Anda di halaman 1dari 11

Structural Determinants of Men's and Women's Personal Networks

Author(s): Gwen Moore


Source: American Sociological Review, Vol. 55, No. 5 (Oct., 1990), pp. 726-735
Published by: American Sociological Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2095868
Accessed: 01/11/2010 05:46

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asa.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Sociological Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
American Sociological Review.

http://www.jstor.org
STRUCTURAL DETERMINANTS OF
MEN'S AND WOMEN'S PERSONAL NETWORKS*

GWEN MOORE
State Universityof New Yorkat Albany

Men's and women's personal networks often differ in composition, with women's more
focused on family and men's on nonkin, especially coworkers. Using data from the 1985
General Social Survey, I find that these gender differences arise in part from dissimilar
social structural locations of men and women, which lead to distinct opportunities for
and constraints on the formation of close personal ties. Most gender differences in
network composition disappear or are considerably reduced when variables related to
employment,family, and age are controlled. However, some gender differences remain.
Women have a larger number, higher proportion, and greater diversity of kin ties in their
personal networks than men, even when compared with men in similar social structural
positions.

Over thepasttwo decadesextensiveresearch suggest that women are less able to use net-
on social networks has demonstratedthe works as instrumentalresources and that men
importance of networks in diverse facets of benefit more from the diverse and extensive
social life, including social support (e.g., networksuseful in findingjobs and advancing
Kadushin 1982), employment (Granovetter their careers. Much research on networks as
1974, 1982; Lin, Ensel and Vaughn 1981; Lin instrumentalresourceshas focusedon weakties.
1982), and power and influence in organiza- But organizationalstudies, usually measuring
tions, communities,and nations (Laumannand strongerties, have confirmedfindings of gen-
Pappi 1976; Moore 1979; LaumannandKnoke der differences, often finding that men have
1987; Miller 1986). Indeed, networkties have more extensive ties than women, especially to
frequentlybeen described as social resources powerfulpersonsin workorganizations(Miller
thatoffer valuablesupport,acquaintances,and 1986; Brass 1988).
information(Lin 1982; McPhersonand Smith- Observed gender differences in networks
Lovin 1982, p.884, 1986; Campbell,Marsden have sometimes been attributedto contrasting
and Hurlbert1986). dispositions of men and women toward inter-
Studies of personal networks, comprised of personalrelationships,concludingthat women
an individualand the othersto whom he or she are more disposed to maintainingcloser ties to
is connected,have found that women and men kin andfewerties outsidethefamily(e.g., Miller
usually have networksof similar size (Fischer 1976; Chodorow 1978; Gilligan 1982). In con-
1982; Marsden 1987). These studies have also trast, the structural perspective has ascribed
found, however, large gender differences in gender differences in networks to the dissimi-
networkcomposition.When comparedto men, lar social structurallocations of women and
women have fewer ties to nonkinandmore ties men (Fischerand Oliker 1983).
to kin, while men include more coworkers in While researchersfrequentlyhave adopteda
theirnetworks(Fischerand Oliker 1983; Well- structuralperspective to explain network dif-
man 1985; Marsden 1987). These findings ferences, the structuralperspective theory has
*Directcorrespondence yet to be subjectedto a comprehensiveempiri-
to GwenMoore,Depart-
mentof Sociology,Social Science340, SUNY at foon Lai and Jeff Torlina for research assistance,
Albany,Albany,New York12222.Thispaperwas and Sharon Baumgardner,Joan Cipperly, and Ei-
presentedat the 1990meetingof theAmericanSo- leen Pellegrinofor manuscriptpreparation.The data
ciologicalAssociation.I thankRichardAlba,Wal- from the 1985 NORC General Social Survey were
ter Ensel,CharlesKadushin,GinaWan-foonLai, obtainedwith the assistanceof BrianFisherfromthe
GlennaSpitze,andASR editorsandreviewersfor Center for Social and Demographic Analysis at
helpfulcommentson previousdrafts,GinaWan- SUNY Albany.

AmericanSociological Review, 1990,Vol. 55 (October:726-735) 726


DETERMINANTSOF MEN'S AND WOMEN'S NETWORKS 727

cal test. Previousstudiesof differencesbetween 1956). Consideringoccupationalgroupsrather


men's andwomen's networkshave largelyused thanspecific occupations,high-statuspositions
local samples and a few structuralvariables may lead their occupantsto form more ties to
(Fischer and Oliker 1983; Campbell 1988; coworkers than do low-status positions. This
Wellman 1985; Gerstel 1988). In this study, I reflects the value of ties among occupants of
investigated structuraldeterminantsof men's high-statuspositions for careermobility, work
and women's networks to identify structural conditions permittinginteraction,and the lim-
factors and to determineif gender differences ited time these persons have for purely social
in networkscan be explained by these factors. relationships.
Family structureand age also affect network
RESEARCHHYPOTHESES composition.The presenceof childrenat home
constrainsthe formationof ties, especiallythose
Blau's axiom, that "social associationsdepend to nonkin who are not neighbors.Persons who
on opportunitiesfor social contact" (1977, p. aremarriedhave moreties to kin andneighbors
281), summarizes the structuralperspective. and fewer ties to othersthando unmarriedper-
Social relations occur within an opportunity sons (Fischer 1982, p. 253; Wellman 1985;
context that precludes or makes possible vari- Gerstel 1988; Hurlbertand Acock 1990). Net-
ous kinds of social contacts (Blau 1977,p.79; works also differ by age: Nonkin ties peak in
Marsdenforthcoming,p. 1). FischerandOliker, the earlythirtiesandgenerallydecline afterthat
reportingon data from a study of friendship (Fischer 1982, p. 253; Fischerand Oliker 1983;
ties in northernCaliforniacommunities,argued Marsden1987, pp. 128-129).
thatstructuralopportunitiesand constraintsare Yet, in one respect,the structuralperspective
the primarycause of genderdifferencesin net- allows a morefundamentalrole for gender.The
works:"Weproposethatthe differingpositions work and family variablesthat shape network
of women andmen in the workforce, in marital structuresfrequently affect men and women
roles, and in parenthoodcreatedifferentsets of differently.As a result,genderand social struc-
opportunitiesfor andconstraintson friendship- tural variables can be expected to interact in
building"(1983, p. 130; also see Fischer 1982, theireffects on personalnetworks(Fischerand
pp. 253-255; Wellman 1985; Gerstel 1988). Oliker 1983). Women are not only less likely
Structuralopportunities,particularlyjobs out- to be employed outside the home and to have
side of the home andhigherincome, occurmore lower individual incomes than men, but they
often for men than for women. On the other also generally retainresponsibilityfor the ma-
hand,structuralconstraintson the development jority of housework and childcare even when
of networkties, suchas responsibilityfor house- they are employed full-time; men's contribu-
work and childcare, are experienced more of- tion to housework is relatively unaffected by
ten by women. employmentstatus(Epstein 1988, pp. 209-212;
Importantstructuralvariables are those re- Berk 1988). Thus marriage and parenthood
lating to work, family, and age. Work-related often constrainwomen's opportunitiesto form
factors,includingpaidemployment,educational network ties to nonkin outside the neighbor-
attainment, and income, are positively (and hood, while marriageoffers men the time and
often strongly)relatedto networksize and ties opportunityto form networkties beyond local
to nonkin;they increasethe opportunityto form andkin boundaries,andhavingchildrenproba-
ties with personsoutside the family and neigh- bly affects men's networks little (Fischer and
borhood(Fischer 1982, pp. 251-252; Marsden Oliker 1983, p. 129; Wellman 1985; Gerstel
1987, p. 129). 1988; Campbell 1988, pp. 191-194).1An addi-
Although key factors in the structuralper-
I Ties with kin are readymade,an advantage to
spective relate to paid employment, Fischer
(1982) did not find occupationto be relatedto persons with scarce time to develop new relation-
network ties. Nevertheless, it is probablethat ships (Wellman 1985, pp. 174-176). Kin may also
assist with childcare,and thus among families with
some occupations offer more opportunityand
children at home may be in more frequentcontact.
incentive thanothersto form ties with cowork- In addition,parentsare likely to have a largernum-
ers. Tightly knit occupations such as typogra- ber of ties to neighbors' as a result of both neigh-
phers and coal miners have been found to fos- bors' proximity and the propensity of children to
ter comradeship among coworkers (Fischer form local friendshipsthat bring their parents,par-
1982, pp. 104-05; Lipset, Trow and Coleman ticularlytheirmothers,in contact with neighbors.
728 AMERICANSOCIOLOGICALREVIEW

tional effect of women's household responsi- specified neitherthe contentof the discussions
bilities may be thatfull-time paid employment nor the number of persons to be named
leads to a decline in ties of all types other than (Marsden1987, p. 123;Burt 1985, p. 119). The
to coworkers,especiallyto nonkin.Among men majority of the 1531 respondents with valid
paid employmentmay increase ties to nonkin, networkdata (85.6%) named between one and
especially coworkers,while having little effect five persons,8.9 percentnamedno one and 5.5
on kin ties. percentnamedsix or morepersons.For each of
Although network structureshave various the first five persons named, additionalques-
components, the focus in this study is on net- tions focused on their relationshipsto the re-
workrangeratherthanotherstructuralfeatures, spondentand otherinformationas well.
such as density. A broad range of ties, with Because persons named were likely to be
many strong or weak connections to diverse those to whom respondents felt close, these
others, is often seen as a valuableinstrumental networkdataarebest seen as measuresof strong
resource,while networkdensityis moreclosely ties. For most respondentsthese were subsets
associated with social support(e.g., Campbell, of a more extensive networkof strongties - a
Marsden and Hurlbert 1986; Marsden 1987; subsetrepresentingthosepersonswho firstcame
Campbell1988, p. 181;Kadushin1982). Range to mind when thinkingaboutrecentdiscussions
has been conceptualized in various ways, in- of importantmatters.3For the small proportion
cluding volume of contacts and diversity of of respondentswho namedmore thanfive per-
alters (Burt 1983; Campbell, Marsden and sons, the data on reportedties are incomplete,
Hurlbert1986;Campbell1988).I measuredvol- omittingrelationshipsto the sixth and, for sev-
ume of contacts as the total numberof persons enteen respondents,personsnamed later.
namedas well as the numberof personsof each Respondentswere askedto indicatethe ways
type (e.g., kin or coworkers)in the network.A they were connected to each of the first five
networkwould have a greatervolume of ties if persons they named, specifically: spouse, par-
it had a largeroverall size and a largernumber ent, sibling, child, other family, coworker,
of specific types of ties. I measuredthe diver- member of group to which they belong (such
sity of alters in two ways: (1) the relative pro- as churchorclub),neighbor,friend,professional
portionof kin in the network,and (2) the num- advisor or consultant, other nonkin. The first
ber of different nonkin and kin types in the five relationships were coded as kin and the
network.Networkswith morediversityof types last six as nonkin.4In orderto clearly separate
of ties have greaterrange and indicate integra- engage in shared activities (Caldwell and Peplau
tion into diversesocial spheres.(Marsden1987). 1982; Aukett, Ritchie and Mill 1988). This argu-
ment for the inflation of women's network size,
DATA AND METHODS however, is not supportedby the resultsof network
research using differently worded questions. Two
The 1985 General Social Survey, a national previousstudiesgatheringdataon ties involvingboth
probability sample of 1534 English-speaking discussions and mutualactivities found, as does this
Americanseighteen years old and older, gath- one, that women and men have networksof similar
ered data on respondents' backgrounds,atti- mean size (Fischer and Oliker 1983; Gerstel 1988).
The more inclusive name generatorsused in those
tudes, and other variables,includingmeasures
studieswith less focus on close ties, however,yielded
of personalnetworks(Davis and Smith 1988). far largernetworks,with a mean of 18 for both gen-
Respondentswere asked to name people with ders in the two studies.
whom they had discussed "importantmatters" I In a study of the personal networks of 33 per-
over the past six months.2 The interviewers sons in Torontothatgathereddataon all individuals
with whomrespondentswere"significantlyin touch,"
2 The wording was, "From time to time, most Wellman found a median of four intimate ties as
people discuss importantmatterswith otherpeople. well as seven less intimateones (1985, pp. 164-165).
Looking back over the last six months - who are His results suggest that most persons do not have
thepeople with whom you discussed mattersimpor- more thanthe maximumof five close ties described
tantto you? Justtell me theirfirst names or initials." in the NORC data.
If fewer thanfive nameswere mentioned,interview- 4The wording was: "Hereis a list of some of the
ers were instructedto probe,with "Anyoneelse?" A ways in which people are connected to each other.
sex bias in the wordingof the networkquestionwas Some people can be connected to you in more than
suggestedby anASRreviewerdue to women'sgreater one way. For example, a man could be your brother
propensityto talk with theirassociates and men's to and he may belong to your churchand be your law-
DETERMINANTSOF MEN'S AND WOMEN'S NETWORKS 729

kin and nonkin and to distinguish the more Table 1. Network Size and Composition Differences
voluntary nonkin ties, persons who were de- Between Men and Women
scribed by the respondent as being connected
Men Women
both by kinship and other relationships were
coded only as kin. Nonkin were all network Variables Mean S.D. Mean S.D.
memberswho were not describedas kin. Overall networksize 3.00 1.83 3.02 1.73
I used 12 dependentvariablesin this analy-
Absolute compositionb
sis. One, networksize, was the total numberof
# of kin 1.50 1.27 1.81** 1.34
persons named.5Eight variables reflected the
# of nonkinc 1.70 1.49 1.40** 1.32
"absolute"composition of each personal net-
work. These were the numbersof kin, nonkin, # of neighbors .19 .58 .26* .60
neighbors, friends, group members, advisors, # of friends 1.41 1.40 1.27* 1.28
coworkers,and othernonkin.6Three measures # of coworkers .62 1.02 .36** .75
of "relative"composition were included: the # of groupmembers .32 .79 .29 .75
proportionof kin, the numberof differentkin # of advisors .31 .73 .18** .52
types, andthe numberof differentnonkintypes # of others .06 .33 .04 .23
in the network. Relative composition
Obviously, the key independentvariable in Proportionkin .51 .38 .58** .36
this analysis was gender (male = 1). The other # of kin types 1.17 .88 1.43** .95
independentvariables can be categorized into # of nonkintypes 1.56 1.31 1.41* 1.28
one of four groups: (1) Variables concerned
withfamily structureincludednumberof chil- *p < .05, two-tailedt-test p < .01, two-tailedt-test
dren under 19 years old in the household, and aNetwork size is computed for all respondentsexcept
three for whom these data were missing (N = 1531). For
marital status (currentlymarried= 1). (2) Age persons with networks of size 6 or more, the number is
variablesincludedtherespondent'sage in years, recodedto the mean of 6.5.
andalso age squared(age2) to test for suspected bThe absolute and relative composition measures are
nonlineareffects. (3) The numberof years of based on all persons with networks of size one or larger.
education was the only variable in the educa- Thosenaming no one areexcluded(N = 1395).Therangefor
absolutemeasuresis 0 to 5.
tion category.(4) Finally,employmentvariables c Respondentscould reportmultiplerelationswith each
included employmentstatus (a set of dummy networkmember.For example, one tie could be described
variablesrepresentingfull time, parttime, and as a brotherwho is also a neighbor. In the calculation of
no employment),personal income (in 17 cate- number of nonkin of each type, persons who are also
gories, each recoded to equal the midpoint in describedas kin are excluded.
thousands),and type of occupation (a dummy
variablewhereprofessional/managerial = 1 and RESULTS
all other occupations = 0). Unfortunately,the
datadid not providemeasuresof the amountof Table 1 presentsthe means and standarddevia-
time devoted to householdlaborand childcare. tions for men and women on the variables
measuringnetwork size and absolute and rela-
yer. When I read you a name, please tell me all the tive networkcomposition.7While overall men
ways thatpersonis connectedto you."This question and women cited the same numberof persons
was followed by the probe:"Whatother ways?"
I After recoding,networksize had a range of 0 to in theirnetworks,they differedin the predicted
6.5. NORC coded all responses of 6 or more ties as ways on most othernetworkmeasures.In terms
6. This included a total of 84 persons, of whom just of absolutenumbersof personsof varyingrela-
17 named seven or more ties. Following the practice tionshipsto respondents,men's networks,when
of Marsden (1987, p. 126), I recoded the category
I The issue of selectivity bias has been raised as a
including all networks of size 6 and larger to its
meanvalue of 6.5. Because few respondentsreported potential problem in the analysis of network data
networks of more than five persons, this truncation when certain cases are systematically excluded on
was unlikely to seriously bias the analyses. the basis of the dependent variable (Marsden and
6 While the absolute composition measures in- Hurlbert 1987). In this analysis persons with net-
cluded counts of the number of nonkin of various works of size 0 were excluded from analyses other
types (e.g., friends and coworkers),it was not logi- than those of networksize. However, because these
cal to include such measuresfor kin since the num- persons made up less than 10 per cent of the total
berof specific types of kin (e.g., parentsandspouses) sample, selection bias was not likely to be a serious
is limited. problemhere (Marsdenand Hurlbert1987, p. 345).
730 AMERICANSOCIOLOGICALREVIEW
Table 2. Regressionof Network Size and Compositionon Genderand StructuralVariables

Absolute Composition Relative Composition


Category/ # #
Independent Network # # # Co- # # # % Kin Nonkin
Variable Size Kin Nonkin Ngbrs. workers Friends Group Advisors Kin Types Types

Gender -.248** -.278** .156* -.014 .016 .049 .030 .141** -.042* -.279** .062
(male = 1) (-2.62) (-3.59) (1.97) (-.39) (.32) (.63) (.65) (3.74) (-1.99) (-5.32) (.83)
Age
Age (years) -.015 -.043 .040** .005 .017* .025* .004 .015* -.013** -.035** .010
(-.96) (-3.40)** (3.06) (.92) (2.07) (1.97) (.56) (2.49) (-3.73) (-4.01) (.79)
Age2 a -.039 .397** -.481** -.049 -.202* -.354** -.026 -.149* .145** .290** -.176
(-.25) (3.00) (-3.55) (-.79) (-2.37) (-2.68) (-.33) (-2.32) (3.97) (3.23) (-1.38)
Education .134** .021 .065** .017** -.007 .048** .041 ** .026** -.011** .011 .059**
(years) (7.81) (1.50) (4.44) (2.62) (-.73) (3.39) (4.86) (3.75) (-2.75) (1.17) (4.32)
Employment
Full-time -.158 -.190 .019 -.093 .305** -.010 .016 -.061 -.039 -.116 .094
employment (-1.21) (-1.78) (.17) (-1.88) (4.44) (-.10) (.26) (-1.19) (-1.33) (-1.61) (.92)
Part-time .209 .072 .165 .007 .267** .166 .290** .044 -.014 .034 .453**
employment (1.36) (.57) (1.28) (.12) (3.31) (1.33) (3.90) (.72) (-.39) (.40) (3.76)
Occupation .207 -.034 .299** -.017 .238** .200* .066 .111* -.043 .031 .334**
(1.85) (-.38) (3.22) (-.41) (4.07) (2.21) (1.23) (2.52) (-1.72) (.51) (3.83)
Incomed .011* -.004 .010** -.003 .012** .008* -.002 -.000 -.002 -.001 .004
(2.41) (-1.12) (2.83) (-1.75) (5.37) (2.34) (-.84) (-.25) (-1.66) (-.49) (1.29)
Family
Maritalstatus .161 .646** -.649** -.045 -.023 -.580** -.041 -.023 .206** .690** -.318**
(married= 1) (1.75) (8.53) (-8.36) (-1.27) (-.48) (-7.68) (-.92) (-.64) (9.85) (13.45) (-4.35)
Children -.093* -.063 .020 .049** -.007 .019 .041* .017 -.008 -.014 .062
(#under 19) (-2.32) (-1.90) (.58) (3.17) (-.33) (.57) (2.09) (1.03) (-.82) (-.62) (1.93)

Intercept 2.091** 2.388** .092 -.040 -.131 .545 -.405* -.492** .902** 1.849** .562
(5.25) (7.38) (.28) (-.26) (-.63) (1.69) (-2.11) (-3.13) (10.11) (8.43) (1.80)
R2 .154 .078 .159 .027 .173 .120 .048 .050 .126 .154 .127

*p < .05 *p < .01


a
Coefficient multipliedby 1000.
b Employmentstatusis a set of dummy variables:full-time = 1, other = 0; part-time= 1, other = 0; not employed full-
time or part-timeis the omittedcategory.
c Managerialand professionaloccupations= 1, other occupations= 0.
d Personalincome was originallycoded in 17 categories.The midpointof each, in thousands,is used.
Notes: Unstandardizedcoefficients, t-statisticsare in parentheses.

comparedto women's, consisted of fewer kin lute networkcomposition,andrelativenetwork


and more nonkin, and included fewer neigh- composition were regressed on gender and
borsbutmorecoworkers,advisors,andfriends. social structuralvariables.The resultsareshown
In relative composition, women's networks, in Table2.8 Becausepastresearchhas suggested
when comparedto men's, incorporateda larger thatsocial structuralvariablesandgenderinter-
proportionof kin overallas well as morediffer- act in the formationof networks, interactions
ent types of kin, but fewer different types of were systematically tested in all equations. A
nonkin. All but two of the mean differences in global test was employed in each case by intro-
Table 1 are statistically significant at the .05 ducinga completeset of producttermsbetween
level or greater.
The remainderof my analysis focused on 8 The "othernonkin"measurewas not examined
structuraland gender determinantsof network further, since, as a residual category, its meaning
composition. Measuresof network size, abso- was unclear.
DETERMINANTSOF MEN'S AND WOMEN'S NETWORKS 731

gender and all other independent variables. exception of the numberof advisors). Nor did
Addinginteractiontermssignificantlyimproved the presence of children have a differential
R2 for three of the 11 dependent variables: impact on the absolute composition of men's
numberof kin, numberof kin types, and num- andwomen's networks.Of the interactionsthat
ber of advisors.Models including all of the in- were significant, the most importanthad to do
teraction terms are presented in Table 3. Be- with the numberof kin. One significant inter-
cause of collinearity, interpretingthe individ- action was between genderand the categoryof
ual coefficients for the interactionterms was employment-relatedvariables (F4,1331= 2.763,
somewhatdifficult. Therefore,I tested models p < .05). More refined analysis (not presented)
in whichI includedinteractionswith each group suggests that this difference had to do with
of variablesseparatelyandthencombinedthose employment status: Full-time employment
groups that produced significant increases in among women reducedthe numberof kin ties
R2.These are presentedas the "trimmed"mod- named;no similareffect was foundamongmen.
els shown in Table 3. Note that althoughnone Another significant interaction involved age
of the interactionswith the individualemploy- (F2,1331
= 4.698, p < .01) which affected the
ment variableshad a significanteffect on num- number of kin named among men, but not
ber of kin, for example, taken together they among women. The interactionof gender and
significantlyimprovedthe R2of the model as a family variablesin the advisors equationindi-
whole. cated thatmarriedmen and those with children
(but not women with the same family statuses)
NetworkSize and AbsoluteNetwork namedmore advisorsamong their close ties.
Social structuralvariablesclearly had more
Composition
importantoverall effects than gender. Earlier
On the whole, gender differences in network research found that family, employment, and
composition were considerablyreduced when age variables have zero-orderrelationshipsto
structuralvariableswere controlled.Abouthalf networkstructures(e.g., Fischer 1982; Fischer
of the differencesthatwere statisticallysignifi- andOliker 1983; Marsden1987;Gerstel 1988).
cant in Table 1 were not significantin Table 2. Each of these structuralvariables played an
One of the remainingdifferences, the naming importantrole in some of the equations. For
of nonkin as networkmembers,had its magni- instance, age had ratherconsistent and nonlin-
tude reduced by half. The main exceptions to ear effects. Nonkin ties tended to rise during
this patternwere network size, where gender the young-adultyears and then fall after that
differences were significant only in Table 2, point, while kin ties (only among men) dis-
and the number of kin. Women apparently played the opposite pattern.Maritalstatushad
maintainclose ties to a largernumberof per- especially large effects. Compared with cur-
sons, especially kin, than do similarly situated rently unmarriedpersons, marriedpersons in-
men.9On the other hand, men in general have cludedmorekin andfewer nonkin(particularly
more ties to advisors and to nonkin. friends) in their networks (see Hurlbert and
Equally telling is the limited numberof in- Acock 1990 for similarfindingsusing the same
teractioneffects involving gender. Significant data set). The expectation that children in the
interactionsoccurredonly in the equationsfor householdwould have an effect similarto mar-
kin, kin types, andadvisors.Suchresultslargely riage - increasing ties to kin and neighbors
refute the expectationthat similar social struc- while decreasing those to nonkin who are not
tural positions have different impacts on neighbors- was not confirmed.Childrendid
women's and men's networks. For example, increase their parents' ties to neighbors (and
the predictionthat marriagewould, in general, groupmembers),buttheirpresenceled to fewer
increase men's nonkin ties while decreasing kin ties and smallernetworksoverall.
women's was not supported(with the modest Net of othervariables,paid employmenthad
I This statementis not affected by the reversalof
little impact on absolute networkcomposition
otherthanincreasingthe numberof ties to group
gender's sign between the kin equations with and
withoutinteractions.The positive gendercoefficient
members, the unremarkableeffect of increas-
in the equationwith interactions(Table 3) does not ing ties to coworkers,and the previouslynoted
indicate that men named more kin; it only partly decrease in kin ties among women. But the re-
counterbalancesthe negative impact of the age in- lated variables of education, occupation, and
teractionsthat apply to men but not women. income had stronger effects, generally being
732 AMERICANSOCIOLOGICALREVIEW

Table 3. Regressionof Network Compositionon Genderand StructuralVariableswith InteractionTerms

# Kin # Kin Types # Advisors


Category/ Full Trimmed Full Trimmed Full Trimmed
IndependentVariables Model Model Model Model Model Model
Gender 1.295* 1.097* .862 .647 .090 .024
(2.00) (2.03) (1.96) (1.77) (.28) (.42)
Age
Age -.019 -.018 -.017 -.017 .013 .016
6
(-1.19) (-1.10) (-1.57) (-1.58) (1.68) (2.61)
Age2 .192 .165 .134 .127 -.122 -.166*
(1.14) (1.01) (1.18) (1.15) (-1.49) (-2.57)
Education .027 .018 .018 .009 .028** .026**
(1.32) (1.28) (1.26) (.93) (2.81) (3.70)
Employment
Full-timeemployment -.233 -.245 -.164 -.168 .051 -.080
(-1.57) (-1.67) (-1.63) (-1.68) (.70) (-1.54)
Part-timeemployment .217 .218 .146 .150 .118 .044
(1.40) (1.42) (1.40) (1.44) (1.57) (.73)
Occupation -.050 -.025 .001 .023 .090 .116-
(-.40) (-.21) (.01) (.29) (1.48) (2.65)
Income -.011 -.010 -.005 -.005 -.007* -.001
(-1.51) (-1.46) (-1.07) (-.97) (-1.99) (-.50)
Family
Maritalstatus .680** .654** .684** .691* -.067 -.082
(6.87) (8.49) (10.21) (13.26) (-1.39) (-1.74)
Children -.061 -.071* -.016 -.020 -.009 -.014
(-1.30) (-2.11) (-.49) (-.87) (-.38) (-.66)

Interactionof gender (male) with:

Age
Age -.063* -.067** -.047** -.046** .008
(-2.41) (-2.66) (-2.62) (-2.72) (.62)
Age2 .562* .612* .426* .434* -.119
(2.05) (2.33) (2.30) (2.45) (-.89)
Education -.017 -.017 -.006
(-.61) (-.86) (-.43)
Employment
Full-time employment .230 .244 .182 .192 -.255*
(1.02) (1.09) (1.19) (1.26) (-2.32)
Part-timeemployment -.354 -.350 -.291 -.293 -.182
(-1.33) (-1.32) (-1.62) (-1.63) (-1.41)
Occupation .085 .036 .101 .054 .062
(.47) (.22) (.82) (.49) (.70)
Income .010 .009 .006 .005 .008* --
(1.14) (1.04) (.98) (.83) (1.98)
Family
Maritalstatus -.063 .019 .125 .139
(-.39) (.17) (1.62) (1.89)
Children -.014 -.007 .043 .055
(-.21) (-.16) (1.30) (1.82)
Intercept 1.739** 1.853* 1.394"' 1.500** -.450* -.415**
(4.06) (4.79) (4.80) (5.73) (-2.16) (-2.61)
R2 .092 .091 .168 .167 .062 .056

F for interactionset 2.264 3.306 2.451 3.552 1.915 4.344


d.f. 9,1328 6,1331 9,1328 6,1331 9,1328 2,1335
Significance level .016 .003 .009 .002 .046 .013

sp <.05 **p <.01


Notes: Unstandardized coefficients, t-statistics are in parentheses. For variable descriptions see Table 2.
DETERMINANTSOF MEN'S AND WOMEN'S NETWORKS 733

positively associatedwith networksize andties had networks that contained similar numbers
to nonkinoverall as well as to coworkers,advi- of nonkin of various types when variables re-
sors, and friends.The numberof kin ties, how- lated to work, family, and age were controlled.
ever, was not dependenton education,income, However, some gender differences remained,
or occupation. The general pattern of these primarily in ties to kin where women's net-
variables indicates that, as expected, persons works contained more and larger proportions
who aremoreeconomically,educationally,and of kin as well as more types of kin.
occupationallyprivilegedformlargernetworks Contraryto expectations, gender and struc-
and establishmore close ties to nonkinof vari- turalvariablesrarelyinteractedin their impact
ous types thando less privileged persons. on networkcomposition. Marriageand parent-
hood, for instance, did not impact negatively
RelativeNetworkComposition on nonkin network formation for women.
However, a few gender-specific effects oc-
Broadly similar patterns were found for the curred, particularly for employment-related
measures of relative network composition variables and age. Full-time employment de-
(Tables 2 and 3). Genderwas a significantfac- creasedthe numberand diversityof kin only in
tor only for kin ties, with women's networks women's networks.Also, increasingage had a
being comprised of larger proportionsof kin strongerimpacton men's thanon women's kin
and more types of kin than were the men's. ties. Severalstructuralvariablesconnectedwith
There were also a few significant interaction employment,maritalstatus,andage were more
effects involving gender, but only in the equa- importantthan gender in their impact on net-
tion for kin types. As before, these were with workcomposition.Ties to nonkin,for instance,
employmentstatus(F41331 = 3.448,p < .01) and were less numerousamong marriedand older
age (F2 1331 = 4.458,p < *05).The role of gender persons,butmorecommonamongpersonswho
in the kin equationsindicates that women, es- were highly educated or in professional and
pecially those who are not employed full time, managerialoccupations.The structuralvariables
not only maintainedclose ties to more kin, but often had the opposite effects for ties to kin.
also maintainedmore diverse kin ties than did Overall, these analyses offer considerable
similarly placed men. This result is consistent support for the structuralperspective. Net of
with women's roles as "kinkeepers,"persons othervariables,genderhad little impacton the
who keep members of the extended family in absoluteandrelativenonkinmeasures,indicat-
touchwith one another(Rosenthal1985, p. 965; ing that men and women with similar family-
di Leonardo1987, p. 443). and work-relatedcharacteristicshave nonkin
Some social structuralvariableshad stronger networksthat are also similar.
effects thangenderon relativenetworkcompo- On the other hand, structuralvariables did
sition. Maritalstatushadthe strongestandmost not fully eliminate the effect of gender on kin
consistenteffect; being marriedled to a higher ties. In theirpersonalnetworks,womenincluded
proportionandgreatervarietyof kin types while more and larger proportionsof kin as well as
having the opposite effect on ties to nonkin. more diverse kin types than did similarly situ-
The effects of education, employment status, atedmen, althoughthe disparitieswere reduced
and occupation were also noteworthy for the to some degree when women workedfull-time.
diversity of nonkin types. In addition, educa- Women may be disposed to focus more of their
tion reduced the proportionof close ties in- close ties on family members, men more on
volving kin. ties to nonkin.These resultsareconsistentwith
studies of caregiving, which typically have
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION found that women are expected to be and are
more active thanmen in the maintenanceof kin
In the aggregate, women and men differed ties and in the care of dependentrelatives, such
considerably in their network compositions as an aged parent(e.g., Leigh 1982; Rosenthal
thoughnot in networksizes. Comparedto men, 1985; Sherman,Ward,and LaGory 1988). But
women's networkswere comprisedof morekin a structuralfactor, proximity,-may also be im-
and fewer nonkin (except neighbors). Most of portant:Older women have been found to live
these gender differences disappearedor were nearerto theirgeographicallyclosest child than
reduced, however, when structuralvariables do men of the sameage (SpitzeandLogan 1989,
were controlled.In particular,men and women Table 3). The proximity factor may also be
734 AMERICANSOCIOLOGICALREVIEW

importantto younger women (perhapsin rela- do men, however, as long as they remain the
tion to theirparents);it could offer the opportu- primary caretakers and kinkeepers in most
nity to maintainstrong ties to relatives, since, families.
in spite of the telephone, frequencyof kin in- This research also offers clear support for
teractionis often foundto dependmost strongly Epstein's more general claim, that "the over-
on geographicdistance(Lee 1980,pp. 928-929). whelmingevidence createdby the past decades
In balance, the evidence here supports the of researchon gender supportsthe theory that
structuralperspective:Most genderdifferences genderdifferentiation... is best explainedas a
in networkswere due to opportunitiesand con- social constructionrooted in hierarchy,not in
straintsarising out of women's and men's dif- biology or in internalization. . ."(1988, p. 15).
ferent locations in the social structure.Men's
and women's networks differed more as a re- GWEN MOORE, Assistant Professor of Sociology at
sult of thatfact and less because they were pre- SUNY-Albany,is currentlyfocusingher research on
disposed to form and maintain differing net- the intersections of gender, networks, and power.
works.Blau's work is importantin understand- This research includes the study of cross-national
variation in the relationshipof gender and author-
ing that networks form within an opportunity
ity, and an analysis of gender and managerial net-
context that facilitates or impedes the forma- works in the New YorkState government."EliteIn-
tion of social ties (1977; see also Marsdenforth- tegration in Stable Democracies: A Reconsidera-
coming). The social structureshapes opportu- tion" (forthcoming,EuropeanSociological Review),
nitiesto formrelationshipsof varioussorts(Blau an article written with John Higley, Ursula
1977). If women do not work for pay, they Hoffmann-Lange,and Charles Kadushin, summa-
cannot have coworkers in their personal net- rizes more than a decade of research on the struc-
works. Unmarriedpersons cannot form ties to ture of elite networks.WithJ. Allen Whitt,Moore is
spouses or in-laws. These structuralfactors co-editingThe PoliticalConsequencesof Social Net-
provide the context within which personalties works.
form.
The structuralperspective has implications REFERENCES
beyond the realm of the personalnetworksin-
vestigated here. Kanter (1977) used it to ex- Aukett,Richard,JaneRitchieandKathrynMill. 1988.
"GenderDifferences in FriendshipPatterns."Sex
plain gender differences in networks and be-
Roles 19:57-66.
havior in organizations.She contended that it Berk,SarahFenstermaker.1988. "Women'sUnpaid
is not the dispositions of men and women, but Labor: Home and Community."Pp. 287-302 in
the small numberof women in high-level posi- WomenWorking:Theoriesand Facts in Perspec-
tions and theirrelativelack of opportunityand tive, 2nd. edition,editedby Ann HeltonStromberg
power in male-dominated organizations that and Shirley Harkess.MountainView, CA: May-
explain the higherperformanceandmobility of field.
men in workorganizations.Largernumbersand Blau, Peter M. 1977. Inequalityand Heterogeneity:
proportionsof women in high-level organiza- A PrimitiveTheoryof Social Structure.New York:
tional positions would decrease women's dis- Free Press.
Brass, Daniel J. 1985. "Men's and Women's Net-
advantage.Kanter'swork on genderin organi-
works:A Study of InteractionPatternsand Influ-
zations has pointed to the power of structural ence in an Organization."Academy of Manage-
effects on behaviorthatwas previouslyascribed mentJournal 28:327-43.
to individualpredispositions. Burt, Ronald S. 1983. "Range."Pp. 176-94 in Ap-
This examinationof strongnetworkties con- plied NetworkAnalysis:A MethodologicalIntro-
firms previous findings of gender differences duction,by Ronald S. Burt,Michael J. Minorand
in networks while also illuminatingtheir ori- Associates. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
gins. These resultssupportBlau's andKanter's ._ _ 1985. "General Social Survey Network
conclusions:If men and women were in similar Items."Connections.8:119-23.
social structuralpositions theirbehaviorwould Caldwell, MaytaA. and Letitia Anne Peplau. 1982.
differ little. As more women move into paid "Sex Differences in Same-Sex Friendship."Sex
Roles 8:721-32.
employment, the genders' network composi- Campbell, Karen E. 1988. "GenderDifferences in
tions can be expected to become more alike, Job-RelatedNetworks." Workand Occupations
with more close ties to nonkin, especially co- 15:179-200.
workers, and fewer ties to kin. Women may Campbell, Karen E., Peter V. Marsdenand Jeanne
still maintaina largernumberof ties to kin than S. Hurlbert.1986. "Social Resources and Socio-
DETERMINANTSOF MEN'S AND WOMEN'S NETWORKS 735

economic Status."Social Networks8:97-117. the Family Life Span."Journal of Marriage and


Chodorow,Nancy. 1978. TheReproductionof Moth- the Family 44:197-208.
ering. Berkeley: University of California. Lin, Nan. 1982. "Social Resourcesand Instrumental
Davis, James A. and Tom W. Smith. 1988. General Action." Pp. 131-46 in Social Structureand Net-
Social Surveys, 1972-1988 [MRDF]. Principal work Analysis, edited by Peter V. Marsden and
Investigator,James A. Davis; Director and Co- Nan Lin. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
PrincipalInvestigator,Tom W. Smith. NORC ed. Lin, Nan, WalterM. Ensel, and J. C. Vaughn. 1981.
Chicago:NationalOpinionResearchCenter[pro- "Social Resources and Strength of Ties: Struc-
ducer]. Storrs, CT: The Roper Center for Public turalFactorsin OccupationalStatusAttainment."
OpinionResearch,Universityof Connecticut[dis- AmericanSociological Review 46:393-405.
tributor].1 datafile (23,356 logical records)and1 Lipset, Seymour Martin, Martin Trow and James
codebook (790 pp.). Coleman. 1956. Union Democracy. New York:
di Leonardo, Michaela. 1987. "The Female World Doubleday.
of Cardsand Holidays:Women, Families and the Marsden,PeterV. 1987. "CoreDiscussion Networks
Work of Kinship."Signs: Journal of Women in of Americans." American Sociological Review
Cultureand Society 12:440-58. 52:122-31.
Epstein, Cynthia Fuchs. 1988. Deceptive Distinc- . Forthcoming. "Network Diversity, Sub-
tions: Sex, Gender and the Social Order. New structures, and Opportunities for Contact." in
Haven:Yale University. Structures of Power and Constraint: Essays in
Fischer, Claude. 1982. To Dwell Among Friends. Honor of Peter M. Blau, editedby CraigCalhoun,
Chicago:University of Chicago MarshallW. Meyer and W. Richard Scott. New
Fischer, Claude, and Stacey Oliker. 1983. "A Re- York: Cambridge.
search Note on Friendship,Gender, and the Life Marsden, Peter V., and Jeanne S. Hurlbert. 1987.
Cycle." Social Forces 62:124-132. "SmallNetworksandSelectivityBias in theAnaly-
Gerstel,Naomi. 1988. "Divorce, Genderand Social sis of Survey Network Data." Social Networks
Integration."Genderand Society 2:343-67. 9:333-49.
Gilligan, Carol. 1982. In a Different Voice: Psycho- Marsden,PeterV., andNan Lin. 1982. Social Struc-
logical Theoryand Women'sDevelopment.Cam- ture and Network Analysis. Beverly Hills, CA:
bridge, MA: Harvard. Sage.
Granovetter,MarkS. 1974. Getting a Job: A Study McPherson,J. Miller and Lynn Smith-Lovin. 1982.
of Contacts and Careers. Cambridge,MA: Har- "Women and Weak Ties: Differences by Sex in
vard. the Size of VoluntaryOrganizations."American
. 1982. "The Strength of Weak Ties: A Journal of Sociology 87:883-904.
NetworkTheory Revisited."Pp. 105-30 in Social . 1986. "Sex Segregation in Voluntary
Structuresand NetworkAnalysis, edited by Peter Associations."American Sociological Review
V. MarsdenandNan Lin. BeverlyHills, CA: Sage. 51:61-79.
Hurlbert,Jeanne S.and Alan C.-Acock. 1990. "The Miller, Jean Baker. 1976. Toward a New Psychol-
Effects of MaritalStatuson the FormandCompo- ogy of Women.Boston: Beacon.
sition of Social Networks."Social Science Quar- Miller, Jon. 1986. Pathways in the Workplace.
terly 71:163-74. Cambridge:Cambridge.
Kadushin,Charles.1982. "Social Density and Men- Moore, Gwen. 1979. "The Structureof a National
tal Health." Pp. 147-58 in Social Structureand Elite Network." American Sociological Review
NetworkAnalysis,editedby PeterV. Marsdenand 44:672-92.
Nan Lin. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Rosenthal, Carolyn J. 1985. "Kinkeeping in the
Kanter,Rosabeth Moss. 1977. Men and Womenof FamilialDivision of Labor."Journal of Marriage
the Corporation.New York: Basic.
and the Family 47:965-74.
Laumann, Edward 0. and Franz U. Pappi. 1976.
Susan, Russell Ward and Mark LaGory.
Networksof Collective Action: A Perspective on Sherman,
CommunityInfluence Systems. New York: Aca- 1988. "Women as Caregiversof the Elderly: In-
demic. strumentaland ExpressiveSupport."Social Work
Laumann,Edward0. and David Knoke. 1987. The 33:164-67.
OrganizationalState: Social Choice in National Spitze, Glennaand JohnLogan. 1989. "GenderDif-
Policy Domains. Madison, WI: University of ferences in Family Support:Is There a Payoff?"
Wisconsin. The Gerontologist29:108-13.
Lee, Gary R. 1980. "Kinship in the Seventies: A Wellman,Barry.1985. "DomesticWork,PaidWork
Decade Review of Research and Theory."Jour- and Net Work." Pp. 159-91 in Understanding
nal of Marriage and the Family 42:923-34. Personal Relationships,editedby Steve Duck and
Leigh, Geoffrey K. 1982. "KinshipInteractionover Daniel Perlman.London:Sage.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai