Anda di halaman 1dari 15

Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Tourism Management
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tourman

The influence of personal values in the economic-use valuation of peri-urban


green spaces: An application of the means-end chain theory
Natalia López-Mosquera, Mercedes Sánchez*
Universidad Pública de Navarra, Dpto. Gestión de Empresas. Edificio Madroños, 31006 Pamplona, Spain

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The implication of land-use managers and the local community in matters relating to peri-urban green
Received 12 February 2010 spaces has been modified by the growing importance of the values attached to such areas. This paper
Accepted 3 August 2010 uses means-end chain techniques to examine whether green space users reflect their own personal
values through the benefits and attributes they perceive in this type of good. The results show key factors
Keywords: to be opportunities for sport and recreational activities, improvement of physical and mental well-being
Peri-urban green space
and enjoyment of landscape beauty. Other values, both individual (personal enjoyment and quality of
Contingent valuation
life) and social (respect for others and a clearer conscience) also emerge. Further analysis to determine
Means-end chain theory
Personal values
whether values differ according to whether or not the visitor assigns a monetary value to the green space
Territorial management showed higher environmental and social awareness to be associated with higher willingness to pay for
Abstraction level peri-urban green space. Thus, the greater the perceived environmental values and the higher their
subsequent monetary valuation, the more effective environmental protection and conservation policies
are likely to be. These results may be worth consideration by land-use managers engaged in environ-
mental cost benefit analysis
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction growing importance attached to the personal values expressed


through green space use (Brown, 2005; Brown & Raymond, 2007)
Most of the existing research on green spaces takes the form of and to the phenomenon of place attachment or people’s emotional
visitor perception studies focusing on the use and valuation of this and cognitive with their physical environment (Williams & Vaske,
type of environmental goods (Asayu-Adjaye & Tapsuwan, 2008; 2003). Several studies have already shown that the evaluation of
Baral, Stern, & Bhattarai, 2008; Jim & Chen, 2006) or the physical a natural space usually reveals a person’s environmental value
and mental welfare benefits they provide (Kaplan, 1995; Korpela, orientation (Coogan, Karash, Adler, & Sallis, 2007; Kaltenborn &
Ylén, Tyrväinen, & Silvennoinen, 2008; Tyrväinen, Mäikinen, & Bjerke, 2002; Stern, Dietz, Abel, Guagnano, & Kalof, 1999, among
Schipperijn, 2007). There has also been a recent upsurge of others). In exploring this last concept, researchers have focused on
studies attempting to explain the variation in how people use and human attitudes and behaviour towards natural landscapes, based
perceive the attractiveness of green spaces (Krenichyn, 2006). The on the theory that they derive from underlying personal values,
literature defines landscape perception as a complex process regarded by some as the simple principles that guide evaluations or
involving both the more technical aspects of vision and psycho- cognitive inferences (Rokeach, 1979; González & Amérigo, 2008).
logical factors relating to cognition, affect and evaluation. All these Given their role in determining people’s attitudes and responses
aspects, which are highly interrelated in visitors’ minds, determine towards specific aspects of the environment (Kaltenborn & Bjerke,
their preferences for specific landscapes (Sevenant & Antrop, 2009). 2002) personal values need to be integrated into the analysis of
Over the last decade, the consideration of cognitive and affective decision-making processes (Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001).
issues in user decision-making processes has led to a substantial The above context of analysis, which examines the individual
change in the conception and valuation of green spaces (Chiesura, decision-making process in terms of the relationship between
2004; Manzo, 2003; Sanesi & Chiarello, 2006; Velarde, Fry, & motivations and values and behaviour, provides the framework for
Tveit, 2007). This change has come about largely as a result of the this study, which has two features that distinguish it from previous
research. The first is that it applies means-end chain methodology
in a cognitive model, to determine whether the differentiating
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ34 948 169396; fax: þ34 948169404. attributes of a given environmental good provide users with
E-mail address: mersan@unavarra.es (M. Sánchez). benefits and reasons for its use and enjoyment that later lead to the

0261-5177/$ e see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2010.08.003
876 N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

fulfilment of personal end values. The environmental good in obtained from the use of green space as a means to fulfil personal
question is a peri-urban green space,1 the Monte San Pedro park values (Krenichyn, 2006).
(A Coruña, Spain), selected for its accessibility and possibilities for Broadly speaking, contact with the outdoors provides two types
recreational enjoyment. The second is that it aims to test for of benefits; some directly related with the environmental good itself,
variations in individual decision-making structures as revealed by and others relating to its effect on users. The reported environmental
behavioural observation, with particular emphasis on contingent benefits include aesthetic appeal, cleaner air, observation of nature,
valuation estimates of WTP. landscape beauty and wilderness, while the benefits for users are
The final understanding of the personal or intrinsic values that predominantly of a psychological nature (Gobster & Westphal,
drive the green space decision-making processes may assist 2004; Kalisch & Klaphake, 2007; Tyrväinen et al., 2007; Vesely,
managers, primarily, to establish and justify their global aims and 2007). Another perspective describes contact with nature as
ecosystem management strategies (Bengston, 1994; Brown & contributing to improve the sensory capacities, reduce fatigue and
Raymond, 2007; Kyle, Mowen, & Terrant, 2004). It might also stress, restore emotional balance, and sharpen cognitive skills
contribute to the anticipation, identification and fulfilment of the (Kaplan, 1995; Velarde et al., 2007; Korpela et al., 2008). The
needs and desires of visitors and to defining value orientations and resulting health benefits and outdoor physical activity are also
potential responses to landscapes. Thirdly, it could potentially reported to have a positive effect on physical and mental well-being,
improve the prediction of public reactions to environmental prac- and a direct relationship has been established between exposure to
tices in the face of future changes in ecosystem management and the natural environment and positive emotional responses
provide new criteria for dealing with management-related conflict. (Hansmann, Hug, & Seeland, 2007; Maas & Verheij, 2007). Finally,
Finally, the analysis of landscape preferences, in terms of green space focusing on the ways in which natural spaces stimulate social inte-
users’ attitudes and environment-related economic behaviour, may gration and personal interaction, one strand of research includes
provide a useful tool for the design of environmental education a social component among the personal benefits (Barbosa et al.,
programmes (Múgica & De Lucio, 1996; Pooley & O’Connor, 2000). 2007; Burgess, Harrisson, & Limb, 1998; Coley, Kuo, & Sullivan,
Additionally, citizen participation in environmental education pro- 1997; Tyrväinen et al., 2007).
grammes helps to stimulate certain perceptive patterns and As well as identifying the beneficial effects derived from the use
promote a positive evaluation of the landscape. All of the above- of environmental goods, investigators have tried to determine to
mentioned actions could potentially bring urban management what extent these effects are subject to personal and behavioural
objectives into line with users’ needs. factors. Thus, the visitor’s decision-making process varies, not only
The remainder of the paper is organised in five parts. Part 2 with the good considered, but also with the user’s personal interests
presents a review of the literature on personal values related to the and life experiences, and with the physical and social setting
environment. Part 3 describes the methodological details and the (Neuvomen, Sievänen, Tönnes, & Koskela, 2007). Life experiences, in
environmental good to be tested. Part 4 presents the main findings. particular, are a major focus of research interest. Two of the
The discussion and the main conclusions and limitations of the study perceptions most widely expressed by visitors to green spaces are of
together with suggestions of lines for future research make up Part 5. freedom and silence (Klijn, Buij, Dijkstra, Luttik, & Veeneklaas,
2000). Other authors mention tranquillity, comfort, emotional
stimulation, security, anxiety, and boredom (Galindo & Corraliza,
2. Personal values related to the environment
2000). Fun, pleasure, tranquillity; the desire for new experiences;
emotional stimulation; health improvement and solitude have also
The growing importance assigned to urban and peri-urban
been documented (Davenport, Borrie, Freimund, & Manning, 2002).
green spaces has altered both how they are conceived and how they
The reported emotional benefits include experience of nature and
are valued. Thus, attention has been drawn to the aesthetic, psycho-
escape from the stressful pace of daily life, and the evidence
physical and social health-related benefits provided by these spaces
suggests that experience of nature is a source of positive feelings,
(Chiesura, 2004), the importance of the social factor in their
such as freedom, connectedness with nature and happiness
valuation has been examined (Sanesi & Chiarello, 2006), and
(Chiesura, 2004). Contact with nature as a means to attain
emotional, health-benefit and landscape factors have been taken
emotional and sensory experiences such as visual beauty and
into consideration (Velarde et al., 2007). This change of focus has
physical and mental well-being has also been mentioned
come about as the result of interest in identifying and quantifying
(Krenichyn, 2006). Recent studies further emphasise the impor-
individuals’ landscape values and environmental values in general,
tance of these personal experiences, describing them as the root of
and in delimiting the nature of users’ personal attachment to them.
various perceptions, behavioural responses and practices among
One important line of investigation has examined the link between
green space users (Tyrväinen et al., 2007; Hanley et al., 2009). These
users’ emotional responses and perceptions of environmental
personal experiences depend on the personal beliefs and values that
goods with a view to gaining a deeper understanding of the
determine people’s aesthetic appreciation and social values. Some
valuation mechanism (Manzo, 2003; Williams, Davids, Burwitz, &
researchers perceive values as the objectives or goals that motivate
Williams, 1992). Subsequent studies have identified people as
or drive people’s actions, with responses varying according to how
active participants in the landscape, established a distinction
individuals draw on their personal experience (Ford, Williams,
between functional and symbolic values, demonstrated the role of
Bishop, & Webb, 2009).
culture, and linked place attachment to landscape values (Brown,
The case for the importance of personal values draws support
2005; Brown & Raymond, 2007). Thus, the research has extended
from the fact that certain studies have found them to have more
to the analysis of users’ reasons for using environmental goods and
explanatory power than context for explaining people’s environ-
the identification of the associated benefits. Another recent
mental valuation and behaviour (González, 2002). Elsewhere, the
contribution to the research is an interesting study of the benefits
evidence suggests values as the main driver of environmental
behaviour (Grob, 1995). A recent strand of research highlights the
1
role of personal values of this type in human decision-making
Taking peri-urban green space to mean any area of outstanding landscape
interest, natural or artificial, located within a metropolitan boundary, where
related to the environment. Thus, the range of existing research
overlapping natural, semi-natural, transition and artificial areas are adapted to includes the distinction between internal values and external
meet the recreational needs of the local community. values (Chryssohoidis & Krystallis, 2005), the influence of these
N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889 877

values on behaviour (Stern et al., 1999; Coogan et al., 2007) and the perception of a good is the result of associations between its attri-
impact of personal values on willingness for environmental butes (the “means”) and more abstract cognitive schemata, which
conservation (Winter, Prozesky, & Esler, 2007). include the personal values underlying certain behaviour (the
The recent incorporation of these behavioural differences into “ends”). Such associations determine the appeal of the characteris-
various cognitive decision models has improved model perfor- tics of the good in question (Reynolds & Gutman, 1988).
mance (Ford et al., 2009). In other words, the inclusion of personal According to this theory, consumers’ product knowledge is
values enables more accurate prediction of public attitudes towards organised into hierarchical levels of abstraction, where the higher
the environment (Bodur & Sarogöllü, 2005; González, 2002; the level of abstraction, the stronger and more direct the connection
Kotchen & Reiling, 2000; Kyle et al., 2004). From the economic to the self. Six ascending levels of abstraction describe the cognitive
perspective, however, the main interest of this study lies in the use structure linking product knowledge (concrete attributes, abstract
of explanatory models incorporating monetary valuations of the attributes and functional consequences) with self-knowledge
environmental good in order to explain the interrelation between (psychological consequences, instrumental values and end values)
behavioural traits, willingness to pay (WTP) and personal values in (Olson & Reynolds,1983). In the case in hand, the attributes are those
green space users. This kind of approach led to the development of properties or characteristics of the environmental good, service or
a model using values, perceptions and motivations for the use or performance that visitors may desire or pursue. The abstract attri-
non-use of the environmental good as the explanatory variables for butes are those whose verification is impossible to verify prior to use
the valuation process (Harris & Brown, 1992). Subsequent research except through internal or external information sources. Explora-
investigated the relationship between attitudinal and motivational tion of the knowledge structure in the environmental context clearly
factors and contingent valuation estimates of environmental non- established the distinction between concrete and abstract attributes
use values (Kotchen & Reiling, 2000). The findings indicate an (Purcell, 1992). Functional consequences are the benefits obtained
association between higher WTP and more positive attitudes directly from the use of an environmental good. Psychosocial
toward the environment. Some research reports that WTP for consequences are of a more personal, social and less tangible nature.
environmental protection is value-oriented (Stern & Dietz, 1994). In Instrumental values are intangible goals related with the beha-
the same vein, some authors have urged a renewal of interest in the vioural means used to achieve the end aims and, finally, terminal
individual motives underlying WTP (Cooper, Poe, & Bateman, 2004). values refer to desired end states (Miele & Parisi, 2000).
This has generated research examining the role of motives in Existing studies using this methodological approach in a similar
explaining WTP values for a set of nested environmental goods with environmental context include an investigation of Dutch people’s
potential use and non-use benefits, the findings revealing greater motivation to recycle under a compulsory recycling scheme
complexity in the motivational structure when the analysis (Smeesters et al., 2003), which showed that they were driven by
considers both social and personal motivational factors. Analysis of environmental and “civic duty”-related values. Prior research
the preferences for multiple use forest management and average attempting to identify recycling goals and their effect on the deci-
social and individual WTP revealed the relevance of personal factors sion to recycle had reached similar conclusions (Bagozzi &
(Mill, Van Rensburg, Hynes, & Dooley, 2007). Yet another strand of Dabholkar, 1994). Exploration of social structure and social rela-
work, relating attitudes towards pollution with WTP for wastewater tions to determine the extent and quality of the environmental
treatment and recycling, reports positive correlation between the impacts deriving from economic activities revealed the deter-
two (Tziakis et al., 2009). Investigation of citizens’ perceptions and mining effect of human skills and human capital on time structure
WTP for a market-based instrument and analysis of the influence of dynamics (Cogoy, 1999). This paper aims to apply means-end chain
social factors on individuals’ views have revealed social capital, methodology to the study of peri-urban green spaces in order to
perceptions of the compliance of fellow citizens and the effective- reveal the cognitive structure of the decision-making process
ness of the proposed policy as significant determinants of WTP underlying the use and evaluation of the environmental good.
(Jones, Evangelinos, Halvadakis, Iosifides, & Sophoulis, 2010).
In the context of analysis presented above, this paper uses 3.1.1. The laddering interview
means-end methodology to reveal the personal values underlying With respect to the choice of data collection method for the MEC
the decision-making processes of visitors to the San Pedro peri- application, the most widely known information-gathering tech-
urban park based on its attributes and related benefits. It also nique is one known as “laddering”, which was first developed by
assesses whether the cognitive structure varies according to Hinkle (1965). Based on the personal construct theory (Kelly, 1955),
contingent valuation estimates of monetary values for the good it is a face-to-face, one-on-one, in-depth, semi-structured inter-
considered. The next section describes the research methodology. viewing technique designed to develop an understanding of how
consumers translate product attributes into meaningful associa-
3. Methodological issues tions with respect to themselves (Bourne & Jenkins, 2005; Gutman,
1982). In other words, its purpose is to reveal people’s motives for
3.1. Means-end chain theory choosing a particular good (Russell et al., 2004). The laddering
interview is a three-stage process: the elicitation of key attributes,
Information concerning the relationship between the decision- the in-depth interview, and the analysis of results. In the first stage,
making process and cognitive structure in green space users is of respondents are required to name the main attributes on which
potential interest to environmental economists. Research on this they focus when comparing and evaluating goods. The revealed key
topic has largely focused on the most concrete level of visitors’ deci- attributes are the starting point for the second stage, which is an in-
sion-making, that is, the attributes that the green space has to offer. depth interview, where respondents are required to explain their
The intricacies of the cognitive structure, however, are such that relevance in terms of the perceived associated consequences and
perceptions and valuations of the attributes of the good often result in personal values. Interviewers repeatedly ask respondents “Why is
complex choice structures. Means-end chain (MEC) analysis involving that important to you?” pushing them to increasing levels of
personal values has revealed the multi-level nature of decision- abstraction (from attributes to consequences and from there to
makers’ cognitive choice structures (Pitts, Wong, & Whalen, 1991). values) until they can go no further. This results in sequences of
The means-end chain theory (Gutman, 1982; Howard, 1977; concepts or “ladders”. The objective in the third stage is to plot the
Young & Feigin, 1975) assumes that the decision-maker’s subjective concepts drawn out by the laddering technique on a so-called
878 N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

Table 1
Identification classification of the attributes, consequences and values used in this study.

Attributes Consequences Values


Concrete - Price (A1) Functional - I have good ecological habits (C1) Instrumental - A source of fun, pleasure
Attributes - A place to practice sport and Consequences - I help the environment (C5) Values and enjoyment (V2)
take physical exercise (A3) - A way to escape from - Enhances my quality of life
- Distance from home (A4) routine/a way to switch off (C6) and security (V4)
- Recreational facilities - Frequency of use /regular - Emotional stimulation (V6)
provided (A7) user of green space (C7) - I’m more successful (V9)
Abstract - Landscape beauty (A2) Psychological - Stress relief /relaxation (C2) End - A sense of social belonging (V1)
Attributes - Closeness to nature (A5) Consequences - Physical well-being /improved Values - Better relationships with others (V3)
- Benefits to the health (A6) fitness (C3) - A sense of self-fulfilment
- Less noise (A8) - Rest (C4) and accomplishment (V5)
- Better health /Mental - Being respected by others (V7)
well-being (C8) - Peace of mind dignity and self-respect (V8)

implication matrix (Chiu, 2005; Miele & Parisi, 2000; Ter Hofstede, Another methodological issue requiring consideration is how
Audenaert, Steenkamp, & Wedel, 1998). This matrix enables the many links to include on the HVM in order to obtain the most
construction of a hierarchical value map (HVM), which is a tree- meaningful results, or “cut-off level”. This indicates the number of
diagram mapping the respondent’s thought process through the linkages registered before a connection ceases on the map (Leppard,
various levels of abstraction (Reynolds & Gutman, 1988). Russel, & Cox, 2004). It is not easy to determine what frequency of
A literature review and a pilot survey carried out on a group of linkages between two levels of abstraction is meaningful or signi-
experts guided the choice of attributes, consequences and values ficant enough to be included on the HVM. A high cut-off level (a high
considered in the survey designed to reveal green space visitors’ frequency of linkages) will give a simpler map, involving fewer
cognitive structures. It comprised eight attributes representing the connections, hence some loss of relevant information, but greater
(concrete and abstract) characteristics of a peri-urban green space ease of interpretation. A low cut-off level (a lower frequency of
and eight (functional and psychological) consequences relating to linkages) will result in a complex map that will contain a large
its use (Table 1). The concrete attributes were drawn from findings amount of information but will be more difficult to interpret. Prior
in the literature indicating that users’ perception and overall research has shown various ways of determining the cut-off point
valuation of green spaces are influenced by the admission fee, (Pieters, Baumgartner, & Allen, 1995), but the consensus is that
distance from home, and the exercise and recreational facilities on a good cut-off point is one that selects the solution combining the
offer (Lee & Han, 2002; Tyrväinen & Väänänen, 1998; among maximum amount of information with the greatest ease of inter-
others). Research showing the influential factors to be landscape pretation (Audenaert & Steenkamp, 1997 in Leppard et al., 2004).
beauty, closeness with nature and green space benefits informed The cut-off determination method used in this study, known
the choice of abstract attributes (Gobster & Westphal, 2004; as “top-down ranking” (Russell et al., 2004; Leppard et al., 2004) is
Krenichyn, 2006). The main functional benefits reportedly based on the premise that a whole group of respondents will not
perceived by green space visitors were the escape from routine, necessarily make the same number of linkages between two levels
frequency of use and increased environmental awareness of abstraction. It may therefore be inappropriate always to use the
(Bernarth & Roschewitz, 2008; Chiesura, 2004; Togridou, Hovardad, same cut-off point when the number of linkages between
& Pantis, 2006) and the most highly valued abstract benefits appear the different levels of abstraction varies (Barrena & Sánchez, 2009).
to be the physical and mental health improvements (Gidlöf- Top-down ranking enables the HVM to include only the most
Gunnarsson & Öhrström, 2007; Kaplan, 1995). The values for the frequent linkages between different levels of abstraction. In other
analysis presented in this paper were adapted from the LOV (list of words, it selects the linkages in order of importance (the most
values) proposed by Kalhe (1985), later modified by the Rokeach important linkage being the one with the most cell entries). This
Value Survey (RVS), which identifies nine key personal values provides an ordered set of HVMs, where the first map is the simplest
that influence people’s lives (Chryssohoidis & Krystallis, 2005; Lee, and easiest to interpret while showing the most important linkages.
Soutar, & Louviere, 2007). The scale has been widely used to study The advantage of this method is that it allows observation of the
the impact of personal values. linkages between levels and permits between-group comparison
The two possible approaches when conducting laddering (Russell et al., 2004; Leppard et al., 2004). The software package
interviews are soft laddering and hard laddering2 (Costa, Dekker, & used in the data analysis was MecAnalyst Plus 1.0.
Jongen, 2004; Grunert & Grunert, 1995). Hard laddering was
selected for the purposes of this study because it is quicker and 3.2. Contingent valuation method
cheaper than soft laddering, places less pressure on the respondent
(Grunert & Grunert, 1995) and is more suitable when working with Using the methodology described above, this paper analyses
large samples (more than 50 subjects) (Russell et al., 2004). The whether cognitive structure influences users’ monetary valuation
technique used in the case in hand is the Association Pattern of an environmental good. Contingent valuation (CV) provides the
Technique (APT) (Gutman, 1982; Ter Hofstede et al., 1998), which WTP estimates. The high flexibility of this method suits a broad
uses two independent matrices: an AC matrix (attributes/conse- range of public goods and situations, hence its widespread use in
quences) and a CV matrix (consequences/values). the valuation of environmental goods. It has the advantage of
providing a hypothetical and direct method of estimating the
monetary value of environmental resources based on the results of
2
Hard laddering refers to all interview and data collection techniques in which public surveys (Mitchell & Carson, 1989). This method overcomes
subjects are compelled to generate or verify associations between elements within the problem that environmental goods cannot be bought or sold in
individual ladders, in sequences that reflect increasing levels of abstraction. In soft
laddering, a natural and unrestricted flow of speech is encouraged during inter-
the marketplace, by presenting consumers with hypothetical
views, with associations between ACV being reconstructed subsequently during the markets, in which they are invited to declare their maximum (WTP)
analysis (Costa et al., 2004). for the environmental good in question or their willing-to-accept
N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889 879

compensation (WTA) for a hypothetical loss (Venkatachalam, contingent valuation studies of natural resources (Asafu-Adjaye
2004). Maximum WTP and minimum WTA estimates are possible and Tapsuwan, 2008; Chen & Jim, 2008; Del Saz & García, 2007).
using the economic concepts of compensating variation and The equation takes the following form:
equivalent variation (Venkatachalam, 2004). If the adopted policy
generates an increase in users’ well-being, the compensation ProbabilityðWTPÞ ¼ a þ b1 bid amount þ b2 age þ b3 gender
variation (WTP) is the amount of personal income that has to be þ b4 education þ b5 income þ b6 frequency
given up by the visitor to attain an increased level of utility þ error ð1Þ
(Venkatachalam, 2004), as in the case considered in this paper.
Equivalent variation (WTA) represents the amount of compensa- where a is the constant and bi are the coefficients of the explana-
tion offered to the user for a utility-level improvement in the event tory variables.
that the provision of the public good does not take place The dichotomous choice format was used to elicit WTP by
(Venkatachalam, 2004). asking respondents whether they would be willing to pay a given
Three different survey modes are available: the face-to-face bid amount A. Visitors were assumed to be utility maximizing
interview, the telephone and e-mail (Del Saz, García, & Palau, 1999), when expressing their WTP the specified bid amount in exchange
the choice depends on the characteristics of the particular for the recreational use of the peri-urban green space. Following
CVM study, and the advantages and drawbacks of each method Hanemann (1984), this paper adopts the assumption that the
(Mitchell & Carson, 1989). Another issue for consideration is the probability of a respondent being willing to pay a given bid amount
choice of elicitation format. There are five different formats for follows a standard logistic variate, if:
eliciting subjects’ WTP for a given public good. The most commonly
used are the dichotomous choice (DC), the open-ended and the vð1; Y  A; sÞ þ 31  vð0; Y; sÞ þ 30 (2)
mixed formats (the last being a combination of the first two). The and rejects it otherwise. Here, v is the indirect utility which is
other two are the payment card format and the bidding game.3 assumed to equal the utility u; Y is income, A is the admission fee, s
Note also that, despite the advantages provided by the CVM,4 it is other socio-economic characteristics affecting individual prefe-
has often been criticised for its potential biases (Carson, Flores, & rence, and 30 and 31 are the identically, independently distributed
Meade, 2001; Sudgen, 2005), which can arise for a variety of random variables with zero means.
reasons. (1) The hypothetical nature of the simulated market can The utility difference (Dv) between the “yes” and “no” answers
trigger strategic behaviour (free riding) by respondents. (2) takes the form:
The “embedding” effect, when WTP for goods and services does not
vary according to the context; (3) The “sequence” effect, where WTP Dv ¼ vð1; Y  A; sÞ  vð0; Y; sÞ þ ð31 þ 30 Þ (3)
varies according to position of the good in the valuation sequence;
(4) The “information” effect, where WTP is influenced by the type of The DC format of CVM has a binary choice dependent variable,
information supplied across different valuation scenarios; (5) The which requires a qualitative choice model. This paper uses a logit
elicitation effect, where WTP is influenced by the elicitation format; model. The probability (P1) that the individual will accept an offer
(6) The hypothetical bias, where the hypothetical market presented (A) takes the form of the following logit model (Hanemann, 1989):
differs considerably from the real market; and, finally, (7) Protest
1 1
zeros, which are motivated by protest behaviour triggered by some Pi ¼ Fn ðDvÞ ¼ (4)
1 þ expð  DvÞ 1 þ expf ða  bA þ gY þ qSÞg
component of the survey design, such as the payment vehicle, or
ethical objections to personal payment for a public good where Fh(/) is the cumulative distribution function of a standard
(Venkatachalam, 2004). Utmost care is necessary to avoid estima- logistic variate and some of the socio-economic variables included
tion problems resulting from biases in order to maintain the relia- in this study. b, g, and q are coefficients to be estimated
bility of the results and the usefulness of this method for the where b  0; g > 0; and 0 < q or q > 0 are expected.
monetary valuation of environmental resources (Arrow et al., 1993). WTP values are the truncated means. The logit model in Eq. (4)
A large number of CVM studies in the context of green space is estimated by the maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method,
management and planning consider both the recreational use value after which, the expected WTP value of can be calculated by
of various environmental goods (Bernarth & Roschewitz, 2008; Del numerical integration, ranging from zero to Maximum Bid (A) as
Saz & García, 2007; Jim & Chen, 2006; Sayadi, González-Roa, & follows:
Calatrava-Requena, 2009) and the non-use value of the conserva-
tion of an area for future use (Baral et al., 2008; Chen & Jim, 2008; Z
Max:A Z
Max:A
 
Pedroso, Freitas, & Domingos, 2007; Zoppi, 2007). EðWTPÞ ¼ F pðDvÞ dA ¼ a* þ bA dA; (5)
0 0
3.2.1. Logit regression and WTP econometric model
This paper uses logit regression to model the relationship of the where E(WTP) is the expected value of WTP, and a* is the adjusted
binary dependent variable (WTP) to the independent variables intercept added by the socio-economic term to the original inter-
based on econometric models presented by Lee & Han (2002), cept term of a. The area beneath the curve in Eq. (5) provides the
Asafu-Adjaye and Tapsuwan (2008), and Baral et al. (2008). Most of truncated mean WTP values. Finally, the chi-square test of inde-
the variables tested have shown significant predictability in other pendence provides an assessment of the associations between
categorical variables.

3
For a more detailed description of interview modes and elicitation formats, see 3.3. Questionnaire design
Mitchell and Carson (1989), Del Saz et al. (1999) and Carson (2000).
4
The main advantages are: (1) It is the only method available when it is The application of CV and MEC theory requires the use of
impossible to establish a link between the quality of the environmental good and a structured survey questionnaire. This study uses a four-part
the consumption of a private good; (2) Its flexibility makes it suitable for addressing
all kinds of public goods and situations; (3) Ex-ante valuation; (4) the Hicksian
questionnaire.
consumer surplus measure can be obtained directly; (5) It allows for the estimation Part 1 collects data on visitors’ real and potential use of the
of non-use values (Carson, 2000; Del Saz & Suárez, 1998). green space and the level of satisfaction experienced during the
880 N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

visit. Part 2 elicits WTP for the recreational use of a peri-urban helped to reduce potential sampling error and increase the likelihood
green space. The scenario5 presented to respondents prior to the CV of generating a representative sample of the A Coruña population.
questions was formulated to be as realistic as possible and remind This methodology section concludes with a brief description of
them of the main assets of the area (Buckley, van Rensburg, & the survey area.
Hynes, 2009; Kotchen & Reiling, 2000; Pedroso et al., 2007). The
purpose of this choice of scenario was to improve the credibility of 3.4. The peri-urban green space selected for the study: Monte de
the proposal and minimise the risk of misinterpretation that is San Pedro Natural Park (Coruña e Spain)
inherent in this valuation method. The standard DC format in
combination with open-ended questions was used to elicit WTP The Monte de San Pedro Natural Park, opened on June 6, 1999, is
(Bateman, Langford, & Rasbash, 1999; Riera, 1994; Zoppi, 2007); the located in the north-west of the Iberian peninsular (Spain). It is
inclusion of a control question helped to identify the reasons for a large, topographically varied area measuring 7.84 ha, offering
possible unwillingness to pay (Jorgensen, Wilson, & Heberlein, vistas of the city of A Coruña (Galicia) and a wide strip of coastline
2001; León, 1996; Tyrväinen & Väänänen, 1998). The three bid stretching from Cape San Adrián and the Sisargas Islands, which lie
prices selected were 1.5V, 2.5V or 3.5V6, and, to ensure indepen- to the west, to Cape Prior and Cape Prioriño to the east. The
dence, they were uniformly and randomly distributed across seashore is of particular interest due to its characteristic rock
visitors (Scarpa, Hutchinson, Chilton, & Buongiorno, 2000). The use formations, and the flora and fauna8 dispersed throughout the park
of three bid prices raised the need for three versions of the ques- add a touch of colour and vitality to the scenery.
tionnaire, one for each bid price. Abandoned army bases have left underground shelters,
Part 3 of the questionnaire was designed to capture those socio- barracks, lookout posts and shore batteries. The panoramic view as
economic and demographic characteristics of respondents (age, one descends from the park includes all the major landmarks of the
gender, level of education and income) that have been shown in city below. In addition to what nature itself has provided, there are
previous studies to play a role in user valuation of similar spaces plenty of tracks and pathways, landscaped areas, public amenities,
(Álvarez & Larkin, 2008; Creel & Farell, 2008; Jim & Chen, 2006; ponds and information panels, etc. Since admission is free to all
Maat & De Vries, 2006; Mmopelwa, Kgathi, & Molefhe, 2007; who wish to enjoy the views, and generally relax, this park makes
Tyrväinen & Väänänen, 1998). a major contribution to the leisure and recreational opportunities,
Part 4 was designed as a laddering interview to reveal the and hence the quality of life, of locals and visitors alike.
means-end chains formed by respondents and thus determine the
benefits they expect to obtain and personal end values they hope to
4. Results
fulfil by using the park. In other words, this procedure tests for links
between certain personal values of the individual, the benefits
4.1. Characterisation of the groups based on their
derived from green space use, and the associated attributes. This
valuation of the green space
information, as stated earlier, may serve to guide environmental
management.
The primary goal of this study is to reveal the cognitive structure
A pre-test carried out prior to the survey on a sample of 30
of visitors to a peri-urban green space. An additional aim is to
subjects helped to improve the validity and user-friendliness of the
determine whether the complexity of the cognitive structure of the
questionnaire. The design and administration of the pilot study took
visitors and the linkages they establish differ according to their
place in a series of meetings and interviews with experts and focus
monetary valuation of the green space considered. To achieve these
groups, who suggested minor adjustments. All those who answered
objectives, two hierarchical value maps were created, one for the
the questionnaire remarked on its clarity, readability and ease of
group in which the contingent valuation estimate of WTP for green
understanding. As a result, no rewording of the items was required.
space benefits was higher than zero and another for the group
The chosen methodology was face-to-face interviews adminis-
declaring unwillingness to pay.
tered to respondents during their visit to the park. On average,
The two WTP groups were first characterised by usage
respondents took 15e20 min to fill out the questionnaires. They
frequency, user satisfaction levels, socio-demographic characte-
were also encouraged to make further suggestions to the inter-
ristics and differences with respect to the overall sample. As
viewer. Interviews with samples of visitors to the two parks took
already noted, of the 180 respondents who completed the ques-
place over the period January and February 2008. This resulted in 180
tionnaire, only 110 correctly completed the in-depth laddering
usable questionnaires from the 200 visitors recruited from the
interview. To assess whether the visitor profile remained invariant
various zones of the “Monte San Pedro Park” (A Coruña, Spain); but
over the whole sample (n ¼ 180) and the subsample (n ¼ 110),
only 110 valid replies to the laddering interview section, due to its
their respective socio-demographic profiles were estimated. Table
length and complexity. This is a larger sample than reported in other
1A in Annex 2 and Table 2 give the profiles of the overall sample
studies applying MEC theory, where, due to the numerous linkages
and subsample, respectively. Comparison of the two tables reveals
generated by this type of methodology, the average is 60 (Leppard
a similar visitor profile in both groups, despite the difference in
et al., 2004). Using distributions published in local census data7 it
sample size. Groups 1 and 2 contain similar proportions of
was possible to stratify the overall sample by age and gender. This
respondents (50% in each group). In both the overall sample
(n ¼ 180) and the subsample (n ¼ 110), group 2 respondents (those
declaring willingness to pay for the use and enjoyment of the park
facilities) show a lower rate of usage (42% versus 50%) and
5
The proposed assessment scenario and the contingent valuation questions are a higher level of user satisfaction (7.17 versus 7.18) than the overall
explained in Annex 1.
6
sample. In socio-demographic terms, the proportion of positive
The three bid prices were set based Silverman and Klock (1989) and Del Saz and
Suárez (1998) and the recommendations of the focus groups and panels of experts
consulted in the pre-test.
7 8
Data from statistical sources in the area concerned (Instituto Gallego de Esta- The flora are dominated by yellow gorse and pink heather interspersed with
dística (IGE)) show the samples to be representative of local age, gender and other typical coastal plants, some of them unique to the area. A great variety of
income strata, allowing us to conclude that San Pedro park is used by a represen- small birds including warblers, goldfinches and linnets add life and colour to the
tative cross-section of the local population. scenery.
N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889 881

Table 2
Characterisation of respondents based on willingness or unwillingness to pay for use of the green space (n ¼ 110).

Group 1 Group 2 c2a/Fb Sig.

Respondents who are unwilling to pay Respondents who are willing to pay for use
for use of the peri-urban green space, 49% of the peri-urban green space, 51%
Frequency of visits
Sporadic 64.8% 50.0% 2.46a 0.12
Weekly/monthly 35.2% 50.0%

General level of satisfaction 6.78 7.18 2.59b 0.11


Age
Under 20 0.0% 8.9% 6.15a 0.19
21e30 25.9% 32.1%
31e50 61.1% 48.2%
51e65 11.1% 8.9%
Over 65 1.9% 1.8%

Gender
Male 53.7% 53.6% 0.00a 0.99
Female 46.3% 46.4%

Income
Low 42.6% 23.2% 5.38a 0.07
Average 46.3% 55.4%
High 11.1% 21.4%

Level of education
Less than high 25.9% 14.3% 3.20a 0.20
School/vocational school 29.6% 42.9%
University Degree 44.4% 42.9%

*All the parameters have a non-significant value, p > 0.05.


a
Levels of statistical significance determined by Pearson’s chi-square tests.
b
Levels of statistical significance determined by the Anova F.

willingness to pay is higher in the 31e50 age category (53.4% for retains its significance (overall sample: t ¼ 4.48 (p < 0.01);
the overall sample and 48.2% for the subsample), and in the subsample: t ¼ 2.82 (p < 0.01)), and higher WTP can still be
middle-high class income group (60.2% for the overall and 55.4% observed among the younger members of the subsample
for the subsample). (t ¼ 1.63 (p < 0.10)) and among the women in the overall sample
The only noteworthy socio-demographic difference between (t ¼ 2.03 (p < 0.05)).
the two groups in both samples is disposable income (for the In order to complete the contingent valuation results and
overall sample: c2 ¼ 10.90 (sig. 0.00); for the subsample: c2 ¼ 5.38 analyse real WTP9, all protest responses were removed from the
(sig. 0.07)), which is not surprising given that WTP for use of the sample (Cho, Yen, Bowker, & Newman, 2008; Verbic & Slabe-Erker,
park is the classification criterion. Income usually tends to play 2008). This reduced the number of observations to 97 in the overall
a role in WTP for environmental goods. Thus, low income is posi- sample and 66 in the subsample. The response options: “I already
tively associated with unwillingness to pay. pay enough in taxes” and “I’m not sure the money would be put to
Following the proposed objectives, the next step is to determine good use” were interpreted as protest responses.
the cognitive structure displayed by each group. The ultimate After screening for protest responses, a repeat logit analysis
interest is to test for variation in cognitive structure in relation to resulted in considerably higher WTP values than those presented in
WTP, taking into account both the range of issues considered in the Table 3 (a mean WTP of 2.38 Euros for the overall sample and 2.74
decision-making process and the level of abstraction. The main Euros for the subsample). The statistical significance of the bid price
distinguishing feature between this study and others relating variable in both models is slightly higher (overall sample: t ¼ 4.65
personal values to WTP (Cooper et al., 2004; Harris & Brown, 1992; (p < 0.01); subsample: t ¼ 3.01 (p < 0.01)), again suggesting the
Kotchen & Reiling, 2000; Mill et al., 2007) lies in the fact that the presence of starting-point bias.
selected methodology enables detection of interrelations between Table 4 reveals the influence of the socio-demographic variables
personal values, desired benefits and the differentiating attributes on real WTP in both samples. The bid price retains its significance
of the good in question. (overall sample: t ¼ 4.46 (p < 0.01); subsample: t ¼ 2.68
(p < 0.01)), and there was no significant variation in the effect of the
4.2. Contingent valuation results remaining socio-economic variables.

As already stated, WTP was estimated by the contingent valua-


4.3. Hierarchical value maps
tion method. Table 3 presents the results of the estimation of the
logit models for the overall sample (n ¼ 180) and the subsample of
The WTP profiles resulting from the CV application described
laddering interviews (n ¼ 110), to enable comparison of the results.
above enabled the construction of two 6-level hierarchical value
The table shows that mean WTP is 1.33 Euros for the overall
maps, one for each WTP group. The cut-off points for the different
sample and 1.014 Euros for the smaller subsample. The significance
levels of abstraction appear in Table 5. These maps show all the
of the bid price variable in both models (overall sample: t ¼ 4.62
(p < 0.01); subsample: t ¼ 2.75 (p < 0.01)) suggests the presence
of starting-point bias. 9
As indicated in the methodology section, the questionnaire included a control
The data shown in Table 3 also reveal the influence of socio- item in order to distinguish protest zeros from real zeros. The scale items designed
demographic variables on WTP in both samples. The bid price to identify reasons for unwillingness to pay are given in Appendix 1.
882 N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

Table 3 Table 5
Logit models estimated to calculate mean WTP and assess the influence of socio- Cut-off points for the 5 levels of abstraction and percentage of total cases.
economic variables.
Group 1 Group 2
Hanemann’s model E(WTP)
Respondents who are Respondents who are
n ¼ 180 n ¼ 110 unwilling to pay for use willing to pay for use
of the peri-urban of the peri-urban
Coefficient T-Ratio Coefficient T-Ratio
green space green space
Constant 1.672 2.779*** 0.918 1.25
Cut-off point % Cut-off point %
Bid price 1.260 4.62*** 0.905 2.75***
Level 1 ACa 32 29.0 34 30.9
Log-likelihood 83.10 56.96 CVb 26 23.6 25 22.7
Chi-squared 26.95 8.67 Level 2 AC 29 26.3 28 25.4
Sig. 0.000 0.003 CV 23 20.9 22 20.0
E(WTP) 1.33 Euros 1.014 Euros Level 3 AC 26 23.6 25 22.7
CV 21 19.0 21 19.0
Level 4 AC 25 22.7 23 20.9
Logit model estimated to assess the influence
of socio-economic variables CV 20 18.1 19 17.2
Level 5 AC 24 21.8 20 18.1
n ¼ 180 n ¼ 110 CV 19 17.2 18 16.3
Coefficient T-Ratio Coefficient T-Ratio Level 6 AC 22 20.0 19 17.2
CV 15 13.6 17 15.4
Constant 1.370 0.87 0.297 0.163
Bid price 1.250 4.48*** 0.965 2.82*** a
Attribute-consequence.
Age 0.289 1.14 0.515 1.63* b
Consequence-value.
Gender 0.817 2.03** 0.777 1.57
Education level 0.087 0.34 0.234 0.71
Income 0.263 0.806 0.029 0.08
Frequency 0.273 0.667 0.20 0.40
off points for the consequence-value linkages are 15 and 17,
respectively.
Log-likelihood 79.37 61.30
Figs. 1 and 2 show the HVM (hierarchical value maps) for the
Chi-squared 34.41 15.14
Sig. 0.00005 0.019 two groups, with cut-off levels of 6 in both cases. The percentage
value that appears next to each element in the chain (attributes,
*p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01.
consequences and values) indicates the proportion of respondents
making that particular linkage. Both groups show a high frequency
attribute-consequence linkages and all the consequence-value of linkages between the different levels on the ladders, giving an
linkages at or above the frequency of the sixth level of importance. initial indication of the relevance of the items selected for the MEC
Following Leppard et al. (2004) the cut-off point is different for analysis. That is, respondents establish a considerable number of
every level of abstraction and group of subjects, enabling use of the linkages between the attributes of the park, the consequences or
maps for between-group comparison. Thus, the cut-off point for the benefits they obtain from it, and their own personal values. This
attribute-consequence linkages is 22 on the map of the “unwilling- supports the notion that personal values play a part in users’
to-pay” group and 19 on that of the “willing-to-pay” group. The cut- perception of the attributes or differentiating features of the envi-
ronmental good, with variations according to the desired conse-
Table 4
quences or benefits.
Logit models estimated to calculate mean WTP and assess the influence of socio- Analysis of the information provided by these linkages reveals,
economic variables without protest responses. first, the existence of interesting between-group similarities. Thus,
Hanemann’s model E(WTP)
in terms of attributes, both groups value concrete features such
as “a place to practice sport and take physical exercise”
n ¼ 97 (total sample) n ¼ 66 (subsample)
and “distance from home”. One of the concrete attributes consi-
Coefficient T-Ratio Coefficient T-Ratio dered only by the positive WTP group is “price”, indicating its
Constant 3.58 4.38*** 3.54 3.12*** influence on WTP for green space facilities. The MEC method also
Bid price 1.502 4.65*** 1.29 3.01*** shows which abstract attributes are important. The results indicate
Log-likelihood 57.47 24.27 that the two groups have similar views on issues relating to the use
Chi-squared 28.24 10.98 and enjoyment of the park, since both value “landscape beauty”,
Sig. 0.0000 0.009 “benefits to the health” and “less noise”.
E(WTP) 2.38 2.74 In this kind of discussion, it is usual to make a distinction
between functional and psychological consequences. As far as
Logit model estimated to assess the influence functional consequences are concerned, both groups appear to
of socio-economic variables
value the frequency of visits (“frequency of use”), the opportunity
n ¼ 97 n ¼ 66 to take a break from the daily routine (“a way to escape from
Coefficient T-Ratio Coefficient T-Ratio routine”) and environmental awareness (“I help the environment”).
Constant 2.60 1.22 3.80 1.36 The finding that the positive WTP group was the only one to focus
Bid price 1.57 4.46*** 1.31 2.68*** on the issue of “ecological habits” is a clear signal to managers of
Age 0.06 0.17 0.205 0.39
Gender 0.39 0.69 0.252 0.31
this type of environmental goods that ecological awareness is
Education level 0.34 0.90 0.568 1.16 associated with higher WTP for green space use.
Income 0.13 0.31 0.315 0.53 The two groups also share a similar profile with respect to
Frequency 0.65 1.20 0.817 1.03 psychological consequences, both showing an appreciation for the
Log-likelihood 57.47 29.76 psycho-physical benefits derived from green space use (“physical
Chi-squared 31.61 14.64 well-being” and “mental well-being”) and the resulting health
Sig. 0.0001 0.023 benefits (“stress relief” and “rest”). In short, visitors appreciate both
*p < 0.10, **p < 0.05, ***p < 0.01. the functional and psychological benefits of green space use,
N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889 883

A sense of self- Better Attributes

Values
Complete ladder

End
fulfilment and relationships Consecuences
Incomplete ladder
Values
accomplishment with others
Enhaces my

Instrumental
13.6% A source of
quality of life

Values
fun, pleasure
and enjoyment and security
17.2%
20.9% 17.2% 23.9%
Consequences
Psychological

19.0% 19.0%

Physical Mental well- Stress relief Rest


well- being 18.1% 17.2%
being
I help the A way to Frequency
Consequences

20.0% 21.8% 26.3% 20.0% 22.7%


Functional

environment escape of use


from
routine

29.0% 23.6%
Landscape Benefits to Less noise
beauty the health
Attributes
Abstract

Distance from
Attributes
Concrete

A place to take home


physical exercise

Fig. 1. Hierarchical value map for Group 1 “Respondents unwilling to pay for use of the peri-urban green space” (for a cut-off point of 6).

Peace of Better A sense of self-


Values
End

mind dignity relationships fulfilment and


and with others accomplishment
selfrespect
Instrumental

A source of 16.3% 19.0% Enhaces my


Values

fun, pleasure quality of life


15.4%
and enjoyment and security
17.2%
17.2% 17.2% 19.0%
17.2%
Consequences
Psychological

20.0% 22.7%

Mental well- Rest


Physical well-being Stress relief being
I have good 25.4% 18.1%
ecological A way to Frequency I help the
Consequences

escape of use environment


Functional

habits
from 20.9% 20.9%
routine

30.9% 17.2% 18.1% 22.7% 18.1%


Benefits to Less noise Landscape
the health beauty
Attributes
Abstract

A place to take Distance from Price


Attributes
Concrete

physical exercise home

Attributes
Complete ladder
Consecuences
Incomplete ladder
Values

Fig. 2. Hierarchical value map for Group 2 “Respondents willing to pay for use of the peri-urban green space” (for a cut-off point of 6).
884 N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

although the ecological component has greater weight among frequent visits. The linkages between park attributes and health
those whose monetary valuation of the good is higher. benefits reveal more abstract or complex associations involving
A final key aspect of the between-group comparison concerns terminal values, such as the enhancement of relationships with
the personal end values included in the hierarchical structure, others and quality of life.
which fall into two categories: instrumental and terminal. Both Nevertheless, the HVMs also reveal a series of between-group
groups mention the instrumental values “A source of fun, pleasure differences in some of the ladders. An important factor for the
and enjoyment” and “enhances my quality of life and security” and positive WTP group is “price”, that is, the admission fee, an attri-
the terminal values “better relationships with others” and “a sense bute associated by this group with the functional consequence
of self-fulfilment and accomplishment”. Another terminal value “frequency of use” (a link formed by 18.1% of the respondents) and
“Peace of mind, dignity and self-respect” appears only in the the instrumental value “a source of fun, pleasure and enjoyment”
positive WTP group. The greater use of values, particularly terminal (17.2%). Similarly, this group linked the abstract attribute “less
values, by this group appears to suggest an association between noise” with the psychological consequence “mental well-being”
WTP and a higher level of abstraction. Overall, the results confirm (18.1%) and the terminal value “better relationships with others”
the relevance of personal values in the valuation of green spaces. (19.0%). Lastly, a linkage appears between the abstract attribute
These initial results, nevertheless, call for deeper analysis and “landscape beauty” and the psychological consequence “rest”
further examination of the ladders generated by the MEC applica- (18.1%) and the instrumental value “enhances my quality of life and
tion to enable clearer conclusions regarding the difference in security” (19.0%). Particularly striking is an incomplete ladder that
abstraction levels between the two groups. is exclusive to this group, linking the functional consequence
“I have good ecological habits” and the terminal value “peace of
4.4. Analysis of the HVM ladders mind, dignity and self-respect” (17.2%).
These results may lend support to the assertion that
The initial results described above are eligible for further ana- cognitive structures involving health-related factors (noise avoi-
lysis aimed at advancing in the understanding of the means-end dance, rest inducement, improvement of mental well-being) are
chain formation process through which users link green space more complex, since they include a greater number of links
attributes to consequences and thence to values. The ladders between abstract attributes and instrumental and terminal values
observed in this study show four linkages common to all respon- (quality of life at the individual level and better relationships and
dents, irrespective of WTP levels. The most important chain is the peace of mind at a more social, collective level). A further finding is
one linking the concrete attribute “a place to practice sport and take that respondents link concrete attributes, such as use of the park for
physical exercise”, the psychological consequence “physical well- recreation, sports practice, and appreciation of the landscape to
being” (formed by 29.0% of the zero WTP group and 30.9% of the personal enjoyment. Lastly, it is worth noting that linkages relating
positive WTP group) and the instrumental value “a source of fun, to ecological issues are more numerous among those declaring
pleasure and enjoyment” (19.0% versus 22.7%). This appears to positive WTP. The following section proceeds with the analysis in
suggest, as might be expected, that one of the main values pursued order to determine whether the initial differences, in terms of the
by green space users is the pleasure of exercise and keeping fit. complexity of the cognitive process as revealed by the HVMs for
A second important chain links the concrete attribute “distance the zero WTP and positive WTP groups, find further reflection in
from home” to the functional consequence “frequency of use” different degrees of abstraction.
(23.6% of the zero WTP group, 17.2% of the positive WTP group) and
the value “a source of fun, pleasure and enjoyment” (17.2% versus
17.2%). This is also unsurprising, given that, where green space use 4.5. Comparison of abstraction levels
is concerned, frequency depends on proximity, and enjoyment
increases with frequency. The third key feature concerns the lin- Having identified the desired attributes, consequences and
kages between the abstract attribute “benefits to the health” and personal values of each WTP group, and established the chains of
the psychological consequences “stress relief” (26.3% for the zero linkages, the next step was to assess the degree of abstraction of
WTP group, 20.9% for the positive WTP group) and “mental well- each group’s cognitive structure. This was done by calculating the
being” (21.8% versus 25.4%). The latter, in turn, appear linked to the average number of ladders and average number of elements of each
instrumental value “a source of fun, pleasure and enjoyment” level (attributes, consequences and values), formed by each group
(19.0% versus 17.2%) and the terminal value “better relationships in order to check for possible between-group variation.
with others” (13.6% versus 19.0%). The sum of ladders for the overall sample was 1,917. The sorting
The last key linkage, common to both groups, is between the of these by WTP group revealed statistically significant differences
abstract attribute “less noise” and the psychological consequences in terms of the average number of incomplete ladders, as shown in
“stress relief” (20.0% for the zero WTP group, 20.9% for the Table 6. Thus, respondents belonging to the positive WTP group
positive WTP group) and “rest” (22.7% for both groups) and the formed an average of 14.70 complete ladders and 2.84 incomplete
instrumental values “a source of fun, pleasure and enjoyment”
(19.0% versus 17.2%) and “enhances my quality of life and secu-
rity” (23.9% versus 19.0%), respectively. This is a further reflection Table 6
of the association between green space use and health benefits. Average numbers of complete and incomplete ladders.
Thus, this methodology provides further confirmation of the F de Group 1 Group 2
interesting linkages that environmental goods users establish Snedecor
Respondents who are Respondents who are
between aesthetic, health-related, psycho-physical and social unwilling to pay for use of willing to pay for use of the
benefits. the peri-urban green space peri-urban green space
Across the sample as a whole, therefore, the ladders that Complete 0.331 13.57 14.70
appear on the HVMs indicate more basic, less complex, associa- ladders
tions between instrumental values, such as fun, pleasure and Incomplete 5.204** 4.11 2.84
enjoyment, and the park’s concrete attributes, such as opportu- ladders

nities for practising sport and keeping fit or the possibility of **p < 0.05.
N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889 885

Table 7
Average numbers of attributes, consequences and values done by each group

F Snedecor Group 1 Group 2

Respondents who are unwilling to Respondents who are willing to pay for
pay for use of the peri-urban green space use of the peri-urban green space
Concrete attributes 0.972 3.64 4.00
Abstract attributes 0.833 7.36 6.74
Functional consequences 2.651* 4.35 5.03
Psychological consequences 1.114 6.75 7.36
Instrumental values 0.505 4.62 4.91
Terminal values 8.573*** 3.32 4.77

*p < 0.10 and ***p < 0.01.

ladders (F ¼ 5.204, sig < 0.05), versus 13.57 complete and 4.11 clear evidence of more complex cognitive structures in respondents
incomplete in the zero/negative WTP group (F ¼ 0.331, sig > 0.10). declaring higher WTP. A fuller explanation of the complexity of the
These initial findings already point to greater complexity in the green space user’s decision-making process will therefore require
positive WTP group, because its members form a greater number of more detailed analysis.
attribute-consequence-value chains. Nevertheless, fuller analysis
will be required in order to support these findings. 5. Discussion and conclusions
The above results reveal a certain degree of abstraction in green
space valuation, varying with WTP. Table 7 presents a summary of The conception and valuation of natural spaces in general, and
the average numbers of attributes, consequences and values eli- peri-urban green spaces in particular, have evolved in line with
cited from each group. The statistically significant differences that increasing awareness of the multiple benefits they provide in
can be observed relate to the number of terminal values (F ¼ 8.573, terms of the physical and mental health of users. This evolution
sig < 0.01), which is greater in the positive WTP group, indicating has given rise to a new approach to the valuation of natural
slightly more complexity or abstraction in their valuation process, resources that takes into account, as well as user socio-demo-
which suggests higher personal involvement (Walker & Olson, graphics, monetary valuation and the psycho-physiological bene-
1991). The presentation of the findings concludes with the fits offered by such areas, the personal values and emotional
following table showing the complexity indices. attachments that visitors associate with them. Emotional benefits
play such an important role in people’s daily lives, particularly in
4.6. Complexity indices decisions relating to the use and monetary valuation of green
spaces, that they can be said to play a key role in generating
The final part of the analysis was to compare the complexity of subjective responses in visitors and influencing the way they use
the attribute-consequence-value chains appearing on the HVMs of and perceive green space facilities.
the two WTP groups, using the so-called complexity index, first Inspired by the suggestion that emotional responses to green
developed by Bagozzi and Dabholkar (1994). Two complexity space use may play a key role, this study set out to explore the
indices were estimated. The first, labelled C1, measures complexity cognitive structures of a sample of green space visitors and estimate
in terms of the concepts used in the HVMs. It is the result of their WTP for such resources. The underlying cognitive structure
dividing the number of complete chains on each HVM by the total was revealed by means of means-end chain (MEC) methodology.
number of attributes, consequences and values represented. The From an overall sample of green space visitors, two subsamples
second, labelled C2, measures complexity in terms of the linkages based on contingent valuation estimates of WTP emerged. The
forming the ladders on the HVMs. It is the result of dividing the resulting visitor profile showed that respondents declaring wil-
length of each chain of concepts (that is, the sum of its linkages) by lingness to pay for the use and enjoyment of park facilities report
the overall sum of linkages on the HVM. lower usage frequency rates and higher user satisfaction levels and
As can be seen from Table 8, the positive WTP group scored tend to fall into the 31e50 year old, middle class/middle income
higher than the zero WTP group on C1 (96.74 versus 94.75), but not group. These findings are consistent with previous research on green
on C2 (1.42 versus 1.50). These results reveal an association space visitor typologies (Jim & Chen, 2006; Vesely, 2007; Yilmaz,
between WTP and greater complexity of the hierarchical value Zengin, & Yildiz, 2007, among others). The only noteworthy socio-
structure in terms of the number of attributes, consequences and demographic difference between the two groups in both samples is
values it includes. In contrast, there is no apparent association disposable income. This initial finding is consistent with prior
between WTP and greater complexity in terms of the number of research, where higher WTP is always associated with higher
linkages established between the attributes, consequences and income groups (Feinerman, Fleischer, & Simhon, 2004; Mmopelwa
values. Thus, as previous analyses have already shown, there is no et al., 2007; Togridou et al., 2006; Gürlük, 2006). This supports the
conclusion that the variation subsequently found in the analysis of
the decision-making structure is due not to differences in visitor
Table 8 profiles but to different perceptions of the attributes, consequences
Complexity indices. and values under scrutiny.
Number of Number Number Total C1 C2
The contingent valuation estimates of WTP were low at 1.33
cognitions of links of paths length of (b/a) (d/c) Euros for the overall sample and 1.014 Euros for the smaller
(a) (b) (c) paths (d) subsample, revealing the possible presence of starting-point bias.
Respondents who 16 1516 16 24 94.75 1.50 Furthermore, only 51% of the subsample and 49% of the overall
are unwilling to pay sample declared their willingness to pay for the facilities.
Respondents who 19 1838 19 27 96.74 1.42 These percentages of positive WTP responses lie within
are willing to pay
the range of sample percentages obtained in similar studies: 79%
886 N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

(Meyerhoff & Liebe, 2006); 35% (Adams et al., 2008; Del Based on the above findings, one interesting conclusion might
Saz, Barreiro, & García-Menéndez, 2003); and 46.8% (Jones, be that the greater the perceived benefits from environmental
Sophoulis, & Malesios, 2008). These high refusal-to-pay rates goods, the greater the user’s personal involvement with them. It
suggest considerable resistance to the elicitation format used and would therefore be useful to convey to the public the range of
the unfamiliarity of Spanish respondents with this kind of decision- physical and psychological benefits the environment has to offer,
making process (Del Saz-Salazar & Rausell-Köster, 2008). in order to increase their involvement in environmental conser-
The high refusal-to-pay rates obtained in this study led to vation and protection and respect for nature. One of the obvious
a repeat analysis excluding protest zeros, which resulted in potential impacts of the perceived mental and physical health
a substantial improvement of the mean WTP values, that is, 2.38 benefits, and therefore worth factoring into the valuation, is the
Euros for the overall sample and 2.74 Euros for the smaller reduction of the social cost of health care. Furthermore, the
subsample, although the possibility of the presence of starting- proximity of peri-urban green spaces makes them highly acce-
point bias still remained. This shows the influence of protest beliefs ssible to urban dwellers, increasing their health benefit potential
on WTP, in the sense that, the higher the number of protest for this segment of the population. These conditioning factors are
responses, the lower the WTP for the environmental good in worth consideration by land-use managers, who might use them
question (Meyerhoff & Liebe, 2006). The high rate of protest bids to promote the social value of natural resources and to strengthen
obtained in this analysis might be explained by poor economic their arguments in decision-making settings involving environ-
status (low income or unemployment) and hence inability to pay, mental cost/benefit issues. It might also be useful to promote
and difficulty in expressing the somewhat abstract idea of valua- cooperation between representatives of the local population and
tion in monetary terms (Jim & Chen 2006; Tyrväinen & Väänänen, land-use managers in green space designs.
1998). The application of alternative methodological and attitudinal
The MEC analysis, performed with the in-depth laddering approaches or social psychology theories could enrich the analysis
interview technique, confirmed that a large number of values are of this type of cognitive response and enable the prediction of
associated with the enjoyment of environmental goods. Observa- environmental goods users’ decisions. Further understanding of the
tion of the HVM (hierarchical value maps) reveals the importance of cognitive structures of non-visitors would also be desirable, in
certain concrete and abstract attributes (sporting activities, distance order to establish differences between users and non-users. Efforts
from home, landscape beauty, health benefits). These results fall in to match visitor profiles to desired green space attributes and
line with findings obtained with different methodology, indicating benefits and to reveal the personal values involved in each case
that green space valuation depends on the admission fee, the would also enable extrapolation of the findings to other types of
proximity, and the facilities provided (Baral et al., 2008; Togridou environmental goods.
et al., 2006; Tyrväinen & Väänänen, 1998). An association between
the positive valuation of the environmental good and the search for Annex 1
beautiful scenery and quieter surroundings has also been reported
(Lee & Han, 2002; Prada, González, Polomé, Gómez, & Vázquez, Questions used in the contingent valuation:
2001). Some findings also point to the influence of awareness of
“A periurban park such as .. can be enjoyed for different
the functional and psychological benefits provided by green space
recreational uses. In the questions that follow, therefore, we
use (Chiesura, 2004; Davenport et al., 2002; Gidlöf-Gunnarsson &
wish to ask you to estimate a monetary value for the satisfaction
Öhrström, 2007). Previous studies using alternative methodology
or feeling of well-being you obtain from such uses, that is, how
had already reached conclusions regarding the influence of value
you value the good. Bear in mind that we are asking you to think
fulfilment, both individual (relating to fun or quality of life) and
in terms of real payment and to assume that the amount spent
social (relationships with others), on the valuation of green spaces
could not be used for any other purpose”
(Ford et al., 2009; González, 2002; Harris & Brown, 1992; Hanley
et al., 2009; Mill et al., 2007; Tyrväinen et al., 2007). Involvement
- Taking into account all the possible benefits provided by the
in ecological issues and social awareness also feature strongly
area as a whole, would you be willing to pay an entrance fee of
among the values revealed by the positive WTP group.
X V?
All of the above supports the conclusion that there is an
 Yes
emotional component in what peri-urban green spaces offer their
 No
users. In addition, respondents expressing willingness to pay show
- Bearing in mind that you would be willing to pay X V, how
greater personal involvement through the terminal values they
much more would you be willing to pay?..V
associate with the environment and claim to get more than mere
- Bearing in mind that you would not be willing to pay X V, what
fun and enjoyment out of their visits. Psychological consequences
is the maximum price you would be willing to pay?..V
or benefits, such as rest or stress relief, feature more prominently
- If you are NOT willing to pay, please indicate your reasons by
in association with positive WTP. The awareness of health benefits
placing an X in the appropriate box
resulting from the enjoyment of environmental goods leads to
more complex cognitive structures, that is, greater involvement of
the subject’s personal values, where social values play a stronger
role than individual values. This confirms the already-accepted
association between health benefits and the use of this type of
goods (Hansmann et al., 2007; Kaplan, 1995; Korpela et al., 2008; Reason for unwillingness to pay Score

Maas & Verheij, 2007; Schultz, 2001; Velarde et al., 2007) while I already pay enough in taxes
This environmental resource is not worth
also revealing the previously reported link with collective or social
an entrance fee
activities (Chiesura, 2004; Sanesi & Chiarello, 2006). Thus, the I’m not sure the money would be put to
emotional attachment of people to nature is manifest not only in good use
their use of green spaces in general or their preference for one in I think entrance should be free of charge
particular, but also in their perception of green spaces as a means I couldn’t afford to pay an entrance fee
Don’t know/no answer
to satisfy personal values.
N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889 887

Annex 2

Table 1A
Characterisation of respondents based on willingness or unwillingness to pay for use of the green space (N ¼ 180).

Group 1 Group 2 c2a/Fb Sig.

Respondents who are unwilling to pay Respondents who are willing to pay
for use of the peri-urban green space, 51% for use of the peri-urban green space, 49%
Frequency of visits
Sporadic 55.4% 58.0% 0.12 0.73
Weekly/monthly 44.6% 42.0%

General level of satisfaction 6.99 7.17 0.83 0.36


Age
Under 20 0.0% 6.8% 7.65 0.11
21e30 27.2% 27.3%
31e50 56.5% 53.4%
51e65 13.0% 8.0%
Over 65 3.3% 4.5%

Gender
Male 53.3% 47.7%
Female 46.7% 52.3% 0.55 0.46

Income
Low 44.6% 21.6% 10.90 0.00
Average 44.6% 60.2%
High 10.9% 18.2%

Level of education 26.1% 17.0% 2.31 0.31


Less than high school/ 39.1% 46.6%
vocational school
University degree 34.8% 36.4%

*Only income shows a significant value, p < 0.05.


a
Levels of statistical significance determined by Pearson’s chi-square tests.
b
Levels of statistical significance determined by the Anova F.

Bodur, M., & Sarogöllü, E. (2005). Environmental sensitivity in a developing country.


Consumer classification and applications. Environment and Behavior, 37, 487e510.
References Bourne, H., & Jenkins, M. (2005). Eliciting manager’s personal values. An adapta-
tion of the laddering interview method. Organizational Research Methods, 8,
Adams, C., Seroa da Motta, R., Arigoni, O. R., Reid, J., Ebersbach, A. C., & 410e428.
De Almeida, S. P. A. (2008). The use of contingent valuation for evaluating Brown, G. (2005). Mapping spatial attributes in survey research for natural resource
protected areas in the developing world: economic valuation of Morro do management: methods and applications. Society and Natural Resources, 18,
Diablo State Park, Atlantic Rainforest, Sao Paulo State (Brazil). Ecological 17e39.
Economics, 66, 359e370. Brown, G., & Raymond, C. (2007). The relationship between place attachment and
Álvarez, S., Larkin, S. L. (2008). Valuing recreational benefits of a national park in landscape values: toward mapping place attachment. Applied Geography, 27,
Andean Columbia. In: Selected paper prepared for presentation at the Southern 89e111.
Agricultural Economics Association Annual Meetings Dallas, TX, February 2e6. Buckley, C., van Rensburg, T. M., & Hynes, S. (2009). Recreational demand for farm
Arrow, K., Solow, R., Portney, P. R., Leamer, E. E., Radner, R. & Schuman, H. (1993). commonage in Ireland: a contingent valuation assessment. Land Use Policy, 26,
Report of the NOAA panel on contingent valuation. January 11, 1993, 66 pp. 846e854.
Asayu-Adjaye, J., & Tapsuwan, S. (2008). A contingent valuation study of scuba Burgess, J., Harrisson, C. M., & Limb, M. (1998). People, parks and the urban green:
diving benefits: case study in Mu Ko Similan Marine National Park, Thailand. a study of popular meanings and values for open spaces in the city. Urban
Tourism Management, 29, 1122e1130. Studies, 25, 455e473.
Audenaert, A., & Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M. (1997). A consumer-oriented approach to the Carson, R. T. (2000). Contingent valuation: a user’s guide. Environmental Sciences
marketing of food products: application of means-end chain theory to the and Technology, 34, 1413e1418.
consumption of beef. In B. Wierenga, A. Van Tilburg, K. Grunert, Carson, R. T., Flores, N. E., & Meade, N. F. (2001). Contingent valuation: controversies
J. B. E. M. Steenkamp, & M. Wedel (Eds.), Agricultural marketing and consumer and evidence. Environmental and Resource Economics, 19, 173e210.
behaviour in a changing world. Kluwer Academic Publishers. Chen, W. Y., & Jim, C. Y. (2008). Cost-benefit analysis of the leisure value of urban
Bagozzi, R., & Dabholkar, P. (1994). Consumer recycling goals and their effect on greening in the new Chinese city of Zhuhai. Cities, 25, 298e309.
decisions to recycle: a mean end chain analysis. Psychology and Marketing, 11, Chiesura, A. (2004). The role of urban parks for the sustainable city. Landscape and
1e28. Urban Planning, 68, 129e138.
Baral, N., Stern, M. J., & Bhattarai, R. (2008). Contingent valuation of ecotourism in Chiu, C.-M. (2005). Applying means-end chain theory to eliciting system require-
Annapurna Conservation Area, Nepal: implications for sustainable park finance ments and understanding users perceptual orientations. Information and
and local development. Ecological Economics, 66(2/3), 218e227. Management, 42, 455e468.
Barbosa, O., Tratalos, J. A., Armsworth, P. R., Davies, R. G., Fuller, R. A., Jonson, P., et al. Cho, S., Yen, S., Bowker, J. M., & Newman, D. (2008). Modeling willingness to pay for
(2007). Who benefits from access to green space? A case of study from Shef- land conservation easements: treatment of zero and protest bids and application
field, UK. Landscape and Urban Planning, 83(2/3), 187e195. and policy implications. Journal of Agricultural and Applied Economics, 40, 267e285.
Barrena, R., & Sánchez, M. (2009). Using emotional benefits as a differentiation Chryssohoidis, G. M., & Krystallis, A. (2005). Organic consumers` personal values
strategy in saturated markets. Psychology and Marketing, 26(11), 1002e1030. research: testing and validating the list of values (LOV) scale and implementing
Bateman, I. J., Langford, I. H., & Rasbash, J. (1999). Willingness to pay question a value-based segmentation task. Food Quality and Preference, 16, 585e599.
format in contingent valuation studies. In J. E. Araña, & C. J. León (Eds.), Cogoy, M. (1999). The consumer as a social and environmental actor. Ecological
Actualización de las preferencias en procesos iterativos de valoración con- Economics, 28, 385e398.
tingente. Hacienda Pública Española/Revista de Economía Pública, 177, 83e115. Coley, R., Kuo, F., & Sullivan, W. (1997). Where does community grow? The social
Bengston, D. N. (1994). Changing forest values and ecosystem management. Society context created by nature in urban public housing. Environment and Behavior,
and Natural Resources, 7, 515e533. 29, 468e494.
Bernarth, K., & Roschewitz, A. (2008). Recreational benefits of urban forests: Coogan, M. A., Karash, K. H., Adler, T., & Sallis, J. (2007). The role of personal values,
explaining visitors’ willingness to pay in the context of the theory of planned urban form, and autoavailability in the analysis of walking for transportation.
behavior. Journal of Environmental Management, 89, 155e166. The Science of Health Promotic, 21, 363e370.
888 N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889

Cooper, P., Poe, G. L., & Bateman, I. J. (2004). The structure of motivation for Jorgensen, B. S., Wilson, M. A., & Heberlein, T. A. (2001). Fairness in the contingent
contingent values: a case study of lake water quality improvement. Ecological valuation of environmental goods: attitude toward paying for environmental
Economics, 50, 69e82. improvements at two levels of scope. Ecological Economics, 36(1), 133e148.
Costa, A. I. A., Dekker, M., & Jongen, W. M. F. (2004). An overview of means-end Kalhe, L. R. (1985). The nine nations of North America and the value basis of
theory: potential application in consumer-oriented food product design. Trends geographic segmentation. Journal of Marketing, 50, 37e47.
in Food Science and Technology, 15, 403e415. Kalisch, D., & Klaphake, A. (2007). Visitors’ satisfaction and perception of crowding
Creel, M., & Farell, M. (2008). Usage and valuation of natural parks in Catalonia, in a German National Park: a case study on the island of Halling Hooge. Snow
2001e2002. Investigaciones Económicas, 32, 5e25. Landscape Resources, 81, 109e122.
Davenport, M., Borrie, W., Freimund, W., & Manning, R. (2002). Assessing the Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative frame-
relationship desired experiences and support for management actions at Yel- work. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169e182.
lowstone National Park using multiple methods. Journal of Park and Recreation Kaltenborn, B. P., & Bjerke, T. (2002). Associations between environmental value
Administration, 20(3), 51e64. orientations and landscape preferences. Landscape and Urban Planning, 59, 1e11.
Del Saz, S., Barreiro, J., & García-Menéndez, L. (2003). Provisión pública versus Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.
provisión privada de suelo industrial: una aproximación hedónica. Santa Cruz de Klijn, J. A., Buij, A. E., Dijkstra, H., Luttik, J., & Veeneklaas, F. R. (2000). The forgotten
Tenerife: X Encuentro de Economía Pública. values of nature and landscape. Use and appreciation measured in money and
Del Saz, S., García, L. M., & Palau, J. M. (1999). Los beneficios sociales de la remodelación emotional value. Wageningen: Alterra Green World Research.
urbanística de la fachada litoral de Valencia: un estudio de valoración contingente. Korpela, K. M., Ylén, M., Tyrväinen, L., & Silvennoinen, H. (2008). Determinants of
Biblioteca Civitas Economía y Empresa, Colección Economía. 209 pp. restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health and Place, 14, 636e652.
Del Saz, S., & García, L. (2007). Estimating the non-market benefits of an urban park: Kotchen, M. J., & Reiling, S. D. (2000). Environmental attitudes, motivations, and
does proximity matter? Land Use Policy, 24, 296e305. contingent valuation of nonuse values: a case study involving endangered
Del Saz-Salazar, S., & Rausell-Köster, P. (2008). A double-hurdle model of urban species. Ecological Economics, 32, 93e107.
green areas valuation: dealing with zero responses. Landscape and Urban Krenichyn, K. (2006). The only place to go and be in the city: women talk about
Planning, 84, 241e251. exercise, being outdoors, and the meaning of a large urban park. Health and
Del Saz, S., & Suárez, C. (1998). El valor del uso recreativo de espacios naturales Place, 12, 631e643.
protegidos: aplicación del método de valoración contingente al Parque Natural Kyle, G. T., Mowen, A. J., & Terrant, M. (2004). Linking place preferences with place
de l’Albufera. Revista Española de Economía Agraria, 182, 239e272. meaning: an examination of the relationship between place motivation and
Feinerman, E., Fleischer, A., & Simhon, A. (2004). Distributional welfare impacts of place attachment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 439e454.
public spending: the case of urban versus national parks. Journal of Agricultural Lee, C.-K., & Han, S.-Y. (2002). Estimating the use and preservation values of
and Resource Economics, 29(2), 370e386. national parks` tourism resources using contingent valuation method. Tourism
Ford, R. M., Williams, K. J. H., Bishop, I. D., & Webb, T. (2009). A value basis for the Management, 23, 531e540.
social acceptability of clearfelling in Tasmania, Australia. Landscape and Urban Lee, J. A., Soutar, G., & Louviere, J. (2007). Measuring values using best-worst
Planning, 90, 196e206. scaling: the LOV example. Psychology and Marketing, 24(12), 1043e1058.
Galindo, M. P. G., & Corraliza, J. A. R. (2000). Environmental aesthetics and psycho- León, C. J. (1996). Double bounded survival values for preserving the landscape of
logical wellbeing: relationships between preference judgements for urban natural parks. Journal of Environmental Management, 46, 103e118.
landscapes and other relevant affective responses. Psychology in Spain, 4, 13e27. Leppard, P., Russel, C. G., & Cox, D. N. (2004). Improving means-end-chain studies by
Gidlöf-Gunnarsson, A., & Öhrström, E. (2007). Noise and well-being in urban resi- using a ranking method to construct hierarchical value map. Food Quality and
dential environments: the potential role of perceived availability to nearby Preference, 15, 489e497.
green areas. Landscape and Urban Planning, 83 (2/3) 115e126. Maas, J., & Verheij, R. A. (2007). Are health benefits of physical activity in natural
Gobster, P. H., & Westphal, L. M. (2004). The human dimensions of urban green- environments used in primary care by general practitioners in The
ways: planning for recreation and related experiences. Landscape and Urban Netherlands? Urban Forestry and Urban Greening, 6, 227e233.
Planning, 68, 147e165. Maat, K., & De Vries, P. (2006). The influence of the residential environment on
González, A., & Amérigo, M. (2008). Relationship among values, beliefs, norms and green-space travel: testing the compensation hypothesis. Environment and
ecological behaviour. Psicothema, 20(4), 623e629. Planning A, 38, 2111e2127.
González, A. (2002). La preocupación por la calidad del medio ambiente. Un modelo Manzo, L. C. (2003). Beyond house and haven: toward a revisioning of emotional
cognitivo sobre la conducta ecológica. Tesis doctoral Universidad Complutense de relationships with places. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 47e61.
Madrid. Meyerhoff, J., & Liebe, U. (2006). Protest beliefs in contingent valuation: explaining
Grob, A. (1995). A structural model of environmental attitudes and behaviour. their motivation. Ecological Economics, 57, 583e594.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 209e220. Miele, M. & Parisi, V. (March 2000). Consumer concerns about animal welfare and
Grunert, K. G., & Grunert, S. C. (1995). Measuring subjective meaning structures by food choice. Italian report on laddering interviews.
the laddering method. Theoretical considerations and methodological prob- Mill, G. A., Van Rensburg, T. M., Hynes, S., & Dooley, C. (2007). Preferences for
lems. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12, 209e225. multiple use forest management in Ireland: citizen and consumer perspectives.
Gürlük, S. (2006). The estimation of ecosystem services’ value in the region of Misi Ecological Economics, 60, 642e653.
Rural Development Project: results from a contingent valuation survey. Forest Mitchell, R. C., & Carson, R. T. (1989). Using surveys to value public goods: The
Policy and Economics, 9, 209e218. contingent valuation method. Washington, D.C.: Resources for the Future/Johns
Gutman, J. (1982). A means-end chain model based on consumer categorization Hopkins University Press. 480 pp.
processes. Journal of Marketing, 46, 60e72. Mmopelwa, G., Kgathi, D. L., & Molefhe, L. (2007). Tourists` perceptions and their
Hanemann, W. M. (1984). Welfare evaluations in contingent valuation experiments willingness to pay for park fees: a case study of self-drive tourists and clients for
with discrete responses. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 66, mobile tour operators in Moremi Game Reserve, Botswana. Tourism Manage-
332e341. ment, 28, 1044e1056.
Hanemann, W. M. (1989). Welfare evaluations in contingent valuation experiments Múgica, M., & De Lucio, J. (1996). The role of on-site experience on landscape
with discrete response data: reply. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, preferences. a case study at Doñana National Park (Spain). Journal of Environ-
71(3), 332e341. mental Management, 47, 229e239.
Hanley, N., Ready, R., Colombo, S., Watson, F., Stewart, M., & Bergmann, E. A. (2009). Neuvomen, M., Sievänen, T., Tönnes, S., & Koskela, T. (2007). Access to green areas
The impacts of knowledge of the past experiences for future landscape change. and the frequency of visits e a case study in Helsinki. Urban Forestry and Urban
Journal of Environmental Management, 90, 1404e1412. Greening, 6, 235e237.
Hansmann, R., Hug, S.-M., & Seeland, K. (2007). Restoration and stress relief through Olson, J. C., & Reynolds, T. J. (1983). Understanding consumers’ cognitive structure:
physical activities in forests and parks. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening, 6, implications for advertising strategy. In L. Percy, & A. Woodside (Eds.), Adver-
213e225. tising and consumer psychology. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Harris, C. C., & Brown, G. (1992). Gain, loss, and personal responsibility: the role of Pedroso, C. M., Freitas, H., & Domingos, T. (2007). Testing for the survey mode effect
motivation in resource valuation decision-making. Ecological Economics, 5, 73e92. on contingent valuation data quality: a case study of web based versus in-
Hinkle, D. N. (1965). The change of personal constructs from the viewpoint of a theory person interviews. Ecological Economics, 62, 388e398.
of construct implications. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State Univer- Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2001). The role of desired and anticipated emotions in
sity, Columbus. goal-directed behaviours: broadening and deepening the theory of planned
Howard, J. A. (1977). Consumer behaviour: Application of theory. New York: behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(1), 79e98.
McGraw-Hill. Pieters, R., Baumgartner, H., & Allen, D. (1995). A means-end chain approach to
Jim, C. Y., & Chen, W. Y. (2006). Recreation-amenity use and contingent valuation of consumer goal structures. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12,
urban greenspaces in Guangzhou, China. Landscape and Urban Planning, 75, 227e244.
81e96. Pitts, R. E., Wong, J. K., & Whalen, D. J. (1991). Consumers’ evaluative structures in
Jones, N., Sophoulis, C., & Malesios, C. (2008). Economic valuation of coastal wáter two ethical situations: a means-end approach. Journal of Business Research, 22,
quality and protest responses: a case study in Mitilini, Greece. The Journal of 119e130.
Socio-Economics, 37, 2478e2491. Pooley, J. A., & O’Connor, M. (2000). Environmental education and attitudes: emotions
Jones, N., Evangelinos, K., Halvadakis, C. P., Iosifides, T., & Sophoulis, C. M. (2010). and beliefs are what is needed. Environment and Behaviour, 32(5), 711e724.
Social factors influencing perceptions and willingness top ay for a market-based Prada, B. A., González, G. M., Polomé, P., Gómez, M. J. M., & Vázquez, R. M. X. (2001).
policy aiming on solid waste management. Resources, Conservation and Recy- Valoración económica del patrimonio. Instituto de Estudios Económicos. Fun-
cling, 54, 533e540. damentos Pedro Barrié de la Maza.
N. López-Mosquera, M. Sánchez / Tourism Management 32 (2011) 875e889 889

Purcell, A. T. (1992). Abstract and specific physical attributes and the experience of to measuring means-end chain. International Journal of Research in Marketing,
landscape. Journal of Environmental Management, 34, 159e177. 15, 37e50.
Reynolds, T. J., & Gutman, J. (1988). Laddering theory, method, analysis and inter- Togridou, A., Hovardad, T., & Pantis, J. D. (2006). Determinants of visitors’ willing-
pretation. Journal of Advertising Research, 28, 11e31. ness to pay for the National Marine Park of Zakynthos, Greece. Ecological
Riera, P. (1994). Manual de valoración contingente. Madrid: Instituto de Estudios Economics, 60, 308e319.
Fiscales. Tyrväinen, L., & Väänänen, H. (1998). The economic value of urban forest amenities:
Rokeach, M. (1979). Understanding human values: Individual and societal. New York: an application of the contingent valuation method. Landscape and Urban
Free Press. Planning, 43, 105e118.
Russell, C. G., Flight, I., Leppard, P., Van Lawick, V. P., Syrette, J. A., & Cox, D. N. Tyrväinen, L., Mäikinen, K., & Schipperijn, J. (2007). Tools for mapping social values
(2004). A comparison of paper-and-pencil and computerized methods of hard of urban woodlands and other areas. Landscape and Urban Planning, 79, 5e19.
laddering. Food Quality and Preference, 15, 279e291. Tziakis, I., Pachiadakis, I., Moraitakis, M., Xideas, K., Theologis, G., & Tsagarakis, K. P.
Sanesi, G., & Chiarello, F. (2006). Residents and urban green spaces. The case of Bari. (2009). Valuing benefits from wastewater treatment and reuse using contingent
Urban Forestry and Urban Greening, 4, 125e134. valuation methodology. Desalination, 237, 117e125.
Sayadi, S., González-Roa, M. C., & Calatrava-Requena, J. (2009). Public preferences Velarde, M. D., Fry, G., & Tveit, M. (2007). Health effects of viewing landscapes e
for landscape features: the case of agricultural landscape in mountainous landscape types in environmental psychology. Urban Forestry and Urban
Mediterranean areas. Land Use Policy, 26(2), 334e344. Greening, 6, 199e212.
Silverman, J., & Klock, M. (1989). The behavior of respondents in contingent valuation: Venkatachalam, L. (2004). The contingent valuation method: a review. Environ-
evidence on starting bids. Journal of Behavioral Economics, 18(1), 51e60. mental Impact Assessment Review, 24, 89e124.
Scarpa, R., Hutchinson, W. G., Chilton, S. M., & Buongiorno, J. (2000). Importance of Verbic, M., & Slabe-Erker, R. (2008). An econometric analysis of willingness-to-pay
forests attributes in the willingness to pay for recreation: a contingent valuation for sustainable development: a case study of the Vol cji Potok landscape area.
study of Irish forests. Forest Policy and Economics, 1, 315e329. Ecological Economics, 68, 1316e1328.
Schultz, P. W. (2001). The structure of environmental concern: concern for self, Vesely, É.-T.. (2007). Green for green: the perceived value of a quantitative change
other people, and the biosphere. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, in the urban tree estate of New Zealand. Ecological Economics, 63, 605e615.
327e339. Walker, B. A., & Olson, J. C. (1991). Means-end chains: connecting products with self.
Sevenant, M., & Antrop, M. (2009). Cognitive attributes and aesthetic preferences in Journal of Business Research, 22(2), 111e119.
assessment and differentiation of landscapes. Journal of Environmental Williams, A., Davids, K., Burwitz, L., & Williams, J. (1992). Perception and action in
Management, 90, 2889e2899. sport. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 22, 147e204.
Smeesters, D., Warlop, L., Cornelissen, G., & Vanden Abeele, P. (2003). Consumer Williams, D. R., & Vaske, J. J. (2003). The measurement of place attachment: validity
motivation to recycle when recycling is mandatory: two explanatory studies. and generalisability of a psychometric approach. Forest Science, 49, 830e840.
Journal of Economy and Management, 48(3), 451e468. Winter, S. J., Prozesky, H., & Esler, K. J. (2007). A case study of landholder attitudes
Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of and behaviour toward the conservation of renosterveld, a critically endangered
Social Issues, 50(3), 65e84. vegetation type in Cape Floral Kingdom, South Africa. Environmental Manage-
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm ment, 40, 46e61.
theory of support for social movements, the case of environmental concern. Yilmaz, S., Zengin, M., & Yildiz, N. D. (2007). Determination of user profile at city
Human Ecology Review, 6(2), 81e97. parks: a sample from Turkey. Building and Environment, 42, 2325e2332.
Sudgen, R. (2005). Anomalies and stated preferences techniques: a framework for Young, S., & Feigin, B. (1975). Using the benefit chain for improved strategy
a discussion of coping strategies. Environmental and Resource Economics, 32,1e12. formulation. Journal of Marketing, 39, 72e74.
Ter Hofstede, F., Audenaert, A., Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M., & Wedel, M. (1998). An Zoppi, C. (2007). A multicriteria-contingent valuation analysis concerning a coastal
investigation into the association pattern technique as a quantitative approach area of Sardinia, Italy. Land Use Policy, 24, 322e337.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai