mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of Chimpanzee cultures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
culture. New York: Oxford University Press. p 1–668. versity Press. p 41–58.
Basasose N, Yamagiwa J. 1997. Predation on mammals Chapman CA, Wrangham RW, Chapman LJ. 1995.
by chimpanzees in the montane forest of Kahuzi, Ecological constraints on group size: an analysis of
Zaire. Primates 38:45–55. spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behav
Bauer HR. 1980. Chimpanzee society and social domi- Ecol Sociobiol 32:199–209.
nance in evolutionary perspective. In: Omark DR, Chapman CA, Chapman LJ, Wrangham R, Isabirye-
Strayer FF, Freedman D, editors. Dominance rela- Basuta G, Ben-David K. 1997. Spatial and temporal
tions: Ethological perspectives of human conflict. New variability in the structure of a tropical forest. Afr J
York: Garland. p 97–119. Ecol 35:287–302.
Boehm C. 1992. Segmentary ‘‘warfare’’ and the manage- Cheney DL. 1986. Interactions and relationships be-
ment of conflict: comparison of East African chimpan- tween groups. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth
zees and patrilineal-patrilocal humans. In: Harcourt RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT, editors. Primate
AH, de Waal FBM, editors. Coalitions and alliances in societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p
humans and other animals. Oxford: Oxford Univer- 227–239.
sity Press. p 137–173. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Andelman SJ, Lee PC. 1988.
Boehm C. 1999. The natural selection of altruistic traits. Reproductive success in vervet monkeys. In: Clutton-
Human Nature: 10. Brock TH, editor. Reproductive success: Studies of
Bock K. 1980. Human nature and history: A response to individual variation in contrasting breeding systems.
sociobiology. New York: Columbia University Press. p Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p 384–402.
1–241. Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE, Albon SD. 1982. Red
28 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

deer: Behavior and ecology of two sexes. Chicago: Hamburg DA. 1991. An evolutionary perspective on
University of Chicago Press. p 1–378. human aggression. In: Bateson P, editor. The develop-
Daly M, Wilson M. 1988. Homicide. Hawthorne, NY: ment and integration of behavior: Essays in honor of
Aldine de Gruyter. p 1–328. Robert Hinde. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Dart RA. 1953. The predatory transition from ape to Press. p 419–458.
man. Intern Anthropol Ling Rev 1:201–218. Harcourt AH. 1987. Dominance and fertility among
Dart RA, Craig D. 1959. Adventures with the missing female primates. J Zool Lond 213:471–487.
link. New York: Harper. p 1–255. Hashimoto C, Tashiro Y, Kimura D, Enomoto T, Ingman-
Darwin C. 1871. The descent of man and selection in son EJ, Idani G, Furuichi T. 1998. Habitat use and
relation to sex. London: John Murray. p 1–475. ranging of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba. Int
Dentan RK. 1992. The rise, maintenance, and destruc- J Primatol 19:1045–1060.
tion of peaceable polity: a preliminary essay in politi- Hausfater G, and Hrdy SB, editors. 1984. Infanticide:
cal ecology. In: Silverberg J, Gray JP, editors. Aggres- Comparative and evolutionary perspectives. Haw-
sion and peacefulness in humans and other primates. thorn, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. p 1–596.
Oxford: Oxford University Press. p 214–270. Henschel JR, Skinner JD. 1991. Territorial behavior by
de Waal FBM. 1982. Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex a clan of spotted hyenas Crocuta crocuta. Ethology
among apes. New York: Harper and Row. p 1–223. 88:223–235.
de Waal FBM, and Lanting F. 1997. Bonobo: The forgot- Hinde RA. 1993. Aggression and war: individuals, groups,
ten ape. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. and states. In: Tetlock PE, Husbands JL, Jervis R,
p 1–210. editors. Behavior, society, and international conflict.
de Ruiter JR, and van Hooff JARAM. 1993. Male domi- Oxford: Oxford University Press. p 8–70.
nance rank and reproductive success in primate Hofer A, Huffman MA, Zeisler G. 1998. Mahale: Begeg-
groups. Primates 34:513–524. nung mit Schimpansen. Verlag Navalon. p 1–159.
East ML, Hofer H. 1991. Loud-calling in a female Hölldobler B. 1981. Foraging and spatiotemporal territo-
dominated mammalian society. II. Behavioral con- ries in the honey ant Myrmecocystus mimicus Wheeler
texts and functions of whooping of spotted hyenas, (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol
Crocuta crocuta. Anim Behav 42:651–669. 9:301–314.
Ehrenreich B. 1997. Blood rites: Origins and history of Hrdy SB. 1990. Sex bias in nature and in history: a late
the passions of war. New York: Metropolitan. p 1–292. 1980s reexamination of the ‘‘biological origins’’ argu-
Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. 1975. Krieg und Frieden aus der Sicht
ment. Yearbk Phys Anthropol 33:25–37.
der Verhaltensforschung. Munich: Piper Verlag. p
Idani G. 1991. Cases of inter-unit group encounters in
1–315.
pygmy chimpanzees at Wamba, Zaı̈re. In: Ehara A, et
Ellis L. 1995. Dominance and reproductive success
al., editors. Primatology Today: Proceedings of the
among nonhuman animals: A cross-species compari-
XIIIth Congress of the International Primatological
son. Ethol Sociobiol 16:257–333.
Enquist M, Leimar O. The evolution of fatal fighting. Society. Amsterdam: Elsevier. p 235–238.
Anim Behav 39:1–9. Isabirye-Basuta G. 1989. The ecology and conservation
Ferguson RB. 1989. Anthropology and war: theory, status of the chimpanzee Pan troglodytes schwein-
politics, ethics. In: Turner PR, Pitt D, editors. The furthii in Kibale Forest, Uganda. Ph.D. dissertation,
anthropology of war and peace: Perspectives on the Makerere University, Kampala, Uganda. p 1–280.
nuclear age. London: Greenwood Press. p 141–159. Janson CH, Goldsmith ML. 1995. Predicting group size
Ferrill A. 1985. The origins of war from the stone age to in primates: foraging costs and predation risks. Behav
Alexander the Great. London: Thames and Hudson. p Ecol 6:326–336.
1–240. Jurmain R. 1997. Skeletal evidence of trauma in African
Frank LG. 1986. Social organization of the spotted apes, with special reference to the Gombe chimpan-
hyaena (Crocuta crocuta). I. Demography. Anim Be- zees. Primates 38:1–14.
hav 35:1500–1509. Kano T. 1992. The last ape: Pygmy chimpanzee behavior
Furuichi T, Idani G, Ihobe H, Kuroda S, Kitamura K, and ecology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Mori A, Enomoto T, Okayasu N, Hashimoto C, Kano T. p 1–248.
1998. Population dynamics of wild bonobos (Pan Keegan J. 1993. A history of warfare. New York: Alfred
paniscus) at Wamba. Int J Primatol 19:1029–1043. A. Knopf. p 1–432.
Ghiglieri MP. 1984. The chimpanzees of Kibale Forest: A Keeley LH. 1996. War before civilization. New York:
field study of ecology and social structure. New York: Oxford University Press. p 1–245.
Columbia University Press. p 1–226. Knauft B. 1991. Violence and sociality in human evolu-
Ghiglieri MP. 1988. Sociobiology of the great apes and tion. Curr Anthropol 32:391–428.
the hominid ancestor. J Hum Evol 16:319–358. Krebs JR and Davies NB. 1997. Behavioral ecology: an
Goodall J. 1986. The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of evolutionary approach. Oxford: Blackwell. p 1–456.
behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kruuk H. 1972. The spotted hyena: A study of predation
p 1–673. and social behavior. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Goodall J, Bandora A, Bergmann E, Busse C, Matama p 1–335.
H, Mpongo E, Pierce A, Riss D. 1979. Intercommunity Kuroda S. 1979. Grouping of the pygmy chimpanzees.
interactions in the chimpanzee population of the Primates 20:161–183.
Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg DA, McCown ER, Larsen CS. 1997. Bioarchaeology: Interpreting behavior
editors. The great apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/ from the human skeleton. Cambridge: Cambridge
Cummings. p 13–54. University Press. p 1–461.
Gould SJ. 1996. The Diet of Worms and the defenestra- Leach E. 1968. Don’t say ‘‘Boo’’ to a goose. In: Ashley
tion of Prague. Natur Hist, Sept: 18–67. Montagu MF, editor. Man and aggression. London:
Grinnell J, Packer C, Pusey AE. 1995. Cooperation in Oxford University Press. p 65–73.
male lions: kinship, reciprocity or mutualism? Anim Leonard WR, Robertson ML. 1997. Comparative pri-
Behav 49:95–105. mate energetics and hominid evolution. Am J Phys
Grossman D. 1996. On killing: The psychological cost of Anthropol 102:265–281.
learning to kill in war and society. Boston, MA: Little, Lorenz K. 1966. On aggression. Translated by Marjorie
Brown. p 1–367. Latzke. London: Methuen. p 1–273.
Wrangham] EVOLUTION OF COALITIONARY KILLING 29
Lowen C, Dunbar RIM. 1994. Territory size and defend- lions. In: Clutton-Brock TH, editor. Reproductive suc-
ability in primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 35:347–354. cess: Studies of individual variation in contrasting
Manson JH, Wrangham RW. 1991. Intergroup aggres- breeding systems. Chicago: University of Chicago
sion in chimpanzees and humans. Curr Anthropol Press. p 363–383.
32:369–390. Palombit RA. 1999. Infanticide and the evolution of pair
Maschner HDG, Reedy-Maschner KL. 1998. Raid, re- bonds in nonhuman primates. Evol Anthropol 7:117–
treat, defend (repeat): the archaeology and ethnohis- 129.
tory of warfare on the North Pacific rim. J Anthropol Parish A. 1996. Female relationships in bonobos (Pan
Archaeol 17:19–51. paniscus): evidence for bonding, cooperation, and fe-
McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A. 1994. Roaring and male dominance in a male-philopatric species. Hum
numerical assessment in contests between groups of Nature 7:61–96.
female lions, Panthera leo. Anim Behav 47:379–387. Pilbeam D. 1996. Genetic and morphological records of
McGrew WC, Tutin CEG, Baldwin PJ. 1979. New data the Hominoidea and hominid origins: A synthesis. Mol
on meat-eating by wild chimpanzees. Curr Anthropol Phylogenet Evol 5:155–168.
20:238–239. Popp J, DeVore I. 1979. Aggressive competition and
Mech LD. 1977. Productivity, mortality, and population social dominance theory. In: Hamburg DA, McCown
trends of wolves in north-eastern Minnesota. J Mam- ER, editors. The great apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benja-
mal 58:559–574. min/Cummings. p 317–338.
Mech LD. 1994. Buffer zones of territories of gray wolves Power M. 1991. The Egalitarians—Human and chimpan-
as regions of intraspecific strife. J Mammal 75:199– zee: An anthropological view of social organization.
202. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p 1–290.
Mech LD, Adams LG, Meier TJ, Burch JW, Dale BW. Prost JH. 1985. Chimpanzee behavior and models of
1998. The Wolves of Denali. Minneapolis, MN: Univer- hominization. In: Kondo S, editor. Primate morpho-
sity of Minnesota Press. p 1–225. physiology, locomotor analyses, and human bipedal-
Miller LE. 1998. Fatal attack among wedge-capped ism. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press, p 289–303.
capuchins. Folia Primatol 69:89–92. Putland DA, Goldizen AW. 1998. Territorial behavior in
Milton K. 1987. Primate diets and gut morphology: the Tasmanian native hen: group and individual
implications for hominid evolution. In: Harris M, Ross performance. Anim Behav 56:1455–1463.
EB, editors. Food and evolution: Toward a theory of Regal P. 1998. Violence and sex. Q Rev Biol 73:473–476.
human food habits. Philadelphia: Temple University Reynolds V, Reynolds F. 1965. Chimpanzees of the
Press. p 93–116. Budongo Forest. In: De Vore I, editor. Primate behav-
Mitani JC, Rodman PS. 1979. Territoriality: relation of ior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. p. 368–
ranging patterns and home range size to defendabil- 424.
ity, with an analysis of territoriality among primate Rodseth L, Wrangham RW, Smuts BB, Harrigan A.
species. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5:241–251. 1991. The human community as a primate society.
Morris D. 1977. Manwatching: A field guide to human Curr Anthropol 32:221–254.
behavior. New York: Harry N. Abrams, Inc. p 1–318. Shea BT. 1984. An allometric perspective on the morpho-
Newton-Fisher NE. 1997. Tactical behavior and decision- logical and evolutionary relationships between pygmy
making in wild chimpanzees. Ph.D. dissertation, Uni- (Pan paniscus) and common (Pan troglodytes) chim-
versity of Cambridge. p 1–325. panzees. In: Susman RL, editor. The pygmy chimpan-
Nishida T. 1979. The social structure of chimpanzees of zee. New York: Plenum Press. p 89–130.
the Mahale Mountains. In: Hamburg DA, McCown Singer JD. 1989. The political origins of international
ER, editors. The great apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benja- war: a multifactorial review. In: Groebel J, Hinde RA,
min/Cummings. p 73–121. editors. Aggression and war: Their biological and
Nishida T. 1990. A quarter century of research in the social bases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mahale Mountains: An overview. In: Nishida T, editor. p 202–220.
The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: Sexual Smuts BB. 1986. Sex and friendship in babboons. Haw-
and life history strategies. Tokyo: University of Tokyo thorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. p 1–303.
Press. p 3–36. Sponsel LE. 1996. The natural history of peace: a
Nishida T. 1994. Review of recent findings on Mahale positive view of human nature and its potential. In:
chimpanzees: implications and future research direc- Gregor T, editor. A natural history of peace. Nashville,
tions. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, TN: Vanderbilt University Press. p 95–125.
Heltne PG, editors. Chimpanzee cultures. Cambridge, Stanford C. 1998a. The social behavior of chimpanzees
MA: Harvard University Press. p 373–396. and bonobos: A critical review. Curr Anthropol 39:399–
Nishida T. 1996. The great chimpanzee ruler killed by a 420.
coalition of previous group mates: cruel political dy- Stanford CB. 1998b. Chimpanzee and red colobus: The
namics in wild chimpanzees. Asahi-Shimbun, Janu- ecology of predator and prey. Cambridge, MA: Har-
ary 31, 1996, evening edition. p 1. vard University Press. p 1–296.
Nishida T, Haraiwa-Hasegawa M, Takahata Y. 1985. Starin ED. 1994. Philopatry and affiliation among red
Group extinction and female transfer in wild chimpan- colobus. Behaviour 130:253–270.
zees in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Z Tierpsy- Sugiyama Y. 1973. Social organization of wild chimpan-
chol 67:284–301. zees. In: Carpenter CR, editor. Behavioral regulators
Nunn CL. 1999. Collective benefits, free-riders, and of behavior in primates. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell
male extragroup conflict. In: Kappeler P, editor. Male University Press. p 68–80.
primates. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p Sugiyama Y. 1989. Population dynamics of chimpanzees
192–204. at Bossou, Guinea. In: Heltne PG, Marquardt LA,
Otterbein KF. 1985. Preface to Second Edition. In: The editors. Understanding chimpanzees. Cambridge, MA:
evolution of war: A cross-cultural study. New Haven, Harvard University Press. p 134–145.
CT: Human Relations Area Files. p 1–165. Sugiyama Y, Kawamoto S, Takenaka O, Kumazaki K,
Otterbein KF. 1997. The origins of war. Crit Rev 11:251– Miwa N. 1993. Paternity discrimination and inter-
277. group relationships of chimpanzees at Bossou. Pri-
Packer C, Herbst L, Pusey AE, Bygott JD, Hanby JP, mates 34:545–552.
Cairns SJ, Mulder MB. 1988. Reproductive success of Sussman RW, editor. 1997. The biological basis of hu-
30 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 42, 1999

man behavior: A critical review. New York: Prentice White FJ. 1998. Seasonality and socioecology: the impor-
Hall. p 1–382. tance of variation in fruit abundance to bonobo social-
Thompson JAM. 1997. The history, taxonomy and ecol- ity. Int J Primatol 19:1013–1027.
ogy of the bonobo (Pan paniscus Schwartz, 1929) with Wolpoff M. 1998. Paleoanthropology. New York: Alfred
a first description of a wild population living in a Knopf. p 1–370.
forest/savanna mosaic habitat. D. Phil. thesis, Oxford Wrangham RW. 1974. Artificial feeding of chimpanzees
University, p 1–315. and baboons in their natural habitat. Anim Behav
Tiger L. 1969. Men in groups. New York: Random House.
22:83–93.
p 1–254.
Trudeau MB, Bergmann-Riss E, Hamburg DA. 1981. Wrangham RW. 1975. The behavioral ecology of chimpan-
Towards an evolutionary perspective on aggressive zees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Ph.D. disser-
behavior: the chimpanzee evidence. In: Hamburg DA, tation, Cambridge University. p 1–280.
Trudeau MB, editors. Biobehavioral aspects of aggres- Wrangham RW. 1987. The significance of African apes
sion. New York: Alan Liss. p 27–40. for reconstructing human social evolution. In: Kinzey
Tuchman BW. 1962. The guns of August. New York: WG, editor. Primate models of hominid evolution.
Macmillan. p 1–511. Albany, NY: SUNY Press. p 51–71.
Turney-High HH. 1949. Primitive war: Its practice and Wrangham RW. 1999a. Why are male chimpanzees more
concepts. Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition
Press. p 1–288. hypothesis. In: Kappeler P, editor. Male primates.
Tutin CEG, Fernandez M. 1993. Composition of the diet Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p 248–258.
of chimpanzees and comparisons with that of sympat- Wrangham RW. 1999b. Is military incompetence adap-
ric lowland gorillas in the Lopé Reserve, Gabon. Am J
tive? Evol Hum Behav 20:3–17.
Primatol 30:195–211.
Tutin CEG, McGrew WC, Baldwin PJ. 1983. Social Wrangham RW, Peterson D. 1996. Demonic males: Apes
organization of savanna-dwelling chimpanzees, Pan and the origins of human violence. Boston, MA: Hough-
troglodytes verus, at Mt. Assirik, Senegal. Primates ton Mifflin. p 1–350.
24:154–173. Wrangham RW, Clark AP, Isabirye-Basuta G. 1992.
van Hoof JARAM. 1990. Intergroup competition and Female social relationships and social organization of
conflict in animals and man. In: Dennen JMG van der, the Kibale Forest chimpanzees. In: Nishida T, Mc-
Falger VSE, editors. Sociobiology and conflict: Evolu- Grew WC, Marler P, Pickford M, de Waal FBM,
tionary perspectives on competition, cooperation, vio- editors. Topics in primatology: Human origins (Vol-
lence and warfare. London: Chapman and Hall. p ume 1). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press. p 81–98.
23–54. Wrangham RW, Chapman CA, Clark AP, Isabirye-
van Schaik CP. 1996. Social evolution in primates: the Basuta G. 1996. Social ecology of Kanyawara chimpan-
role of ecological factors and male behavior. Proc Brit zees: implications for understanding the costs of great
Acad 88:9–31. ape groups. In: McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T,
van der Dennen JMG. 1995. The origin of war: The editors. Great ape societies. Cambridge: Cambridge
evolution of a male-coalitional reproductive strategy. University Press. p 45–57.
Groningen, Netherlands: Origin Press. p 1–861.
Wrangham RW, Conklin-Brittain NL, Hunt KD. 1998.
Watts DP. 1999. Coalitionary mate-guarding by male
chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Dietary response of chimpanzees and cercopithecines
Behav Ecol Sociobiol 44:43–55. to seasonal variation in fruit abundance: I. Antifeed-
White FJ. 1988. Party composition and dynamics in Pan ants. Int J Primatol 19:949–970.
paniscus. Int J Primatol 9:179–193. Zihlman AL. 1996. Looking back in anger. Nature
White FJ. 1996. Comparative socio-ecology of Pan panis- 384:35–36.
cus. In: McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T, editors. Zihlman AL, Cramer DL. 1978. Skeletal differences
Great ape societies. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- between pygmy (Pan paniscus) and common chimpan-
sity Press. p 29–41. zees (Pan troglodytes). Folia Primatol 29:86–94.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai