Anda di halaman 1dari 12

This article was downloaded by: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia]

On: 14 March 2011


Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 781191584]
Publisher Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-
41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Journal of Environmental Education


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t914957653

Environmental Knowledge, Attitudes, and Behavior in Dutch Secondary


Education
Hans Kuhlemeier; Huub Van Den Bergh; Nijs Lagerweij

Online publication date: 31 March 2010

To cite this Article Kuhlemeier, Hans , Bergh, Huub Van Den and Lagerweij, Nijs(1999) 'Environmental Knowledge,
Attitudes, and Behavior in Dutch Secondary Education', The Journal of Environmental Education, 30: 2, 4 — 14
To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/00958969909601864
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00958969909601864

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses
should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,
actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly
or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
The Journal of Environmental Education, 1999, Vol. 30, No. 2, 4-14

Environmental Knowledge,
Attitudes, and Behavior in Dutch
Secondary Education
HANS KUHLEMEIER, HUUB VAN DEN BERGH, and NIJS LAGERWEIJ
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

ABSTRACT In the Dutch National Assessment Program, environmental knowledge,


environmental attitudes, and environmentally responsible behavior were studied in a
nationwide sample of more than 9,000 students (aged f 15 years) from 206 secondary
schools. Fifty-seven percent of the 9th-grade students had a (very) positive attitude
toward the environment, and 35% were prepared to take extra pains or to make
(financial) sacrifices for the environment. The students' knowledge about environ-
mental problems was fragmentary and often incorrect, however. Similarly, the envi-
ronmentally responsible behavior of many of the students was inadequate. The rela-
tion between environmental knowledge and environmental attitudes and behavior
proved to be very weak. There was a substantial relation between environmental atti-
tude, willingness to make personal sacrifices, and environmentally responsible
behavior. Consistent with theories on attitudes, environmentally responsible behav-
ior was more strongly connected with willingness to make sacrifices than with atti-
tude toward the environment.

I n the Netherlands, environmental education is a new and


still relatively little dealt with subject. Because of a lack
of suitable teaching materials, Dutch teachers seldom pay
Hausbeck, Milbrath, & Enright, 1992; Hendnks & van der
Zande, 1990; Munson, 1994). Little nationally representa-
tive research has as yet been carried out in the Netherlands
systematic attention, for example, to the interaction be- into the actual environmental knowledge of students; simi-
tween man and the environment, the causes of environmen- larly, not a great deal is known about students' attitudes
tal problems, or potential solutions. The environmental toward the environment and their ecological behavior. The
knowledge that students already possess is likely to be Dutch government considers environmental education to be
determined by topicality and to be fragmentary or incorrect, one of the instruments for establishing a more durable rela-
or both (Eberg, Eijkelhof, Kortland, & Stokking, 1991; tion between man and the environment. Accordingly, the
government promulgated goals in the field of environmen-
~
tal knowledge, insight, and skills in the National Environ-
Hans Kuhlemeier is a senior researcher at the National mental Policy Plan (Nationaal Milieubeleidsplan, 1989) and
Institute .for Educational Measirrement (Cito). Huub van also expressed its intent to persuade students to have more
den Bergh is an assistant professor at the Utrecht Institute positive attitudes with respect to the environment and even-
of Linguistics of the Universitv of Utrecht. Nijs hgenveij is tually to behave more responsibly. The underlying idea is
a researcher at the Faculty of Educational Science of the that students who know a lot about the environment have a
Uni\,ersity of' Amsterdam. positive attitude toward it and are likely to behave in an en-
vironmentally responsible manner.
4
Kuhlemeier. van den Bergh, and Lagenveij i

To what extent does that assumption have empirical sup- for analogous reasons, willingness 10 make \acrifict-s
port'! Does more environmental knowledge indeed lead to a should not have a lower correlation with attitude toward tlic
positive attitude toward the environment-as many assume environment than with environmental knowledge. Third.
i t docs? Doea a positive attitude with respect to the envi- because knowledge and attitudes affect behavior oiily
ronment result in environmentally responsible behavior, or through behavioral intentions, environmentally responsible
arc the two entirely unconnected? behavior should not have a lower correlation with willing-
ness to make sacrifices than with environmental attitude o r
Theoretical Framework environmental knowledge.
A theoretical framework within which those kinds of Because the second hypothesis still provides rlither weah
questions can be studied was developed by Fishbein and evidence for Ajzen and Fishbein's theory (1980; Fishbein B
A j m i (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). Ajzen, 1975). we could also pose the restriction Ihiit thcl-c
I n their theory of reasoned action. they distinguished among should be mutually higher correlations between directlj
f o u r hasic concepts: beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and related aspects than between all the other. indirectly rcIatcc1
behavior. Holii$s involve knowledge or opinions concerning aspects. In the third, much more easily rejectable hypothe-
thc attitude object; uttifiides involve emotions and evalua- sis, we assumed that the direct connections are strongest.
tions with respect to that object; intentions refers to behav- In this article. we describe Dutch ninth-grade secondary
ioral itinis: and hehuiior involves the actual action itself. school students' environmental knowledge, attitudcs. and
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

Fishbein and Ajzen ( 1975; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980)pos- behavior. Furthermore, we examine the hypotheses discusacd
tuluted ;I specilic pattern of effect relations among the four earlier as we explore the relation between environmental
components. In their view, for instance, actual behavior is, knowledge, environmental attitude, willingness to milkc xic-
tirst. ;I function of behavioral intentions, and second, one of rifices, and environmentally responsible behavior. After di\-
:illittides that. in turn. are affected by knowledge. A critical cussing our results, we make several recommendationa.
is sump ti on in their theory is that knowledge and attitudes
influence actual behavior only through behavioral inten- Method
tions ( A j m i & Fishbein, 1980; Cooper & Croyle, 1984; Participants and Collection of Data
Hines. Hungerford, & Tomera, 1986-1987; Ryan & Bon-
An environmental study was conducted in I992 ;is part id'
lield. 1975). According to Ajzen and Fishbein, behavioral
the National Assessment of Educational Achievement Pro-
intentions ;ire the best predictor of actual behavior, better
gram in the Netherlands. In the geography assessniciit
t h m the attitude and knowledge components.
(Kuhlemeier et al., 1994), a two-staged cluster saniple was
I n line with the theory of Fishbein and Ajzen (1975;
drawn. In the first stage, 206 schools were selected. with ;I
A,j/en & Fishhein. 1980). at least four aspects of the topic
sampling probability proportional to the number 01' nintli-
o f the environment can be distinguished: knowledge and
grade students. In the second stage. students were s;itiiplc*tl
opinions concerning the environment, attitude toward the
within schools. Using a matrix sampling design. we riiii-
cnvironment. willingness to make personal sacrifices for the
domly assigned the environmental instruments t o the atti-
ciivironmcnt (the behavioral intention), and environmental-
dents. Of the total of more than 9,000 participants. YPi
l y responsible behavior. On the basis of Fishbein and
were higher general education students, 40% were interiiic-
A.jzen's theory. we formulated three, incremental hypothe-
diate general education students, and 2 I % were lower voc+
aes cmcerning the relations between knowledge. attitude.
tional training students. The student population sample coii-
willingness to make sacrifices, and behavior.
tained slightly more girls ( 5 2 % ) than boys (48%).
First, knowledge of environmental problems. attitude
Not every student was presented with each instrutnciit.
toward the environment, willingness to make sacrifices, and
Consequently, data from considerably fewer students were
environmentally responsible behavior in everyday life
available for the calculation of the correlation between instru-
\hould be positively related. Because we made no require-
ments than were available for the calculation o f the tiit'iiiis
tiicnta a s to the nature of the correlation, this first hypothe-
and the variances for each instrument separately. The numhcr
\ i s may be hard to reject.
of students for each combination of instruments varied troiii
I n the second hypothesis, we included the effect relations
approximately 2.000 for the correlation between eiiviroii-
postulated hy Fishbein and Ajzen ( 1975; Ajzen & Fishbein.
mental knowledge and the remaining instruments to ahotit
1980): Directly related aspects should not have a lower cor-
5.500 for the relation between environmentul attitude atitl
relation than should other aspects interlinked only indirect-
willingness to make sacrifices; the number o f schools wax at
ly through yet other aspects. At least three criteria for the
least 205 for all instruments, however.
accond hypothesis were formulated. First, according to
Fishbein and Ajzen. knowledge affects actual behavior only Instruments
through attitude and behavioral intentions; therefore, envi-
rontnentiil knowledge should not have a lower correlation Environmental Kno~Jedge
with environmental attitude than with willingness to make Insight into the interaction between human\ and tlic c i i \ I
aacri licea o r environmentally responsible behavior. Second, ronnient and into the mechanisms that underlie e m iroii
6 The Journal of Environmental Education

mental problems is an extremely complex subject that may spare time for the environment” and “I am prepared to pay
be viewed from various perspectives and disciplines. We did a little more for an environmentally friendly product.”
not attempt to measure environmental knowledge in its
entirety and depth. Instead, in this study, the concept of Etivitonmentally Responsible Behavior
environmental knowledge was restricted to knowledge of The domain of environmentally responsible behavior is
environmental problems, for instance, to those areas that very broad and heterogeneous (Schahn & Holzer, 1990:
have been the focus of the media and the educational mate- Ungar, 1994). We measured the degree to which studcnts
rial of the government. Moreover, environmental problems behave in an environment-friendly or -unfriendly way i n
were looked at from a geographical point of view. daily life by using a questionnaire containing 20 questions.
The environmental knowledge test consisted of 80 items. Eight were Likert-type questions, and 12 allowed the stu-
The distribution of items across the various themes and dent to choose more alternatives simultaneously. The
;ireas was as follows’: Eleven of the 30 items dealt with soil, behavioral items related to behavior relevant to ninth-grade
water. iind air pollution; 6 were about waste processing and students. We chose those items from various environment
recycling: 4 about farming and market gardening; 4 about guides, some of them specifically intended for secondnry
transportation, tourism, and recreation; 3 about energy us- school students (e.g., Reijnders, 1991: Silver & Vdlcly.
age: and 1 about government regulations; the distribution 1991). We were also guided by information from the
across the three regions-the Netherlands, Europe, and the instructional campaign of the government (Ministcrie,
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

world-comprised 12, 3, and 14 items, respectively. 1991). The focus of the questions was on environmentally
Each item was a statement about an environmental prob- friendly or unfriendly behavior that has a certain reality f o r
lem. The student had to indicate whether or not the state- students, that could really be exercised by them in daily life.
ment was true. From a pilot study carried out in 88 schools and in which environmental considerations can play ;I role.
(SSO students), it was evident that the environmental knowl- For instance, no questions pertained to the purchase o r thc
edge of many ninth-grade students was poor. That justified use of phosphate-free detergents, dishwashers, tumble dry-
the addition of a third response category: I really do not ers, cars, and other consumer goods that are indicative o f
h ~If ,a student
. really did not have any idea about the cor- the ecological behavior of adults. The questions pertained
rcctncss of ;I statement, the third response category could be only to the environmentally responsible behavior of the s t u -
chosen. Three characteristic items read as follows: “Widely dent, as an energy user, a consumer, or a garbage produccr.
purchased potato types such as binrje and eigenheimer are Only observable, concrete behavior was chosen. The stu-
usually cultivated without chemical pesticides,” “The water dents’ underlying motives were left out of consideration. N o
o f the Rhine i n the Netherlands has become somewhat questions were asked about the environmental considern-
clcaner in recent years.“ and “Most cars consume the same tions that might be taken into account when students pur-
quantity of petrol at a velocity of 90 kilometers per hour as chase or use certain products, such as (disposable) batteries
;it I20 kilometers per hour.” or (recycled) paper. We did that to avoid contamination by
socially desirable responses as much as possible.
Attitirdr and Willingness to Make Sac,rijkes
I~ii~~ir(itiiii~Jiitci1

We measured attitudes toward the environment and the Reliability


willingness to make personal sacrifices by means of an adapt- Before describing our findings, we review some tiic;i-
ecl version of questionnaires developed by Maloney and Ward surement properties (see Table I ) .
( 1973). Maloney, Ward, and Braught (19751, and Nelissen, The reliability* of the environmental knowledge test was
Pcrcnbooni. Peters. and Peters ( 1987). The instrument con- .73, which is reasonable for a test consisting of 30 items.
s i s t h ol‘ 2 0 Likert-type statements. Each of the subscales- The internal consistency of the environmental attitude
environmental attitude and willingness to make sacrifices- scale in its entirety was 3 5 . The estimates for the’separiite
is represented by 10 statements. The students indicated on a subscales of environmental attitude and willingness to makc
4-point scale their agreement with each of the statements. sacrifices were obviously a little lower because of the sninll-
The subscale rnvironmrntal uttitude included statements er number of statements: .75 and .79. respectively.
that pertain to the degree of environmental inclination The internal consistency of the environmentally respon-
(environment-mindedness), environmental concern. envi- sible behavior scale was .7 1. That value is not unsatisfacto-
ronmental indifference, and denial of environmental prob- ry for a scale with 20 widely divergent behavioral items
Icms. Two characteristic statements were “Things will sure- (van der Meer, 1980, 1981). In a reasonably reliable way.
ly not take that turn with the pollution of the environment” one can measure the degree to which students exhibit envi-
;incl “Owing to pollution of the environment, the world ronment-friendly behavior in their daily life by using that
threatens to become unfit to live in for future generations.” brief questionnaire. The question of validity-the extent t o
The subscale rc*illingness t o make sucrt~cesconcerns which we truly measured environmentally responsible
rcudiness to make an extra effort for the environment, to behavior with a paper-and-pencil test-is, of course. c o n -
take extra pains, or to make extra sacrifices. Two character- pletely different and can be answered only in part on thc
istic statements were “I would not mind sacrificing a little basis of our study.
Kuhlemeier, van den Bergh, and Lagenveij

TABLE 1. Some Descriptive Statistics for Environmental Knowledge, Attitude,


Willingness to Make Sacriflces, and Behavior

Possible Observed No. of No. of Reliability


Scale M sn range range students items

Knowledge 17.91 4.53 0 to 30 0 to 30 5,803 30 .73 .03


Attitude 31.15 4.36 10 to40 10 to40 5,778 10 .75 .03
Sacrifice 28.89 4.64 10 to40 10 to 40 5,739 10 .79 .02
Behavior 6.82 5.47 -20 to 20 -18 to 20 5,173 20 .71 .02

The scores on the 30 questions were recoded in such a deviations of the students' scores from the school means
way that the score 1 represented environmentally friendly were estimated in a between-students covariance matrix.
hehavior; the score 0, neutral behavior; and the score -1, Because four instruments were used, four variances and six
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

environmentally unfriendly behavior. The range of scores covariances were estimated. The covariances at the student
possible. in theory, was therefore between -20 and 20, level represented the correlation between the scores of mi-
where the scores -20 and 20 indicated extremely environ- dents within schools. At the school level, the deviations of
mentally unfriendly and extremely environmentally friend- the school means from the population mean were estimated
ly behavior, respectively. The center of the scale (the score in a between-schools covariance matrix. That matrix also
0) was considered neutral. contained 10 elements. The covariances at the school level
I t should be noted that we proceeded with caution in provided insight into the correlation between the school
defining environmental friendliness. We counted even rela- means for the four instruments. By dividing the estimate of
tively polluting behavior as an indication of environmental the between-schools variance by that of the total varianct..
friendliness. For instance, in the questions regarding eco- an intraclass correlation rho was obtained (Kish, 1965) that
nomical energy usage, only those students who declared signified how large or small the differences between
that they (almost) never use tap water, lighting, or heating schools were proportionally. For the sake of the comparn-
economically were counted among the environmentally bility of the effect estimations, the scores were standardized
unfriendly. In the question on the packaging of milk, soft for each instrument separately, with a zero mean and unity
drink\, and so on, we considered not only the use of glass variance.
hottlts as environmentally friendly but also the use of paper
Results
cartons ( i n contrast to plastic bottles and cans).
A behavioral index may become invalid if not all of the Environmental Knowledge
hehavorial activities included are accessible to all respon- The students' environmental knowledge is shown i n Table
dents (Scott & Willits, 1994). Some questions in our study I . The average student selected the correct answer for I X 01'
were not applicable for a substantial number of the students. the 30 items. When the third response category (I red/! dolt 'I
I f a student never goes shopping, for instance, he or she, of know) was counted as wrong, the degree of difticulty of thc
course. cannot behave in an environmentally friendly or items varied from .I7 to .92. with a mean of .60.The distrib-
unfriendly way with respect to that activity. In instances in ution of the test scores is shown in Table 2. The 15% highest
which the behavioral activities were inaccessible, the stu- scoring students achieved a score of 23 or higher. The 17!1
dents concerned were not counted among the environmen- lowest scoring students answered, at most, I 3 of the 30 item
tiilly unfriendly but were assigned to the neutral category. correctly; scores such as those would be expected i t the stu-
dents selected the answers randomly. From that finding, we
Analysis tentatively concluded that the environmental knowledge 01'
One can study the relations between knowledge, attitude, many students left much to be desired.
willingness to make sacrifices, and behavior in a multivari- That conclusion became more forceful when the degree
ate multilevel model (Goldstein, 1987; Goldstein & of attraction of the third response category was scrutinized,
McDonald, 1988; Longford & MuthCn, 1992) by using the It amounted to .18. on average, over all 30 items. Only 13%
ML3 program (Prosser. Rasbash, & Goldstein, 1991). The of the students did not select I real/? do not k n o w a single
cluster effects caused by the sampling procedure are taken time, 76% selected it I to 10 times, and the rest ( I 1%') did
into account explicitly. A two-level variance component so at least 1 I times. Apparently, many of the ninth-grade
model, with students nested within schools, was fitted to the students were uncertain whether or not the statements on
data. I n the fixed part of the model, the mean scores on the environmental topics presented to them were correct.
four instruments were estimated. At the student level, the The knowledge of the students varied quite a hit betwc.cn
S The Jourtial of Environmental Education

very positive. whereas the environmental attitude 01' 47'


'IAB1.E 2. Distribution of Scores for Environmental could be characterized as neutral. According to o u r delitii~
Knowledge tion. only I% of the students had a weakly t o very wcabl!
developed negative environmental attitude. They did not ( ;it
K'lllp! (11 N o . of Percentage of all), for example. agree with the statcment, "Polluting
\LXIlC\ ktudents students industry should be dealt with more harshly." whereah t h q
did agree (entirely) with "Acid rain is receiving an exccssi\~*
0 I0 33 I ti amount of attention" and "That species of plants ;ire hcconi
I I I3 ti27 II
ing extinct is all the same to me."
I4 I6 1.126 19
17-10 1,387 26 When we looked at the distribution of the scores for will-
20-22 I3 5I 2.3 ingness to make sacrifices. the second scale. we found t l i i i t
23 3 0 xx I 15 60%.of the ninth-grade students scored somewhere neiir thc
center of the scale. That group appeared to be little prep;ireii
to make an extra effort for the environment. That tinding
was true to an even greater extent for the 4%. who scored i t 1
'IAHLE 3. Distribution of Scores for Environmental the lower reaches of the scale: Probably n o willingnchx
Attitude and Willingness to Make Sacrifices
whatever to contribute to the environment can be assumccl
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

for them. For 3S% of the students, o n the other hand. the
Kaiipc (11' bcorc\ No. of Percentage of willingness to make sacrifices was strongly to very strong-
students students ly developed. They agreed entirely with statements such ;ix
"1 do not mind sacrificing a little spare time for the cn\i-
thirontiit'ntal ronment," "I want to use electricity o n l y when i t is re;iIl!
;iltitudc
11-20 75 I
necessary," or "1 am quite prepared to pay ii little tnort' I O I
2 1-30 2.4OX 42 an environmentally friendly product."
.1 I -4 ) 3.295 57
Williiigiiess lo Environmentally Responsible Behavior
iii;ihe s x r i lice\ The data on environmentally responsible behat.J I O I ;Ire
I0 - 2 0 24 1 3
? I 30 3.464 ti0 presented in Table I. The mean score on the environmental
3 I -40 2,034 35 ly responsible behavior scale amounted to nciirly 7 (on ;I
scale that ranged from -20 to 20). That mean lay too 1;ir
beyond the center of the scale (the score 0) to be consicleretl
as neutral. We therefore could have concluded that the eco-
itenis. I n Appendix A several items that were mastered rel- logically responsible behavior of the students was not s o
ati\,cly well iind several i t e m that were mastered relatively bad after all. However, it is noteworthy that the mean was
~ x ) o I l y;ire discussed. obtained after recoding the scores to the 20 questions and
that we proceeded with caution in defining environnient;il
Environmental Attitude and friendliness. The distribution of the scores is shown i n Tahlc
Willingness to Make Sacrifices 4.In that table, one finds that 4% of the students were i n ttw
A student could score a minimum of 10 and a maximum lowest reaches of the scale (a scorc of 4 or lower). 'They
of 40 points on the scales for environmental attitude and stated that they paid little attention to the enviroiitnetit.
u-illingncss to make sacrifices.>Assuming that a mean score More than one fifth of the students werc somewhcrc ne;ii'
of 15. corresponding to the center of the scale, represented the center of the scale (range of scores -3 to 3). Their znvi-
;I iicutral attitude. the data certainly did not allow us to con- ronmentally responsible behavior could be chnructerixd ;I\
clude that the average student in our study had a negative neutral, in the sense that environmentally friendly behnvior
attilude toward the environment. On the contrary. when we and environmentally unfriendly behavior were rensonahly
considered the nieans in Table I , we noted that these were well balanced. Nearly half of the students were i n the catc-
diatiiiclly higher than the middle score category (31 and 29, gory 4 to 10. For those students, environmentally fricndly
rL-xpcctivcIy 1. behavior was exhibited more often than envirotimcn~nll~
What proportions of our students had a negative, neutral, unfriendly behavior. According t o our standard. the hchnv-
or positive environmental attitude'? To answer that question, ior of approximately a quarter of the students c;in I>c
w e \cp;ir;ited the distribution of scores into three categories: described as (extremely) environmentally friendly. I n ;it
htudcnts who scored 20 or lower, students who scored least I I of the 20 environmentally responsible form\ ol
hctween 21 and 30, and those who scored higher than 30 behavior, they opted. either consciously or not, for il nioclc
( ~ L YTable 3). of behavior that taxes the environment relatively less. That
From Table 3. one can see that S7% of the students had does not alter the fact that a lot of things still need i n l i m n -
; i n attitude toward the environment ranging from positive to ing i n the environmentally responsible behavior o f large
Kuhlemeier, van den Bergh. and Lagenveij 9

In conformance with the second and third hypotheses.


TABLE 4. Distribution of Scores for Environmentally aspects between which a direct relation is postulated should
Responsible Behavior
be correlated at least equally highly or more highly with one
another than with other, only indirectly related aspects. The
Range of No. of Percentage of correlations in Table 5 should exhibit a so-called simp/i~.r
scores students students structure (Guttman, 1954). In other words, they ought to
become lower as one moves further away from the diagonal.
-18 to -4 I98 4
We explored by means of a multiple comparison procedure
-3 to 3 1,145 22
4 to 10 2,468 48 (Goldstein, 1987, p. 29) whether the correlation matrix in its
I I to IS 1,136 22 entirety showed such a pattern. That indeed appeared to be
I6 to 20 226 4 the case (pc .001). Not every paired comparison was signif-
icant, however. Corresponding with the second hypothesis,
environmental knowledge did not have a lower correlation
groups of students. Only 4% achieved the maximum possi- with environmental attitude (r = .26) than with the willing-
ble score or the score directly below that. ness to make sacrifices ( r = .21) or with environmentally
In the context of this article, we did not consider it useful responsible behavior ( r = .20). The third hypothesis, howev-
to discuss at length the responses to all the questions on the er, according to which immediate relations should he
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

environmentally responsible behavior scale. For a descrip- stronger than indirect ones, was not confirmed here. Emi-
tion of the findings that were most conspicuous, refer to ronmental knowledge did not have a higher correlation with
Appendix B. environmental attitude than with willingness to make sacti-
fices or with environmentally responsible behavior.
Differences Between Schools and Between Students In accordance with the third hypothesis, willingness to
The between- and within-school variances of environ- make sacrifices was more strongly connected with environ-
mental knowledge, environmental attitude, willingness to mental attitude (r = .54) than with environmental knowl-
make sacrifices, and ecologically responsible behavior are edge (r = .2 I ) . Finally, environmentally responsible behav-
shown on the diagonals of Table 5 . ior had a significantly stronger correlation with willingness
The between-schools variances deviated from zero sig- to make sacrifices (r = .56) than with environmental attitude
nificantly for both environmental knowledge, environmen- (r = .36) or with environmental knowledge (r = .20). C h i
tal attitude, willingness to make sacrifices, and environ- average, the correlation between directly related aspects
mentally responsible behavior. Schools differed greatly in was .20 higher than that between the indirectly related
average knowledge of environmental problems (p = .21).4 aspects. In short, apart from the relatively weak relations
The differences, in terms of environmental attitude, willing- with the knowledge test, the correlational pattern appeared
ness to make sacrifices, and environmentally responsible to be roughly in accordance with predictions of the theory
behavior, may well be significant, but were of less impor- on the effect relations between knowledge, attitude, behav-
tance here (ps = .09, .07, and .09, respectively). ioral intention, and behavior (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fish-
bein & Ajzen, 1975). Thus, support was obtained for thc
Correlation Between Knowledge, (discriminant) validity of using the four instruments tor
Attitudes, and Behavior measurements with students.
The correlations between environmental knowledge, The correlations between the school means are presented
environmental attitude, willingness to make sacrifices, and in the upper part of Table 5 . All correlations deviated from
environmentally responsible behavior at the school and stu- zero significantly. The correlation, which can be described.
dent level are also shown below the diagonals in Table 5. without exception, as high to very high, provided contirma-
The correlations at the student level are presented in the tion for our first hypothesis: A more positive environmental
lower part of the table. All the correlations between the stu- attitude (r = .88), greater willingness to make Sacrifices 0.=
dent scores were significant and in the expected direction. 34). and environmentally friendlier behavior ( r = .78) were
When evaluating the magnitude of the correlations in the found in schools whose students knew relatively more about
light of our first hypothesis, we observed that not all rela- environmental problems than in schools whose students
tions were substantial. The relations assumed by many were less knowledgable. At schools whose students indiciit-
between the knowledge of environmental problems, on the ed that they take the environment into account to a greater
one hand, and environmental attitude (r = .26), willingness extent, on average, more willingness to make sacrifices ( r =
to make sacrifices (r = .21), and environmentally responsi- .91). a more positive environmental attitude (I’= 8 I ), and
ble behavior ( r = .20), on the other, were so low that no more knowledge of environmental problems ( r = .78) were
practical meaning could be attached to them5 Students who also expressed, Note that that correlation profile does not
possessed a great deal of environmental knowledge hardly seem contrary to the effect relations between knowledge.
seemed to be distinguishable by attitudes and behavior from attitude, behavioral intentions, and behavior postulated hy
other students. Fishbein and Ajzen (1980; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1975). On
The Journal of Environmental Education

TABLE 5. Between- and Within-School Variances and Correlationsfor


Environmental Knowledge, Attitude, Willingness to Make Sacrifices, and Behavior

Variance Knowledge Attitude Sacrifice Behavior

Between-schools (N= 206)


Knowledge 4.46
Attitude .88 (.12) 1.65
Sacrifice 3 4 (.12) .91 (.14) 1.55
Behavior .78 ( . I 1) .81 (.12) .91 (.13) 2.83
Between-students (N= 9,422)
Knowledge 16.52
Attitude .26 (.02) 17.54
Sacrifice .21 (.02) .54 (.02) 20.05
Behavior .20 (.02) .36 (.02) .56 (.02) 27.30
lntraclass correlation Rho .21 .09 .07 .oo
Note. Observed variances are on the diagonal. Below are correlations, and standard errors are between
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

brackets.

TABLE 6. Correlations Before and After Correction for Unreliability

Knowledge Attitude Sacrifice Behavior


r SE r SE r SE r SE

Knowledge .33 .02 .25 .02 .27 .02


Attitude .44 .02 .55 .02 .40 .02
Sacrifice .32 .02 .69 .o 1 .58 .02
Behavior .36 .02 .53 .02 .76 .o 1
Note. Correlations before are shown above the diagonal, and correlations after are shown below.

average, the correlation was .09 higher between directly Actual Environmental Knowledge,
related aspects than between indirectly related aspects. On Attitudes, and Behavior
the surface, the expected simplex structure was apparent: Knowledge of environmental problems seemed to be
The further away from the diagonal, the lower the correla- weakly developed in many of the students. Large groups of
tions appeared to be. We used significance testing to deter- students lacked knowledge regarding environmental topics
mine whether the correlation matrix at the school level did such as energy usage; soil, air, and water pollution; recy-
indeed exhibit such a structure. This time, however, it did cling; agricultural activities; tourism; transportation; and
not (p = .986): The differences between the correlations recreation. Many also seemed to be badly informed about
were not significant. Thus, partial support was found for the the minor successes of the environmentalist movement in
validity of using the four environmental instruments in the Western Europe. For instance, very few students were
measurement of schools. aware that the water of the Rhine has become a little clean-
er in recent years, that the emission of sulfur dioxide has
Discussion
decreased in the Netherlands, and that the use of lead-free
In this study, we have shown that the environmental gasoline is beginning to bear fruit. That is regrettable,
knowledge, attitudes, and behavior of ninth-grade students because such information can help reduce attitudes of envi-
in secondary education can be measured in a relatively reli- ronmental powerlessness and environmental defeatism.
able way. Furthermore, the pattern of correlations gave us The lack of environmental knowledge did not seem to pre-
no reason to seriously question the validity of the instru- vent students from caring about the environment. Given their
ments. Apart from the near absence of a relation between scores on the environmental attitude scale, 57% of the stu-
environmental knowledge and the other instruments, all dents had, according to our definition, a positive to very pos-
relations were substantial and certainly not contrary to what itive environmental attitude. They agreed on the seriousness
might be expected on the basis of relevant theories. of the environmental problems, expressed concern about
Kuhlemeier. van den Bergh, and Lagerweij II

endangered plant and animal species, and had a positive atti- Relations
tude toward the protection of nature reserves, among other
things. The conclusions to be drawn about their willingness A substantial relation between knowledge of environ-
to make sacrifices are a little less favorable. According to our mental problems, on the one hand, and attitudes and behuv-
definition, there appeared to be a reasonable to very high ior, on the other, could not be demonstrated in our study.
degree of willingness to take extra pains for the environment That finding is somewhat surprising, for what is more PliiLt-
in 3S% of the students, indicated by their willingness to make sible than the assumption that students with more knowl-
iin extra effort and to make financial sacrifices. They
edge of environmental problems care about the environ-
expressed, for example, the intention to deal with energy ment more, are more prepared to put up with financial
more economically (more often), to collect paper for recy- sacrifices, and take the environment into account more i n
cling, to pay a little more for environmentally friendly prod- everyday life? Various explanations can underlie thiit find-
ucts, and to work harder for a cleaner environment. ing. Possibly there was indeed no clear relation in our pop-
The average ninth-grade student achieved a score of 7 on ulation between knowledge, on the one hand, and attitude
the environmentally responsible behavior scale, with a and behavior, on the other, or perhaps environmentally
range of scores from -20 to 20. According to our standard, responsible behavior cannot be measured properly by
the behiivior of approximately a quarter of the students can means of a questionnaire. It is difficult to give 11 clciir
be described as (extremely) environmentally unfriendly to answer to these questions on the basis of our exploratory
study. For that, further research is necessary. In addition. wc
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

neutral. In about half of the environmentally responsible


forms of behavior investigated, they selected, either con- should not be too optimistic about the correspondence
sciously or not, a mode of behavior that burdens the envi- between what people say and what they really do. Scott and
ronment to a relatively large extent. After comparing the Willits (1994) put it this way: “The observed low a t t i -
observed environmentally responsible behavior with the tude-behavior linkage may be less a result of questioning
recommendations of the Dutch government (Ministerie, wording or measurement error than a real disparity betwccn
1991), we concluded that there is still much room for words and deeds” (p. 255).
improvement. In our study, evidence was found for another comnion
sense opinion-that environmental attitude, willingness to
Differences Between Schools make sacrifices, and environmentally responsible behavior
The schools differed in terms of the average knowledge are correlated. Although the relations found were stronger
of environmental problems of the students. Various expla- than in most other studies (Hines et al., 1986-1987). the
nations can be given for those differences, for example, the correlation pattern raises a number of questions. For exani-
composition of the student population (prior achievement, ple, why was the correlation between willingness to make
family background); the enthusiasm, experience, and com- sacrifices and environmentally responsible behavior s o
petence of the team of teachers; the cumcular offering; the low-no higher than .56? Why didn’t students with a posi-
quality of instruction; and the school climate (Gamoran & tive attitude and a high degree of willingness to make sacri-
Nystrand, 1994; Kreft, 1987; Willms, 1992). The fact that fices put their behavioral intentions into practice more
the differences between schools were greater for environ- often? One of the many possible explanations is that they
mental knowledge than for environmental attitude, willing- just do not have sufficient knowledge of the consequenccs
ness to make sacrifices, and environmentally responsible of their actions on the environment (Barry, 1990; Ungilr.
behavior requires an explanation. Because environmental 1994). Were they willing but lacking the know-how’?Envi-
problems are never taught or are seldom dealt with in Dutch ronmental education might help the students by providing
secondary education, it is unlikely that the between-schools them with knowledge of and skill in using environmental
differences in environmental knowledge were the result of action strategies (Sia, Hungerford, & Tomera, 1985- 1986).
differences in instructional quality. In the Netherlands, the It is particularly important to stress the link between envi-
allocation of students to school types and tracks during and ronmental problems and students’ personal lifestyles
after the transition from primary to secondary education (Gigliotti, 1992) and to raise the students’ awareness of the
largely takes place on the basis of the knowledge and skills environmental choices they face as residents, consumers,
of the students (aspects that correlated more strongly with garbage producers, and travelers.
environmental knowledge than with environmental attitudes A second partial explanation for the absence of a perfect
and behavior). The type of school is consequently partly a correlation between willingness to make sacrifices and
surrogate (proxy) for differences between schools in the environmentally responsible behavior was found in the
selection of their students. One could then perfectly well dependency relation generally found between school-age
interpret the relative magnitude of the between-schools dif- students and their parents. For instance, students who do the
ferences on the knowledge test as an effect of differences shopping for their parents are not, of course, entirely at lib-
between types of training in the cognitive level of the stu- erty to buy those products that involve the least damage to
dent population, partially as a consequence of differential the environment. They might be willing but not allowed to
enrollment (selection). purchase appropriate products.
12 The Journal of Environmental Education

A third possible explanation can be inferred from the pat- ly better than a model with only one indivisible environmental attitude fac-
tor. In that way, empirical support was found for the a priori arrangement
tern of correlations between attitude, willingness to make of the statements and the distinguishability of environmental attitude arid
sacritices, and behavior. Environmental attitude was more willingness to make sacrifices.
highly correlated with willingness to make sacrifices ( r = 4. For the subject of geography, the intraclass correlation, based o n cur-
riculum-valid tests of more than 600 multiple choice and constructed
5 4 ) than with actual environmentally responsible behavior response items, was 5 1 (Kuhlemeier el al.. 1994).
i n day-to-day life ( r = .36). Many students seemed to man- 5. The correlations reported in Table 5 were not corrected for unrelia-
age reasonably well in translating a high environmental atti- bility. In that connection. it is not inconceivable that the correlation wil\
higher in reality. The correlations, after correction for unreliahility, per-
tude into environmentally friendly behavioral intentions. formed by means of LISREL (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1989) on the total
When it came to putting behavioral intentions themselves covariance matrix. were indeed higher, but the pattern of correlationa re-
into practice, however, a part of the noble but noncommittal mained roughly constant (Table 6).
environmental motives diminished rather soon (Gigliotti,
1994).In principle, a individual might truly want to take the
REFERENCES
environment into account, but that person might prefer not
to be one of the first to do so, particularly if others do not, Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding urtitudes and prdit.tiir,q
if he or she has to make a great effort, or if that effort social behavior: Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Barry. J. M. (1990). Environmental awareness: How green must we he”
entails extra expenses. In short, in addition to environmen- Journal of Environmental Health, 53(2). 62.
tal knowledge and concern, psychological, social, and eco- Cooper, J.. & Croyle, R. T. (1984). Attitudes and attitude change. Atrriictrl
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

nomic considerations appear to play a part in the realization Review of Psychology, 35,395426.
Eberg, J. W., Eijkelhof. H. M. C., Kortland. J., & Stokking, K. M. ( 199 I ).
of environnientally responsible behavior (e.g., Hines et al., Naar een didactiek voor natuur- en milieu-educ~utiein the undenrij.\
19116- 1987). [Toward an approach for science and environmental education]. Utrrcht.
The Netherlands: Rijksuniversiteit. ISOWCentruni voor Didactiek vim
Limitations of the Study Wiskunde en Natuurwetenschappen.
Fishbein. M.. & Ajzen, 1. (1975). Belief; altitude, intention und hch\ior:
In the present study, we measured, by means of question- An introduction to theory and research. New York: Addison-Wesley.
naires, environmental attitude, willingness to make sacri- Gamoran, A., & Nystrand. M. (1994). Tracking, instruction and achieve-
ment. International Journal of Educational Resecrrrh. 21. 2 17-23 I .
tices. and environmentally responsible behavior. We realize Gigliotti, L. M. (1992). Environmental attitudes: 20 years of change’! Tho
that findings may be biased by the way they have been col- Journal of Environmental Education. 24( 1). 15-34.
lected. The estimate of the mean and the variance of the Gigliotti, L. M. (1994). Environmental issues: Cornell students’ willing-
ness to take action, 1990. The Journal of‘Enrimnmental Edirc~terioti.
measurement, in particular, appears to depend on the mea- 26( I ), 34-42.
suring technique employed (Heidelberg, van den Bergh, Goldstein, H. ( 1987). Multilevel models in edrcc~crtional crnd socitrl
Kuhlemeier, & Hoeks, 1993; Otter, Mellenbergh, Blok, & research. London: Griffin.
Goldstein. H., & McDonald. R. P. (1988). A general model lor the analy-
vim den Bergh, 1992). That very variance is essential when sis of multilevel data. Psychometrika, 53,455-467.
one studies relations among variables. In their meta-analy- Guttman, L. A. (1954). A new approach to fiictor analysis: The radex. I n f?
sis of research on responsible environmental behavior, F. Lazersfeld (Ed.), Mathematical thinking in the ,w(iul scienc.rs (pp.
258-348). New York: Columbia University Press.
Hines et al. (1986-1987) reported higher correlations Hausbeck. K. W.. Milbrath. L. W., & Enright. S. M.(1992). Environmeri-
hctween environmental knowledge, attitudes, and behavior tal knowledge, awareness and concern among 11th-grade students: New
i n situations in which actual behavior was assessed than in York State. The Journal of Environmental Education, 21(3), 27-34.
Heidelberg. J.. Bergh. H. van den, Kuhlemeier, J. 8.. & Hoeks, J. (1Y93).
situations in which behavior was self-reported. We recom- Tijdsbesteding van docenten in het voortgezet ondenvijs op verschil-
mend that the extent to which findings such as ours depend lende manieren gemeten: Een validatiestudie (Measuring time spent it1
on the measuring technique be checked. To what extent do secondary education: A validation study 1. Tjdschrifr w o r Ondenm~ijsr+
search. 18, 306-316.
other techniques, such as observations, logbooks, or inter- Hendriks. H.. & Zande, P. van der (1991 ). Vakdeelleerplun ~~irtrcc~r- (‘II
views, result in other outcomes? In short, the findings of our milieu-educatie in de basisvonning biolugie IA curriculum for science
investigation need to be interpreted with some caution. and environmental education in basic education biologyl. Enschede. The
Netherlands: SLO.
Hines. J. M.. Hungerford, H. R.. & Tomera. A. N. (19861987). Analysih
and synthesis of research on responsible environmental behavior: A
NOTES meta-analysis. The Journal of Environmental Education. IX(2). 1-8.
Joreskog, K. G.. & Sorbom. D. (1989). LISREL 7: U.wrs’.y reference jirtitlc
I . One item. which concerns an evidently nongeographical topic. was (1st ed.). Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software.
;iLuirdingly excluded from the classification. Kish, L. (1965). Survey sampling. New York: Wiley.
7 . We determined the reliability (internal consistency) by using a boot- Kreft. G. G. (1987). Models and methods for the iiirasurement of s t . h o o l
strap procedure. The mean correlations. corrected for test length, between eflects. Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam.
randomly combined but parallel test halves are reported. Kuhlemeier, J. B.. Bergh. H. van den. NottC. H.. Wagenaar, H.. Verstralen.
3. To study the distinguishability of the scales for environmental atti- H.. & Cappers, R. ( 1994). Balans van her aardrijkskunde-ondernrj.y i r i
tude and willingnesa to make sacrifices, we conducted an exploratory fac- her derde leerjaar van her voortgezet ondenvijs IReport on the state of
tor analysis. When we imposed a two-factor structure upon the data in a the art of geography in the third year of secondary education]. Arnhem.
principal component analysis with varimax rotation. 18 of the 20 state- The Netherlands: Cito.
ments appeared to load higher on their “own” subscale than on the other Longford, N. T..& Muthbn, B. 0. (1992). Factor analysis for clustered
one. In addition. we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis by using observations. Psychometrika. 57, 58 1-597.
LISREL (Jiireskog & Sorbom. 1989). A model with separate factors for Maloney, M. P.. & Ward, M. P. (1973). Ecology: Let’s hear from the p c i -
environmental attitude and willingness to make sacrifices was found to fit ple. An objective scale for the measurement of ecological attitutles and
the data extremely well (GFI = ,993); moreover, that model fit significant- knowledge. American Psvchologist, 28, 583-586.
Kuhlemeier, van den Bergh, and Lagerweij 13

Maloney, M. P., Ward, M. P.. & Braucht. G. N. (1975). Psychology in tional value (energy) a particular product possesses (49%).
action: A revised scale for the measurement of ecological attitudes and Approximately one third of the students were aware that tourism
knowledge. Anierican Psychologist, 30, 787-79 1 . on the Italian coast has decreased greatly in local areas as a result
Meer. E van der ( 1980). Arhtergronden van milieugedrag [Backgrounds of the growth of algae in the Adriatic Sea (36%). that the water in
ot‘ environmental behavior]. Leiden, The Netherlands: Universiteit
I .eiden.
the Rhine has grown cleaner in recent years (34%), that there is
Meer, F. van der ( 1981). Environmental attitude and behavior [Attitude en less lead pollution (34%). and that the emission of sulfur dioxide
inilieugedragl. Leiden. The Netherlands: Universiteit Leiden. (37%) has decreased in the Netherlands. A quarter of the students.
Ministerie van Volkshuisvesting. Ruirntelijke Ordening en Milieubeheer at the most, knew that photochemical air pollution occurs espe-
( / Y Y l J . Zestig dingen die u voor het milieu kunr doen [Sixty things one cially when there is a hot layer of the atmosphere over a cold layer
can do for the environment]. ‘s-Gravenhage, The Netherlands: Minis- of the atmosphere (25%), or were acquainted with the concept ot
tcrie van VROM. environmental impact reporting ( 17%).
Munaon. 6. H. (1994). Ecological misconceptions. The Journal of Envi-
ronmmlul Educution, 24(4). 30-34.
Nationaal Milieubeleidsplan (1989). Kiezen of verliezen [Choosing or los-
ing]. ’s-Gravenhage. The Netherlands: SDU.
Nelissen, N..Perenboom, R.. Peters, P., & Peters, V. (1987). De Neder- Appendix B: A Description of the Environmentally
Irrndrrs cn hun milieu [The Dutch and their environment]. Zeist. The Responsible Behavior of Ninth-Grade Students
Netherlands: Kerckebosch. in the Netherlands
Otter. M.. Mellenhergh, G. J., Blok, H.. & Bergh. H. van den (1992). Mea-
suring time spent on teaching native language. Journul of Experimentol Student as Consumer
b.d~l(.~iIioti, 60. 28 1-287.
Proser. R., Rashash. J.. & Goldstein, H. (1991 ). M U : Softwarefijr three- In reply to the question, “How often do you use more tap wilier
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

Iri~rluntrlwis. User’i guide. London: Institute of Education. than is strictly necessary (e.g., take a long shower, let the w;iier
Reijnders. L. ( I99 I ). Help her milieu: War elke Nederlander kan doen run continually when brushing your teeth or doing the dishes)?”
[Helping the environment: What each Dutchman can do]. Amsterdam: 10% of the students said they hardly ever use more tap wiiter than
Van Grnnep. is strictly necessary; however, another 10% of them said that they
Ryan. M . J.. & Bonfield. E. H . (1975). The Fishbein extended model and
consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research. 2. 118-136. do that (nearly) always. In response to a similar question regard-
Schahn. J . . & Holzer. E. (1990). Studies of individual environmental con- ing leaving the lights on unnecessarily, I in 3 students responded
cern. The role of knowledge, gender, and background variables. Envi- “hardly ever”; yet a quarter answered “often” or “(nearly)
ronnient und Behat1i06 22, 767-786. always.” Well over 40% claimed that they put on a sweater often
Scott. D.. & Willits. E K. (1994). Environmental attitudes and behavior. A or (nearly) always when it is cold at home instead of turning u p
Pennsylvania survey. Environment und Behavior. 26, 239-260. the heat.
Sia. A. P.. Hungerford. H. R.. & Tomera. A. N. (1985-86). Selected pre- In response to the question about the purchase of soft drinks or
dictors of responsible environmental behavior: An analysis. The Journal fruit juice, well over one third of the students claimed they usual-
of Enidronmm~ulEducation, /7(2), 3 1-40.
ly buy those products in glass bottles ( 2 1 8 ) or in cartons ( 14%’);
Silver. D., & Vallely, B. (1991).Her milieu jduw zaak! 101 tips voorjon-
,gurtn om zelfiets uun her milieu IY doen [The environment your case!
the others said they usually buy cans (14%). plastic bottles (43%),
101 tips for adolescents]. Amsterdam: Veen. or never buy soft drinks or fruit juice (8%).
Ungar. S. ( 1994). Apples and organges: Probing the attitude-behaviorrela- When asked about the size of the packaging of soft drinks, milk,
tionship for the environment, Cunadian Review of Sociology and or yogurt, nearly all the students (88%) stated that they usually buy
AnthropriloKy, 3 1 , 288-304. those articles in large packs of 1 L or more.
Willms. J . D. (1992).Monitoring school perjormunce. A guide for educu- Food that had not been sprayed and on which no fertilirer had
Ior.7. Easingstoke. United Kingdom: Burgess Science Press. been used was hardly ever bought by one third ofthe students.
No less than 59% of the students claimed hardly ever to take a
look at the packaging of a product to see what harmful sub-
stances it contains.
Appendix A: Some Items That Were Relatively Well
Two-thirds of the students said that they make use of a shop-
Mastered and Some That Were Relatively Poorly Mastered
ping bag, plastic bag, or carrier bag to transport the shopping.
Practically all the students (91%) recognized the concepts of Seven percent, however, usually took nothing to put the shopping
recycling, chemocar (86%). and small chemical waste (86%). In in. Those students buy a new plastic bag each time. The rest.
addition, at least three fourths (77%) knew that most cars consume nearly a quarter of the students, indicated that they never do any
less gasoline at 90 k d h than at 120 k m h , that the exhaust gases shopping.
of cars do not become totally clean with a controlled three-way
catalyzer (74%), that chlorine is less environmentally friendly than Recycling
vinegar (77%), that the greenhouse effect causes the icecaps to Nearly 60% of the students said that they usually take nonde-
melt (82%), that refuse incinerators have been closed at times in posit glassware to the bottle bank, whereas 6% said they usu;illy
the Netherlands because too many toxic gases were released dump it in the dustbin or rubbish bag. Three percent usually leave
(72%). and that it is better for the environment if individuals buy it where it is most convenient for them. That question had little rcl-
soft drinks. milk, or yogurt in a large bottle or pack than in a small evance for 32%: Someone else, such as the father or the mother,
one (78%). takes care of all nondeposit empty bottles. For the analogous ques-
Some relatively difficult items were mastered by less than half tion with regard to the way empty deposit bottles are dealt with,
of the ninth-grade students. Nearly half knew that Dutch potato the percentages were 68, 2, 2. and 28, respectively.
species such as hinrje and eigenheimer are usually not cultivated Three quarters of the students claimed they usually returned
without chemical pesticides (44%), that cut flowers from the empty or broken (disposable) batteries to the shop or placed them
florist’s are usually not grown without poison (39%). that heavy into the batteries compartment of a bottle bank. Nine percent of’
metals are not radioactive substances used in nuclear power sta- the students said they usually dispose of batteries in the garbage:
tions (4 I %), that more chemical pesticides and fertilizer are used They dump their batteries in the dustbin or rubbish bag.
in regular agriculture than in alternative agriculture (47%). and The students were asked where they leave their used writing
that an E number (e.g., E 330) does not indicate how much nutri- papers, magazines, newspapers, and such when they are no loiieer
I4 The Journal of Environmental Education

needed. More than half (55%) of the students said that most paper Foodstuffs and Refuse
1;inds i n the dustbin or rubbish bag. Less than half (45%) separat- How did the students take food to school’?More than half o f t h r
ed il for recycling. students usually used a sandwich box or a plastic sandwich hag.
Of the students who claimed to come across old medicines once One third took it to school in a plastic bag or in foil. Some IS1
i n a while. 77% usually took them back to the chemist’s, 18% usu- claimed they never take foods to school. Asked the question. “How
ally put them in the dustbin or rubbish bag, and 5% usually flushed often do leave your litter behind when you are outside (e.g., throw
tlieiii down the toilet. candy wrappers, cans, or chewing gum out on the street)?” 17% ol
More than half of the students put litter from fruit or vegetables, the students declared they hardly ever leave litter behind; 61% said
lor example, potato, orange, and banana peels, with the other they sometimes do that. On the basis of that question, one fifth ol
household garbage (dustbin or refuse bag); the others placed it the students could be counted among the notorious polluters. They
elsewhere (e.g.. compost pile, compost bin, waste food collector, claimed that they leave litter behind often or nearly always wheii
iii the street). they are outside.
Downloaded By: [2009 Universiti Putra Malaysia] At: 14:31 14 March 2011

ERRATUM
I n the article “Evaluating Adult Groundwater Education,” by Argyrios Gerakis. which
appeared in Volume 30, Issue 1 (Fall 1998), a figure was printed incorrectly. The correct
figure is shown below. The editors of The Journal of Environmental Education regret the
error.

Experimental Group Control Group


Morning (Pretest)

Pretested Pretested

D B
Afternoon (Posttest)

Pretested Nonpretested Pretested

FIGURE 1. The modified separate-sample pretest-posttest control group quasi-


experimental design.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai