Anda di halaman 1dari 14

Susan Auty, Richard Elliot (1998), "SOCIAL IDENTITY AND THE MEANING OF

FASHION BRANDS", in European Advances in Consumer Research Volume 3, eds.


Basil G. Englis and Anna Olofsson, Provo, UT : Association for Consumer Research,
Pages: 1-10.

European Advances in Consumer Research Volume 3, 1998      Pages 1-10

SOCIAL IDENTITY AND THE MEANING OF FASHION BRANDS

Susan Auty, Lancaster University, U.K.

Richard Elliot, Oxford University, U.K.

ABSTRACT -

This study considers the importance of social identity in the interpretation of brands of
jeans as measured by Snyder’s Revised Self-Monitoring Scale, which discriminates
between people who are highly motivated to respond to social cues and those who remain
'true to themselves’. It was found that self-monitoring is a significant mediator of
meaning with regard to unbranded, but not branded, jeans. A model of choice by
elimination of the unacceptable is suggested by high self-monitoring responses. It has
implications for the amount of advertising required to support a fashion brand.

INTRODUCTION

Diverse theorists have demonstrated the use of clothing as a code, a language which
allows a message to be created and (selectively) understood (McCracken and Roth 1989;
Holman, 1981; Hollander, 1978). Perceptions of brand-users have been found to differ
for nearly identical brands within a product category (Swartz, 1983). The consumer in the
street confirms the existence of a clothing code and indicates the importance of branding
to the code: 'If I’m wearing a white T-shirt and sneakers, that label [Armani] will fill in
the rest of the information about me. I want to give out the right impression’, says a
customer in a London shop selecting her tenth pair of Armani jeans (Financial Times,
1995).

Since this consumer’s next door neighbour is a 'Moschino person’, it is clear that not
everyone wants to give out the same 'right’ impression nor reads the label in the same
way. McCracken and Roth (1989) make this point to distinguish between the code of
clothing and the code of language: 'The knowledge of a code may have more uneven
distributions for products than it does for language’ (p 29) with the consequence that
there is 'much greater variation in code mastery than is the case for language’. Belk,
Mayer and Bahn (1981) carried out one of the first studies into the decoding of messages
conveyed by a person’s choice of a particular product. The variables of age, gender and
social class were hypothesised to account for differences in perception for the highly
visible and socially expressive products of automobiles and houses. They found that
students were more likely than older adults to rely on 'consumption-based stereotypes’
and thus make more nearly unanimous judgements about the owners of particular cars
and houses. They were surprised to find that men had more consistent judgements than
women, and explained this with reference to the importance for men of recognising status
differentials 'for achievement motivation and career preparation’. Both lower and higher
social classes exhibited consumption-based stereotypes in their judgements, but different
stereotypes prevailed among the two groups.

Clothing is particularly susceptible to differences in consumption stereotyping, and


therefore to differences in ability to decode a range of messages. Noesjirwan and
Crawford (1982) make the point that 'clothing is primarily a means of communicating,
not personal identity, but social identity’. They conclude that 'clothing is symbolic of that
[social] identity and the values espoused by the group. The same values serve as a
yardstick for judging the clothing worn by others and the social identity symbolized by it.
. . . Advertisements that project an implied social identity through a model’s clothing are
likely to have quite different effects on different kinds of consumers.’

Understanding how people interpret clothing, and how different groups of people make
different judgements about the same brand of clothing is critical to clothing
manufacturers and their advertising agencies. A knowledge of how different groups are
likely to differ in their response to a particular set of symbols means that the marketer has
more control over the decoding of the brand image when advertising is 'consumed’. Jeans
are an ideal case for examining brand meanings: there is a wide range of branded and
unbranded jeans available, and they are almost universally worn by people of both sexes
from adolescence through at least to middle age.

In their seminal research on the interpretation of clothing 'codes’, McCracken and Roth
(1989) found that females were significantly better than men in interpreting the syntax of
clothing codes. That is, women more readily recognised a 'look’ and were more sensitive
to fashion cues than men were. Recent research has shown that men and women differ in
the way they pay attention to cues in advertising (Meyers-Levy and Maheswaran, 1991)
and similarly that they read fashion symbols with different criteria (Meyers-Levy and
Sternthal, 1991; Elliott, 1994). Females have been found to be more sensitive to the
informative details provided in ads than men generally are.

Following on from McCracken and Roth, Elliott (1994) also found a difference in the
way different age groups respond to popular brands in an exploratory study of the
meanings of brands of sneakers. McCracken and Roth discovered that age can be a highly
significant variable in the interpretation of certain fashion codes, particularly 'punk’;
Elliott observed a remarkable difference between secondary school and university
students in the semantic clusters elicited in relation to sneakers. The younger children
made no specific reference to advertising messages, but had very strong opinions on
which brands were fashionable and which were definitely not.

FASHION INVOLVEMENT AND SOCIAL IDENTITY


Social class, which might seem to be a critical variable where social identity is involved,
is a problematic variable to measure, primarily because it is difficult to obtain accurate
measurements for young adults (acknowledged by Belk, Mayer and Bahn). Nevertheless,
because of their attention to fashion this age group must figure highly in any study of
clothing codes. In any case, the difference in class may not be so important as the
difference in the strength of group affiliation and the consequent attention to a particular
code. McCracken and Roth note that the degree of fashion involvement may be a relevant
variable in the interpretation of clothing codes. Fashion involvement is likely to be
associated with differences in sensitivity to social surroundings in that those who are
highly motivated to fit into a particular group will need to be aware of the fashion cues
not just of that group but of other less desirable groups so that the 'wrong’ cues may be
avoided.

Research in psychology, and more recently in consumer behaviour, has made use of
Snyder’s Self-monitoring Scale (1974; revised in Snyder and Gangestad, 1986) to
distinguish between people who are sensitive to the social cues around them (high self-
monitors) and those who are more likely to suit themselves whatever their social
surroundings (low self-monitors). The revised scale measures a person’s control over
their social presentation using 18 true-false statements as shown in Exhibit 1. People are
then usually divided into two groups using the median, where one group comprises the
'chameleons’ (who constantly change their coat to fit into their surroundings) and the
other the 'leopards’ (who never change their spots).

EXHIBIT 1

SNYDER'S REVISED SELF-MONITORING SCALE

Several studies have been conducted to show the influence of self-monitoring groups on
responses to advertising (Snyder and DeBono, 1985; Lennon, Davis and Fairhurst, 1988;
Snyder, 1989; Johar and Sirgy, 1991; Shavitt, Lowrey and Han, 1992). In particular,
Shavitt et al. hypothesised that multiple function products (having both social identity
and utilitarian functions) would elicit the strongest differences in response to advertising
between high and low self-monitors. In their study, jeans were considered to be a
multiple function product 'because they are associated with important utilitarian outcomes
(e.g., comfort, durability) as well as social image implications (e.g., conveying one’s
style, taste).’ In the actual experiments sunglasses and watches were used as multiple
function products, and the findings supported the view that 'self monitoring comes into
play when a product affords high and low self-monitors the opportunity to focus on
different functional goals’. Hence it was found that there are few differences in attitudes
to aspirin but quite distinct differences in social identity or mixed function products.

This study takes the case of jeans and looks at the meanings associated with an
unbranded product and the most popular brand in the category to see if advertising
succeeds in creating meanings which reside in the product. If so, are these consistently
decoded differently by different groups of people? By using the same pair of Levi’s jeans
with all brand markings removed for the unbranded stimulus (thereby ensuring that all
functional attributes are identical) one can measure the strength of the brand’s meanings.
It is hypothesised that high self-monitors will associate more positive meanings with
branded jeans than unbranded jeans and that they will differ significantly from low self-
monitors in the extent of their positive associations. Low self-monitors are expected to
respond more favourably to the utilitarian attributes of both pairs of jeans because they
should pay more attention to the product’s construction rather than image in their
evaluation.

HYPOTHESES

H1. The meaning of a branded fashion product (Levi's) will be different from the
meaning of an unbranded fashion product.

H2. The meaning of a fashion product (jeans) will differ by age and gender.

H3. High self-monitors will have more positive attitudes towards a branded fashion
product (Levi's) than will low self-monitors.

H4. Low self-monitors will respond more favorably than high self-monitors to the
utilitarian attributes of both a branded and unbranded fashion product (jeans).

METHODOLOGY

Sixteen focus groups were held on a university campus in the north of England with the
objective of discovering the meanings people attach to various brands of jeans and to
jeans in general. Students aged 18-35 attended and were asked if the meanings they
attached to brands had changed since their secondary school days. A list of 32 bipolar
adjectives were drawn from the focus group discussions and tested for comprehension on
a convenience sample of children aged 14 to 18. Three pairs were removed as a result of
the tests. The adjective pairs were randomly distributed with regard to potentially positive
and potentially negative connotations along the right hand side. A seven point scale
separated each pair. (See Exhibit 1)

A three-part survey instrument was then prepared. The first part was a 7-point semantic
differential scale consisting of the 29 bipolar adjectives; the second part contained the 18
self-monitoring true-false statements as devised by Snyder(see Exhibit 2); the third part
asked demographic questions. After a pilot survey on campus, two further pairs of
adjectives were removed; minor word changes to the Snyder statements were made for
adaptation to British usage; and usage questions were added to the demographic section.

803 respondents were interviewed in various town centres in the UK. Males and females,
in each of three age bands - 14-17; 18-24; 25-34 - were selected by quota guidelines.
These age bands comprise 58% of the market for jeans in the UK (Retail Business, 1994).
Half were shown a photograph with Levi's brand markings clearly displayed. Levi's jeans
were used because they are the most highly advertised brand by far in the UK; their spend
(between .L3m and L5m annually) in the five years prior to this study was four times
higher than their closest rival, Wrangler (Retail Business, 1994). ne other half were
shown the same pose with the brand markings (pocket stitching, red tab and leather
nameplate) removed by computer techniques. The photograph was a back view of a
model of indeterminate sex (confirmed by pilot).

98 responses were rejected for analysis by interviewers because of influence by


respondents' friends or other interview problems. A further 36 were rejected because of
incomplete responses in the self-monitoring section. The final samples were thus 336
who were shown the unbranded stimulus and 333 who were shown the branded.

SPSS was used to analyse each data set. The bipolar scales were reordered so that
potentially positive values were all on the right hand side. The mean rating for each
attribute was compared across data sets to determine the extent to which branding affects
perception. Crosstabulation by sex, self-monitoring score and age was used to identify
overall differences between different groups' perceptions of branded and unbranded
jeans. The scale of the semantic differential pairs was then collapsed from 7 down to 3,
scores of 1-2, 3-5, 6-7 forming groups of strongly positive or negative and neutral for
crosstabulation at an individual variable level. The reason for choosing this method of
collapsing the categories, rather than the more traditional one whereby only 4 is
considered neutral, was to contrast the positive and negative more strongly with a 'buffer'
group.

When the seif-monitoring scores were divided into two groups of 'high' (9-18) and 'low'
(1-9), as in Snyder's research, virtually 50% was in each group in both data sets.
However, in order to pick out self-monitoring effects more strongly than in previous
research, it was decided to use three groups, having a 'buffer' group between the highest
and lowest self-monitors. Thus the scores were divided up into three groups of low (I to
6), medium (7-12) and high (1318) for the purposes of analysis. This grouping showed a
remarkable consistency between the two samples in that the difference in proportions
varied by only.1%, giving confidence in the samples (Table 1). By segregating the large
group of medium self-monitors, we can be more certain that the results reflect underlying
personality differences and not merely statistical fluke.

MEAN DIFFERENCES IN ATTRIBUTE SCORES BETWEEN BRANDED AND


UNBRANDED JEANS

The mean scores for the 27 attributes were compared across both data sets. The means of
all but six variables were significantly different (p<.000). HI is thus supported. The
largest differences are displayed in Table 2.

As one would expect, the Levi's were perceived to be much more well-known than were
the unbranded pair. The other large differences testify to the strength of Levi's brand
image: they are considered more expensive and high quality than the unbranded pair. The
extent to which this mass brand is considered to be ,original' and to 'make a statement'
more than its unbranded counterpart is further evidence of Levi's strong image. The
nondiscriminating adjectives were: 'for men', 'individual', 'practical', ,associated with
cowboys', 'common', 'aggressive'.

AGE AND GENDER DIFFERENCES

For the purposes of crosstabulation of the individual variables, it was decided to collapse
the seven point scale down to three, with a strongly positive and negative grouping
formed from the two points at each extreme. The attributes were crosstabulated against
age, sex and self-monitoring score. (Results of the self-monitoring analysis are discussed
in detail below.) The Mantel-Haenszel test for linear association was used to determine
significance (p <05) supported by the eta coefficient in the case of sex (a nominal
independent variable).

Crosstabulation of the individual variables suggests that 14-17 year olds are more
positively disposed to many of Levi's attributes than older age groups are. Indeed, the
findings support Elliott's (1994) conclusion that the 'meaning of fashion brands may
differ between genders and, less surprisingly, between age groups'. There were significant
differences between the youngest and oldest age groups in the perception of Levi's with
regard to cut, desirability, fit, sexiness, stylishness and trendiness. It was also found that
men and women had different attitudes to branded jeans, specifically to image,
modernity, practicality, quality and trendiness, with women having the more favourable
opinions. It is interesting that in both cases there is a predominance of image-related
attributes, casting doubt on the tendency for women to process functional information
more actively than men, or the youngest age group to be exclusively focused on image.

In contrast to the branded jeans, there are relatively few differences between age and
gender groups in perceptions of unbranded jeans. H2 is thus supported only in relation to
a branded product. Men perceive unbranded jeans to be more American and more
original than women do (suggesting that they are either rejecting Levi's messages or not
processing the cues as accurately as women do), while 25-35 year olds see them as less
well-cut than younger groups do (perhaps because they see all jeans as less well cut). A
multivariate analysis of variance revealed a significant interaction (p <.04) between age
and self-monitoring and the 27 descriptive variables, particularly in relation to the cut, fit
and ,authenticity' of unbranded jeans. Otherwise there was no significant interaction
between the three independent variables and the descriptive variables in either set.

EXHIBIT 2

SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL SCALE

POSITIVE ATTITUDES TO JEANS AND SELF- MONITORING


A composite attitude towards branded and unbranded jeans was determined by
respondents' mean rating of 15 pairs of bipolar attributes, each of which could be
identified as having a definitely positive and definitely negative pole. That is, twelve
pairs such as >American/not American= were removed because they were capable of
being interpreted both negatively and positively by the population. The positives were:
high quality; hard wearing; comfortable-, well cut; authentic; sexy; practical; 'classy';
easy to wear; desirable; stylish; a good fit; convey an image; modem; trendy.

TABLE 1

PERCENTAGE OF EACH SAMPLE IN THREE SELF-MONITORING GROUPS

TABLE 2

DIFFERENCES IN MEAN SCORES OF UNBRANDED AND BRANDED JEANS

TABLE 3

POSITIVE ATTITUDES TO JEANS

As might be expected, attitudes towards branded jeans were much more positive, with the
mean score being 2.8 (where 1 is most positive) as opposed to 3.6 for unbranded jeans.
For the crosstabulation of this variable each sample set was divided into three roughly
equal groups representing positive, neutral and negative attitudes. Note that the entry
standards' differed between the two sample sets, so that a 2.4 mean was required to get
into the positive group in the branded sample, whereas 3.3 was sufficient for the
unbranded sample. Hence, 33% would be expected in any cell given percentages in terms
of the three self-monitoring groups (only two of which are shown in the tables below;
also, for comparative purposes, the percentages of the 'unbranded' sample using the cutoff
points of the 'branded' sample are shown in brackets.)

Self-monitoring scores were found to correlate negatively with attitudes towards


unbranded jeans (Table 3).

High self-monitors were significantly less likely to regard unbranded jeans positively
than were low self-monitors (p<.01). However, contrary to H3, there was no significant
difference in the 'branded' sample with regard to self-monitoring.

To test the hypothesis (H4) that low self-monitors are likely to respond more favourably
than high self-monitors to the utilitarian attributes of both pairs of jeans, a composite
'utilitarian attitude' variable was derived from the individual attitude scores towards fit,
cut, practicality, comfort and durability. It was analysed as described above (Table 4).

Low self-monitors appear, as hypothesised, to have more favourable attitudes to


functional qualities of both branded and unbranded jeans than high self-monitors do
(p<.02), perhaps because they pay more attention to such matters and therefore have
stronger opinions on them.

CROSSTABUILATION BY SELF-MONITORING GROUPS

Quite surprisingly, the individual variables produced only one significant difference with
regard to branded jeans and self-monitoring groups, with Levi's being perceived as much
sexier by high self-monitors than low self-monitors. In contrast, there were many
differences among the perceptions of self-monitoring groups with regard to unbranded
jeans. The implication is that high self-monitors virtually define jeans as 'branded jeans'.
Unlike the youngest age group they do not have an especially positive attitude towards
branded versions of the garment so much as negative ones towards unbranded versions.
Interestingly, multivariate analysis using the 15 descriptors of the composite variable
suggests that it is the youngest high self-monitors above all who reject the unbranded
goods as being not well cut, not a good fit and above all, not authentic (p <006). A
summary of all the hypothesised findings shows that a new hypothesis is needed to more
accurately describe attitude differences among personality groups with regard to branded
and unbranded fashion goods (see Table 5)

TABLE 4

POSITIVE ATTITUDES TO UTILITARIAN ATTRIBUTES OF JEANS

TABLE 5

SUMMARY OF HYPOTHESISED FINDINGS

TABLE 6

SELF-MONITORING BY WELL CUT (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

The following tables of detailed findings are a composite of the ,strongly positive'
columns from the crosstabulations of both data sets, and hence significant differences are
as noted in the text and do not apply to the tables displayed. For the same reason the
percentages in the tables do not add up to 100% because each column was based on a
different data set and extracted from a nine cell table calculated with row percentages in
order to indicate differences that the three personality types (only two shown here) have
on attitudes.

Six variables gave rise to a significant difference in response towards unbranded, but not
branded jeans. One variable showed a difference between high and low self-monitors
regardless of branding. Both utilitarian and image-related attributes of unbranded jeans
were perceived differently. High self-monitors were much less likely to consider them to
be well cut, in comparison both to low self-monitors and to high self-monitors' responses
to branded jeans (Table 6).

Another functional attribute with significant differences between high and low self-
monitors for the unbranded stimulus is 'hard wearing' (Table 7).

Perceptions of quality are similarly affected by personality differences. High self-


monitors were much less willing to ascribe high quality to unbranded jeans than low self-
monitors were, although the branded jeans showed no significant difference between the
two personality groups (Table 8).

Surprisingly, many of the image-related variables such as ,makes a statement' 'classy' or


'conveys an image' showed no significant difference in perceptions by personality types.
The whole sample was in general agreement that unbranded jeans do not score highly in
these categories in contrast to branded jeans. Only with regard to 'trendy' did high self-
monitors show a significantly different reaction (Table 9).

Here it is the willingness of one-third of low self-monitors to consider unbranded jeans to


be trendy which is notable. Given the much lower differential between their attitudes to
branded and

unbranded pairs than those of other personality groups, their consideration of jeans here
seems to be generic rather than brand specific. High self-monitors appear to limit
trendiness much more to branded jeans. Thus, while low self-monitors do recognise that
branded jeans convey a different image and are more classy, they still tend to regard
wearing jeans in general as the fashion trend, rather than the more code-conscious high
self-monitors who do not.

TABLE 7

SELF-MONITORING BY HARD WEARING (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

TABLE 8
SELF-MONITORING BY HIGH QUALITY (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

TABLE 9

SELF-MONITORING BY TRENDY (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

TABLE 10

SELF-MONITORING BY ORIGINAL (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

There is a similar finding with regard to the originality of jeans, although in this case the
high self-monitors consistently rate jeans, both branded and unbranded, as less original
than low self-monitors do (Table 10).

It may be that originality is not highly prized by high self-monitors, and therefore they
are less likely to rate branded jeans highly on this attribute than low self-monitors are.

High self-monitors' perceptions differ most from low self-monitors in their assessment of
the individuality of unbranded jeans (Table 11).

High self-monitors clearly rate them as 'not individual'. Less than 10% of all but low self-
monitors (13%) positively ascribe individuality to any pair of jeans, the majority being
neutral. What stands out, however, is the clear view of high self-monitors that unbranded
jeans are definitely not individual.

Similarly, while the majority of respondents perceive all jeans to be comfortable, high
self-monitors think unbranded jeans are less comfortable than low self-monitors do
(Table 12).

However, they are also significantly less likely than low self-monitors to think that
branded jeans are comfortable! Their criteria for judging the positive values of jeans may
mean that comfort is not an important distinction to make, in keeping with H4.

In contrast, sexiness is an attribute of great importance to high self-monitors (Table 13).

This attribute was the only one for which there was a significant difference in perception
among personality types in the branded sample, but not the unbranded. Surprisingly the
majority of respondents in both samples are neutral with regard to sexiness. The table
shows that it is not so much that high self-monitors are distinguished by their perceptions
of branded jeans' sexiness so much as low self-monitors refuse to ascribe this attribute to
jeans, especially branded ones.
DISCUSSION

There were distinct differences in the perceptions of branded and unbranded jeans by
different self-monitoring groups. As expected, the branded jeans were perceived as much
more well-known than the unbranded. This finding merely confirms that the brand
markings on the unbranded photograph were successfully removed. Otherwise, the
largest differences in perception between the branded and the unbranded jeans were
'Expensive' and 'High Quality', followed by several image-related descriptors: Original,
Stylish, Classy and Makes a Statement. In general, therefore, the findings suggest that
Levi's have an established brand image close to the image purveyed in their advertising.

TABLE 11

SELF-MONITORING BY (NOT) INDIVIDUAL (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

TABLE 12

SELF-MONITORING BY COMFORTABLE (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

TABLE 13

SELF-MONITORING BY SEXY (UNBRANDED AND BRANDED)

When the mean scores of respondents on all unambiguously positive attributes are
compared, the overall mean of the branded group is 2.8 where 1 is most positive,
compared to 3.6 for the unbranded group. Given, however, that the range of means is
almost identical for the two data sets, there are clearly differences in perception
attributable to independent variables. Branded jeans were perceived differently according
to the age and sex of the respondent, though there were no interaction effects. Unbranded
jeans were viewed significantly more negatively by high self-monitors than low self-
monitors, especially when mediated by age. The findings suggest that young high self-
monitors arc likely to be positive towards branded jeans but much more notably negative
towards unbranded. This strength of negative feeling directed towards unbranded jeans
was not the expected finding.

Self-monitoring scores, which depend on the extent to which a person desires to stay in
tune with other people, appear to have an effect on the perceptions of unbranded jeans in
particular. Indeed, there are no significant differences between the three types of self-
monitoring with regard to branded jeans. One might say that high self-monitors define
jeans in terms of branded jeans, and thus unbranded jeans are regarded negatively
whereas branded jeans are just jeans.

This negative effect is not confined to perceptions of image as one might expect.
Interestingly, all personality types agree that branded jeans have more of an image and
are classier than unbranded jeans. The difference in conviction occurs noticeably with
regard to functional attributes. There is a tendency for unbranded jeans to appear less
well cut, less hard wearing and of lower quality to high self-monitors. Only with regard
to sexiness are branded jeans rated significantly more positively by high self-monitors,
and even this appears to be the result of a strongly negative reaction by low seif-monitors
to the idea of any jeans being sexy rather than a particularly positive one by high self-
monitors.

The findings support DeBono's contention (1987) that attitudes fulfil different goals for
high and low self-monitors and therefore attitudes that are apparently similar (eg 'branded
jeans are high quality') may be held for very different reasons. While high self-monitors
show no significant differences compared to low self-monitors in their attitudes towards
the cut and durability of branded jeans, it is clear from the overall results that these are
rated positively only because the brand is socially acceptable (their ratings on these
attributes for unbranded jeans are much lower than low self-monitors'). Their attitudes
appear to have been formed for what DeBono calls asocial adjustive function, that is, to
allow them 'to fit into important social situations and behave in ways appropriate to
various reference groups.' In contrast, low self-monitors form attitudes for a value
expressive (utilitarian) purpose and thus show much smaller differences between their
ratings of branded and unbranded jeans with regard to utilitarian attributes. However, it is
worth noting that the branded jeans were consistently rated more positively by all groups,
and the difference was particularly high for the perception of quality.

This finding suggests that for high involvement items of fashion which are actively used
as code by wearers, such as jeans, sneakers (trainers) and watches, there is no need for
advertisers to highlight functional quality in order to compete with weakly supported
brands or private labels. High self-monitors seem to transfer a negative image to the
functional attributes of unbranded goods, and do not let their opinion of a product's utility
affect their overall appraisal of a branded item, while low self-monitors share their
overall high appraisal of the brand. It is more important to fulfil the symbolic needs of
consumers because their functional needs in this case are to a large extent dependent on
their symbolic ones. In other words, if the jeans do not satisfy a consumer's image
requirements, they will not be perceived to be well cut or comfortable either. This is in
keeping with the findings of Sirgy, et al (1991) who note that self-congruity (that is, a
match between the symbolic or 'value-expressive' attributes of a product and the self-
concept of the consumer) 'biases functional congruity'.

The study supports a model of choice (in high self-monitors) by elimination of the
unacceptable rather than positive selection. Branding becomes crucial in this model in
that the product class may be defined in the terms dictated by the branding, and even
extended to include attributes not 'covered' by the brand. If jeans are unconsciously
benchmarked by high self-monitors against the most visible (that is, highly advertised)
brand, then all other jeans including other brands will suffer by comparison. There will
still be trade-offs in the actual decision such that lesser brands may be selected for a
number of reasons, but for high self-monitors the gap between the top brand and a
generic product will be such that the generic product is in a different product class and
will not even be considered.

This process of choice by rejection of the unacceptable is very similar to Bourdieu's


(1984) concept of the 'refusal of other tastes'. This suggests that the vital act of consumer
choice may be not to choose that which is most pleasing, but to reject that which is most
distasteful: 'when they have to be justified, [tastes] are asserted purely negatively, by the
refusal of other tastes'. With fashion products which cross class boundaries, such as jeans
and sneakers, there is no longer any 'taste of necessity' underlying the choice (jeans are
not worn because they are more affordable than other garments); instead the ubiquity of
jeans can be explained by a desire to avoid 'the conspicuous intention of aloofness'. In
this model of choice only low-self monitors will risk expressing an indifference to the
judgement and taste of their peers by wearing jeans with no social meaning. ne
preference for branded jeans expresses the desire for 'distinction', for discrimination
between themselves as 'belongers', who have chosen to interpret the social code implied
by the brand, and those who remain aloof, in effect a threat to their social identity.

LIMITATIONS

In this study it was not possible to compare each respondent's perception of the branded
versus the unbranded product, hence the findings are based on aggregate rather than
individual perceptions. Further research is being carried out in which all respondents will
receive three different stimuli: a primary brand, a secondary brand and a private label.
This should make it possible to measure the effects of branding more precisely than the
current study.

By presenting photographs rather than labels, perceptions concerning attributes such as fit
might have been affected by the stimuli rather than being based on the respondents'
existing attitudes. A possible problem with using labels, however, is that some
respondents may not be familiar with one or more of them and therefore have no opinions
on them.

CONCLUSION

This study suggests that advertisements do indeed create meanings in fashion items that
are decoded with surprising consistency depending on differing levels of fashion
involvement. Self-monitoring, which can be seen as a surrogate for fashion involvement,
discriminates perceptions of unbranded products in particular. For young high self-
monitors in particular, negative meanings appear to be more influential on attitudes than
positive meanings. Our findings are in keeping with the school children in Elliott's 1994
study of the meaning of sneakers, for whom 'if a brand was not fashionable it was very
definitely unfashionable with strong emotional valence' found in the unprompted
adjectives elicited. These findings suggest that advertisers cannot afford to become lax in
supporting their fashion brands: a large part of the target market forms attitudes for
'social-adjustive' purposes, and will be quick to change attitude if another brand becomes
more visible and hence more socially acceptable.

Anda mungkin juga menyukai