Anda di halaman 1dari 2

General Commentary

published: 10 February 2011


doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00021

High skies and oceans deep: polarity benefits or mental


simulation?
Daniël Lakens*
School of Innovation Sciences, Eindhoven University of Technology, Eindhoven, Netherlands
*Correspondence: d.lakens@tue.nl

A commentary on were faster to judge which of the two dots time benefits for right key-presses are not
was the lower yo-yo. The crossover pat- ubiquitous in the literature, nor is such a
Congruency between word position and tern observed in this study supports the main effect present in the study performed
meaning is caused by task-induced spatial idea that the default polarity codes of by Pecher and colleagues. Therefore, it is
attention up as +polar and down as −polar can be doubtful whether right key-presses should
by Pecher, D., Van Dantzig, S., Boot, I., Zanolie, reversed by framing the task in a different be coded as +polar.
K., and Huber, D. E. (2010). Front. Psychol. way. Proctor and Cho (2006, p. 428) simi- Overall, the results of Pecher and col-
1:30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00030 larly conclude: “In two-choice tasks with leagues provide little evidence against a
orthogonal stimulus and response sets, the polarity explanation for congruency effects
Pecher et al. (2010) presented targets (e.g., responses are coded relative to multiple between word meaning and vertical posi-
helicopter, submarine) up and down on a frames of reference.” tion. Given that previous studies that have
computer screen. Participants were either A similar reframing occurs in the study investigated the vertical representation of
asked to indicate whether these objects performed by Pecher and colleagues. When concepts often show that +polar words
were typically found in the ocean, or typi- thinking of skies, as in thinking of balloons, (e.g., powerful, moral) are categorized
cally found in the sky. The authors exam- high skies are the default. Therefore, up faster than −polar words (i.e., powerless,
ined whether congruency effects between becomes the default endpoint of the vertical immoral), and words presented up are cate-
the vertical position of words and their dimension, and words presented up on the gorized faster than words presented down, it
meaning were best accounted for by mental screen are responded to more quickly. When seems premature to exclude polarity effects
simulations or by polarity benefits (default thinking of oceans, as when thinking of yo- from influencing categorization times(e.g.,
asymmetries in the way people process yos, the deep ocean is the default. Therefore, Schubert, 2005; Meier et al., 2007). Indeed,
dimensions). I believe their conclusion down becomes the default endpoint of the as predicted by the polarity correspond-
that polarity benefits cannot account for the vertical dimension, and words presented ence principle (Proctor and Cho, 2006),
interaction in reaction times between the down on the screen are responded to more these experiments typically do not reveal
meaning and the position of words is at best quickly. This means that the interaction differences in the categorization times for
premature. Moreover, instead of explaining between task and position reported by –polar words presented up or down. At the
language understanding in terms of either Pecher and colleagues is an a priori predic- same time, studies have revealed that words
simulation processes or linguistic input, a tion of (and not evidence against) a polarity can direct attention upward or downward
more fruitful approach might be to exam- explanation. (Richardson et al., 2003; Bergen et al., 2007;
ine when meaning emerges from simulation The second polarity effect Pecher and col- Van Dantzig, 2009). Instead of attributing
processes, and when meaning is extracted leagues investigate is based on the assump- effects to either simulation processes or
from linguistic information (see Louwerse tion that right-hand key-presses receive a polarity effects, several researchers have
and Jeuniaux, 2010). reaction time benefit because right is +polar. recently proposed models of conceptual
Pecher and colleagues state that This prediction does not immediately fol- processing that rely on both semantic and
“Certain dimensions are always coded low from previous theoretical or empirical embodied information (Barsalou et  al.,
in the same way.” (p. 2). Although it is work. One study has revealed that responses 2008; Andrews et  al., 2009; Louwerse,
true that people are by default faster to are faster for arrows pointing toward the in press).
process above relationships compared to right than to the left, but importantly, these Such an interplay between linguistic
below relationships (Clark, 1969), studies effects are eliminated when arrows are sub- and embodied sources of meaning might
have shown that this default processing stituted by words (Olson and Laxar, 1973), be able to provide a more integrative
benefit can easily be reversed, depending and seem to be limited to explicit spatial model of conceptual thought, especially
on the nature of the task and character- categorizations where right key-presses for more abstract concepts. As an exam-
istics of the stimuli. For example, Banks are mapped onto stimuli presented up on ple, consider Lakoff and Johnson’s (1980)
et al. (1975) asked participants to indicate the screen (Weeks and Proctor, 1990; Cho Conceptual Metaphor Theory which
which of two dots was located above (vs. and Proctor, 2002). Furthermore, several describes how abstract concepts (e.g.,
below) the other. These dots were referred researchers explicitly note that left-to-right power, valence, morality) are mapped onto
to as balloons, or as yo-yos. Participants space is symmetric, and should by default concrete dimensions (e.g., verticality, size,
were faster to ­indicate which of the two not reveal polarity differences (Clark, 1973; brightness). These mappings are argued to
dots was the higher balloon, whereas they Seymour, 1974; Làdavas, 1988). Reaction result from experiential co-occurrence of

www.frontiersin.org February 2011  |  Volume 2  |  Article 21  |  1


Lakens Polarity correspondence or mental simulation?

the abstract and concrete domains, such Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., and Olson, G. M., and Laxar, K. (1973). Asymmetries in
Wilson, C. D. (2008). “Language and simulation in processing the terms “right” and “left.” J. Exp. Psychol.
as drooped postures when we feel down.
conceptual processing,” in Symbols, Embodiment, 100, 284–290.
This reasoning cannot explain the fact and Meaning, eds M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg, and Pecher, D., Van Dantzig, S., Boot, I., Zanolie, K., and
that practically all conceptual metaphors A. C. Graesser (Oxford: Oxford University Press), Huber, D. E. (2010). Congruency between word
map abstract + polar concepts onto con- 245–283. position and meaning is caused by task-induced
crete + polar endpoints of dimensions (e.g., Bergen, B., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., and Narayanan, spatial attention. Front. Psychol. 1:30. doi: 10.3389/
S. (2007). Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual fpsyg.2010.00030
happy  =  up, warmth  =  affection, power-
imagery in sentence comprehension. Cogn. Sci. 31, Proctor, R. W., and Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspond-
ful = large) and vice versa (see Krzeszowski, 733–764. ence: A general principle for performance of speeded
1997). Perhaps linguistic structures such as Cho, Y. S., and Proctor, R. W. (2002). Influences of hand binary classification tasks. Psychol. Bull. 132, 416–442.
polarity differences constrain the specific posture and hand position on compatibility effects Richardson, D. C., Spivey, M. J., Barsalou, L. W., and
mappings that emerge between abstract for up–down stimuli mapped to left–right responses: McRae, K. (2003). Spatial representations activated
evidence for a hand-referent hypothesis. Percept. during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cogn. Sci.
and concrete dimensions, thereby shaping 27, 767–780.
Psychophys. 64, 1301–1315.
how concepts are grounded. Clark, H. H. (1969). Linguistic processes in deductive Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: vertical positions
Activated word associations, statisti- reasoning. Psychol. Rev. 76, 387–404. as perceptual symbols of power. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
cal regularities in language, and linguistic Clark, H. H. (1973). “Space, time, semantics and the 89, 1–21.
structures can guide simulation processes child,” in Cognitive Development and the Acquisition Seymour, P. H. K. (1974). Asymmetries in judgments of
of Language, ed. T. E. Moore (New York: Academic verticality. J. Exp. Psychol. 102, 447–455.
(for examples, see Barsalou et  al., 2008; Press), 27–63. Van Dantzig, S. (2009). Mind the body: grounding
Louwerse and Jeuniaux, 2010), and Krzeszowski, T. P. (1997). Angels and Devils in Hell: conceptual knowledge in perception and action.
should be taken into account when aim- Elements of Axiology in Semantics. Warsaw: Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Erasmus
ing to explain language comprehension. Wydawn. University, Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
Làdavas, E. (1988). Asymmetries in processing hori- Weeks, D. J., and Proctor, R. W. (1990). Salient-features cod-
Linguistic and concrete information both
zontal and vertical dimensions. Mem. Cogn. 16, ing in the translation between orthogonal stimulus–re-
contribute to conceptual thought; the 377–382. sponse dimensions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 119, 355–366.
challenge for future research lies in speci- Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live
fying how meaning emerges from their By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Received: 14 December 2010; accepted: 01 February 2011;
dynamic interaction. Louwerse, M. M. (in press). Symbol interdependency in published online: 10 February 2011.
symbolic and embodied cognition. Top. Cogn. Sci. Citation: Lakens D (2011) High skies and oceans deep:
doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01106.x. [Epub ahead polarity benefits or mental simulation? Front. Psychology
References of print]. 2:21. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00021
Andrews, M., Vigliocco, G., and Vinson, D. P. (2009). Louwerse, M. M., and Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguis- This article was submitted to Frontiers in Cognition, a
Integrating experiential and distributional data to tic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. specialty of Frontiers in Psychology.
learn semantic representations. Psychol. Rev. 116, Cognition 114, 96–104. Copyright © 2011 Lakens. This is an open-access article sub-
463–498. Meier, B. P., Hauser, D. J., Robinson, M. D., Friesen, C. ject to an exclusive license agreement between the authors
Banks, W. P., Clark, H. H., and Lucy, P. (1975). The locus of K., and Schjeldahl, K. (2007). What’s “up” with god? and Frontiers Media SA, which permits unrestricted use,
the semantic congruity effect in comparative judgments. Vertical space as a representation of the divine. J. Pers. distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 104, 35–47. Soc. Psychol. 93, 699–710. the original authors and source are credited.

Frontiers in Psychology  |  Cognition February 2011  |  Volume 2  |  Article 21  |  2

Anda mungkin juga menyukai