Anda di halaman 1dari 5

Leading Edge

Essay

Autism: Many Genes, Common Pathways?


Daniel H. Geschwind1,*
1
Neurogenetics Program, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
*Correspondence: dhg@ucla.edu
DOI 10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.016

Autism is a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental syndrome with a complex genetic etiology. It is


still not clear whether autism comprises a vast collection of different disorders akin to intellectual
disability or a few disorders sharing common aberrant pathways. Unifying principles among cases
of autism are likely to be at the level of brain circuitry in addition to molecular pathways.

Introduction lenges, measurable progress has been wide significance (Cantor et al., 2005). A
Autism represents a broadly defined achieved, placing several key questions recent large collaborative genome scan
disorder of behavior and cognition with into relief. by the Autism Genome Project (AGP)
onset prior to age 3 affecting the core of nearly 1200 sibling pairs with ASD
domains of language and social devel- Autism Is Heritable but Genetically (Szatmari et al., 2007) identified several
opment and involving abnormal repeti- Heterogeneous regions of interest, including chromo-
tive and restrictive behaviors. Because Three decades of research on autism some 11, but did not identify one region
autism is characterized by groups of involving twin and family studies support at genome-wide significance, despite a
symptoms and signs even in its narrow- a significant genetic contribution to its eti- marked increase in sample size over the
est conception, it is a highly variable ology. However, high heritability does not largest previous studies. Similarly, the
neurodevelopmental syndrome and not necessarily imply a particular model of homozygosity mapping collaborative for
a unitary condition. Children diagnosed genetic transmission or an easily identifi- autism (HMCA; Morrow et al., 2008) did
with autism differ significantly in sever- able major gene causing the disorder. On not report genome-wide significant loci
ity along many cognitive and behavioral the contrary, the last decade of research shared by two or more of the approxi-
dimensions, spawning the term autism in autism genetics reveals significant mately 80 consanguinous families, con-
spectrum disorder (ASD) to emphasize genetic heterogeneity. For example, sev- sistent with the existence of many dis-
its full scope. eral dozen distinct genetic disorders or tinct autism loci in this population as
Basic genetic and neuroscience identified chromosomal abnormalities can well. However, HMCA investigators were
research in ASD has grown exponen- result in autism, including Joubert’s syn- able to identify six independent homozy-
tially, reflecting a remarkable trajec- drome, Rett’s syndrome, tuberous scle- gous deletions segregating with autism
tory that likely represents many factors, rosis, Fragile X syndrome, and maternally in this unique cohort, implicating several
including public awareness and the inherited duplications of chromosome new genes in autism susceptibility while
realization that ASDs are a significant 15q11-13, the latter two each account- again highlighting the genetic heteroge-
cause of lifetime neuropsychiatric mor- ing for 1%–2% of ASD cases (Veenstra- neity of ASD (Morrow et al., 2008).
bidity, affecting nearly 1/150 live births. Vanderweele et al., 2004). In all, known Whole genome association (WGA)
However, in contrast with many other rare chromosomal disorders and genetic studies using various microarray plat-
disorders of the brain, for example neu- mental retardation syndromes account forms are beginning to replace linkage
rodegenerative diseases such as Par- for ~10% of ASD, each single cause con- studies in the analysis of complex (non-
kinson’s or Alzheimer’s diseases, autism tributing to no more than 1% of cases Mendelian) genetic disease including
lacks any clear unifying pathology at the on average (Abrahams and Geschwind, ASD. These genome-wide association
molecular, cellular, or systems level. Fur- 2008). analyses test the association of common
thermore, although ASDs appear to be The existence of considerable genetic single-nucleotide variations (SNPs) with
highly heritable overall, their underlying heterogeneity is also supported by sev- disease in a population. If a disease like
genetic etiology is complex, likely involv- eral dozen genetic linkage studies over autism is primarily caused by rare muta-
ing many genes, some of which may the last decade, which have often iden- tions in certain chromosomal regions,
represent common genetic variation, as tified nonoverlapping regions of inter- WGA is unlikely to be adequately pow-
well as potential interactions with envi- est and largely failed to formally repli- ered to identify most of these, whereas
ronmental factors. Thus, ASD research cate autism linkage findings at the level linkage may be powered to identify the
has to contend with not only the com- of genome-wide significance. There chromosomal region where they reside.
plexity and broadness of the phenotype are a few notable exceptions including Subsequent resequencing of genes
itself, which encompasses the biological regions on chromosome 7q21-35, sup- within a linkage region would then be
basis of human social interactions and ported by meta-analysis (Badner and necessary to identify the actual causal
language, but genetic and environmental Gershon, 2002), and chromosome 17q, gene. No large WGA studies have yet
complexity as well. Despite these chal- which has been replicated at genome- been published in ASD, but studies of

Cell 135, October 31, 2008 ©2008 Elsevier Inc.  391


common variant association in link- rare, large, affected other organ systems fully clarify the contribution of rare sin-
age regions suggest that analyses per- in addition to the central nervous system, gle-nucleotide variants to ASD. It should
formed will be underpowered and that or simply seemed to be special cases, be emphasized that the contribution of
at least a doubling in sample size (many this mechanism was largely unappreci- inherited CNVs as a source of more com-
thousands of cases, similar to studies of ated as a potential cause of idiopathic mon genetic contributions to ASD has
type I diabetes) will be needed to iden- autism. The CNVs identified by Sebat et not been explored in depth. This will be
tify more than a few loci at genome-wide al. (2007) were composed of deletions important because some heritable CNVs
significant association. (70%) and duplications (30%) of DNA may have subtle phenotypic effects and
These results echo findings in other fragments ranging from 160 kb to several will contribute to common variations in
common diseases with a complex megabases in size, thus containing seg- cognition and behavior.
genetic basis, in which early underesti- ments from the size of a single gene to The occurrence of rare de novo muta-
mation of heterogeneity and overestima- chromosomal regions harboring a dozen tions in ASD raises important issues
tion of the magnitude of risk imparted or more genes. Such genomic level de regarding mechanisms causing muta-
by any given susceptibility allele led to novo mutational events were only found tions (Lupski, 2007). Paternal age is asso-
underestimates of sample sizes needed in 1% of control individuals and were ciated with increasing point mutations in
for adequate power to detect common all duplications rather than the typically sperm, and complex genetic conditions
variant associations. From this perspec- more deleterious deletions observed in associated with increasing paternal age
tive, the results of linkage and associa- ASD (Sebat et al., 2007). Remarkably, may have a higher percentage of new
tion studies in autism imply that attain- most of the CNVs were unique, providing mutations. New mutations may be par-
ing massive sample sizes through large an indication that a significant fraction ticularly pronounced in the offspring of
collaborative efforts and sample shar- of ASD may be accounted for by rare, older fathers, who may be a reservoir for
ing—for example, through the Autism essentially private, mutations in simplex such de novo events. Notably, advanced
Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) (one affected child) autism families. Sim- paternal age has recently been shown to
and the Autism Genome Project (AGP) ilarly, the large AGP linkage study identi- significantly increase risk for ASD in two
(Geschwind et al., 2001, Szatmari et al., fied a handful of rare, likely causal CNVs distinct patient samples (Cantor et al.,
2007)—will be necessary to successfully using a lower-resolution platform (Szat- 2007; Reichenberg et al., 2006). These
find many common susceptibility genes. mari et al., 2007). data suggest one of many potential
It is too early to predict with certainty mechanisms through which environmen-
Rare or De Novo Mutations in ASD from these data the contribution of de tal factors could play a role in creating
Perhaps the most remarkable advance in novo CNVs to ASD susceptibility. Larger de novo genetic events causing autism,
ASD genetics in the last year was driven sample sizes ascertained from indepen- that is, accumulation of mutations in the
by the earlier discovery that regional dent, clearly defined populations will be male germline. Such factors could occur
variations in gene copy number, either necessary to accurately define the role of in isolation or in conjunction with genetic
heritable or arising de novo (not seen CNVs. Sample characteristics are impor- susceptibility loci. In the latter case, cer-
in parents), are a significant source of tant: de novo mutations are observed tain inherited haplotypes, for example,
genetic variation in humans (Sebat et al., more frequently in those with more could render specific regions more vul-
2004). Copy number variation (CNV) is a severe intellectual disability or dysmor- nerable to mutagens, thus increasing the
form of structural variation in the genome phology (Jacquemont et al., 2006). The frequency of mutational events. Alterna-
in which there is a gain or loss in a chro- contribution of de novo CNVs is also sig- tively, certain regions may be more vul-
mosomal region greater than 1 kilobase nificantly less in multiplex families hav- nerable to other environmental factors
(kb) in size, in contrast to the more com- ing two or more autistic children (Sebat that could affect chromatin structure or
mon SNPs, which are changes at one et al., 2007), a finding confirmed in sub- gene expression, leading to epigenetic
base pair of DNA. Recently, Sebat et al. sequent studies (Weiss et al., 2008; Mar- causes of autism (Jiang et al., 2004).
(2007) identified de novo CNVs in 3% of shall et al., 2008). Because the number
autistic children from multiplex families of CNVs detected is clearly related to the Multigenic versus Major Gene
(having two or more affected members) resolution of the microarrays used, the Contributions
and in 10% of autistic children from contribution of known CNVs to autism Currently, the predominant genetic model
simplex families (having one child with is expected to increase beyond 10% supposes the presence of multigenic
ASD). These findings were presaged by as microarray probe density increases. inheritance of common polymorphisms
previous studies using lower-resolution Similarly, single base pair mutations contributing to autism risk in multiplex
methods that identified a number of in a few genes encoding the synaptic families (Abrahams and Geschwind,
large chromosomal anomalies associ- adhesion proteins neuroligins 3 and 4, 2008). At face value, the paucity of mul-
ated with ASD and mental retardation the voltage-gated calcium ion channel tigenerational pedigrees segregating
(Jacquemont et al., 2006), including a CaV1.2, the tumor suppressor PTEN, and ASD argues against Mendelian, more
duplicated region on chromosome 15q shank3, a cytoplasmic binding partner specifically, dominant inheritance. How-
identified more than a decade ago (Veen- of the neuroligins, have been identified in ever, Wigler and colleagues (Zhao et al.,
stra-Vanderweele et al., 2004). Perhaps rare cases of ASD. The advent of efficient 2007) recently reassessed this notion
because some of these mutations were partial genome sequencing will more based on the identification of rare CNVs

392  Cell 135, October 31, 2008 ©2008 Elsevier Inc.


as significant contributors to autism
genetic risk and the strikingly higher inci-
dence in males (4:1 male-female ratio).
The model is based on a formal analysis
of recurrence risk in multiplex families
and is consistent with a significant con-
tribution from two major risk categories:
low-risk families in which there is little
genetic loading for autism in other family
members but in which the proband car-
ries highly penetrant de novo mutations
(accounting for about half of the cases)
and higher-risk families consistent with
dominant inheritance in males (account-
ing for about one-third of cases). This
model fits the family data collected by
several groups if the high-risk alleles have
lower penetrance with respect to the ASD
phenotype in females. Future gene hunt-
ing efforts will provide an empirical test
of this model. Nevertheless, the current
data are consistent with the notion that
autism spectrum disorders are caused by
a higher proportion of rare mutations than
previously anticipated and that the con-
tribution of common variants will mostly
consist of alleles with small effect sizes.

Identification of Common Genetic


Variants
Moving from genetic linkage studies
to identifying the multiple causal vari-
ants that likely underlie chromosomal
regions with the strongest linkage sig- Figure 1. Common and Rare Genetic Variations and the Heterogeneity of Autism
nals remains a challenge. It may be that Susceptibility to autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may in some cases reflect the contribution of normal
rare mutations underlie these signals and variation in heritable, potentially distinct core components of autism (endophenotypes). This simplified
model (A) depicts how a variety of risk variants, large and small, may contribute to ASD in aggregate
identifying these will require large-scale (B). The notion of hetereogeneous genetic mechanisms acting in different patients is captured by three
resequencing of the entire region in large individual examples depicted by circles (C).
numbers of cases and controls. In sev- (A) Normal variations in language, social behavior, and repetitive-restrictive behavior are illustrated as
overlapping but with distinct functions (blue and orange demarcate the highest level of functioning and
eral cases, the identification of causal
progressive abnormality, respectively). In this scheme, moderate abnormality in all three areas is neces-
common variation has been illusory sary to be diagnosed with autism, but abnormality in one area, for example language, leads to a more cir-
despite strong overlying linkage signals, cumscribed condition, such as specific language impairment. Patients with Asperger’s syndrome would
supporting the potential role of multiple have normal language but would lie in the orange zone for the other two components. Each of these
heritable cognitive-behavioral components (endophenotypes), although genetically complex, is likely to
rare mutations or many common vari- be less heterogeneous than the syndrome of autism. These endophenotypes can be studied separately
ants, each of small effect size. to increase power to detect common genetic risk variants.
Even as the current strategies identify (B) The contribution of genetic or environmental factors is emphasized by size or position of each repre-
sentative weight. Mendelian mutations, such as de novo CNVs, are depicted by a large ball because they
reproducible underlying genetic variants, are considered causal in many cases and would tip the balance, placing an individual essentially into the
the question still remains to what aspect orange zone in all categories shown in (A) (pleiotropy), resulting in autism.
of the ASD phenotype are they related? The reality, however, is that there are few mutations that act alone or are fully causal (green circle
in C.) Many mutations, including those that are recurrent, are not fully penetrant, and some patients
Similarly, the striking phenotypic het- have more than one de novo CNV. Similarly, most mutations, even rare mutations such as those in
erogeneity and clinical variability even the gene encoding neuroligin 4, or recurrent mutations such as del (16p), show variable expressivity,
among twins suggest that distinct forms resulting in developmental delay or mental retardation in some individuals and autism in others. So,
the environment or other common variants that might modulate social behavior or language could be
of autism may exist or that distinct
tipping the balance toward autism in a child who would otherwise have had mild or moderate mental
genetic risk factors may be related to retardation (blue circle in C). In other cases, multiple common variants, each with a small effect and
specific phenotypic features. This notion related to different components, would push an individual toward the right of the distribution and
is brought into focus in the recent HMCA along with environmental or epigenetic factors would result in an individual with ASD (multicolored
circle in C). Each of these scenarios, or a combination of the three, may be acting in a given individual
study (Morrow et al., 2008), where rare, with autism. Defining the extent to which each contributes on a population level to ASD is a goal of
potentially disease-causing mutations in current genetic research.

Cell 135, October 31, 2008 ©2008 Elsevier Inc.  393


the gene Slc9a9 were increased in AGRE endophenotype and a 160 kb deletion functional categories clearly can cause
families with autism and epilepsy, but in CNTNAP2 was detected in a single ASD, but how? Do known mutations
not in those without epilepsy. Genotype- proband (Alarcón et al., 2008). Concur- converge on a few common molecular
phenotype correlations will become even rently, another group discovered rare pathways or do they represent diverse
more salient as attempts are made to causal de novo chromosomal variation biological etiologies, and if so, how does
produce relevant animal models, not to and point mutations in CNTNAP2 (Bak- disruption of such diverse functions
mention the needs of families undergo- kaloglu et al., 2008), and yet another result in the syndrome of autism? The
ing prognostic counseling in the future. identified common variation in CNTNAP2 answer to this question goes beyond the
Moreover, from a neurobiological per- potentially associated with ASD (Arking boundaries of the current data. Further-
spective, different aspects of human cog- et al., 2008), providing multiple converg- more, several of the known autism genes
nition and behavior are served by distinct ing lines of evidence for the involvement including NLGN4 clearly cause mental
brain regions, which are likely to be pat- of CNTNAP2 in ASD. In addition, Strauss retardation, and others such as the 16p11
terned and maintained by distinct genetic and colleagues had previously discovered CNV are associated with more general
factors. Thus, specific genetic risk factors a single rare recessive truncating muta- forms of developmental delay (Weiss et
may correspond to changes at the level of tion in CNTNAP2 that caused a syndrome al., 2008), perhaps more frequently than
specific brain structures or neural systems of focal epilepsy and neuronal migration they cause ASD. So how does disease
that contribute to autism, such as those abnormalities in affected individuals in specificity emerge?
serving language or social cognition, an Amish family (Strauss et al., 2006). Whatever the known molecular and
rather than the broad syndrome of autism Remarkably, the majority of affected chil- biological functions of ASD susceptibil-
itself (Geschwind and Levitt, 2007; Figure dren were also found to suffer from lan- ity genes, they must converge on the
1). These heritable components or endo- guage delay and ASD, further supporting disruption of function in brain regions
phenotypes involving language, social the role of CNTNAP2 genetic variation supporting language, social cognition,
responsiveness, or behavioral rigidity are related to language systems that are dis- and behavioral flexibility. This could
also observed at higher frequency in first- rupted in ASD. This work on CNTNAP2, involve focal gene expression of the spe-
degree relatives of autistic subjects and in which variation in the same gene may cific gene product during development;
can be measured as continuous, quanti- lead to distinct clinical phenotypes, fur- when the risk allele is expressed, there
tative variables. Compared with the cat- ther emphasizes that current notions of is disruption of the cortical and subcor-
egorical diagnosis of autism, approaches disease status based on clinical diagnos- tical brain networks supporting social
based on linking quantitative endophe- tic schema can create artificial boundar- responsiveness or language. Remark-
notypes to underlying genetic risk may ies between conditions that may share ably, this appears to be the case for
provide more power, as has been appre- similar genetic underpinnings. CNTNAP2, which is enriched in highly
ciated in other complex genetic condi- evolved, anterior regions of the develop-
tions. This quantitative trait locus (QTL) Connecting Genes to Brain and ing human cerebral cortex that overlap
approach has the additional benefits of Behavior with circuitry involved in the development
including unaffected relatives and the full Most mutations known to cause autism of joint attention (Alarcón et al., 2008), a
range of variation in a particular measured are de novo mutations often involving social precursor to language that is one
phenotype rather than the arbitrary cate- multiple genes or identified genetic syn- of the early behaviors disrupted in ASD.
gorical determination of affected (autistic) dromes. Common variants have been However, most known ASD sus-
and unaffected (not autistic), which has implicated in autism association, but ceptibility genes do not demonstrate
changed over time. We and others have most of these are either in small sam- such regionally restricted expression,
successfully used QTL mapping to iden- ples or have not been replicated. Nev- so other factors must also be operat-
tify chromosomal loci related to cognitive ertheless, common variations in several ing. The core areas affected in autism
endophenotypes, such as language, non- genes including EN2 (Benayed et al., involve rapid and coherent integration
verbal communication, and social cogni- 2005), the MET proto-oncogene (Camp- of information from multiple, higher-
tion (e.g., Alarcón et al., 2008). Moreover, bell et al., 2006), and others in addition to level association areas (Geschwind
because we postulate that these features CNTNAP2 either have been implicated in and Levitt, 2007). Such functions could
involving language, social behaviors, and large samples or independently repli- be easily perturbed by minor, but rela-
other behavioral or cognitive traits repre- cated. But none of these published asso- tively widespread disruptions in neu-
sent one end of a continuum, normal or ciations individually account for a large ral transmission, for example, due to
otherwise (Figure 1), they are likely to be fraction of the genetic risk for ASD. either subtle mis-wiring or synaptic
related to many different neuropsychiat- Considering these common variants dysfunction. Circuit mis-wiring could
ric and neurodevelopmental conditions, and the known rare mutations, autism be either local or long distance and
in addition to ASD. susceptibility genes appear to have many could be caused by myriad conditions
One sign of success comes from a distinct roles in neural development and such as neuronal migration abnormali-
recent high-density SNP association neuronal function, ranging from basic ties, disrupted axon pathfinding, loss
analysis of the chromosome 7q language metabolism, synaptic transmission, and or dysfunction of local inhibitory con-
QTL, in which a common allele of CNT- RNA splicing to neuronal migration. nections, or immature synaptic func-
NAP2 was associated with a language Mutations in genes implicated in these tion, all culminating in what has been

394  Cell 135, October 31, 2008 ©2008 Elsevier Inc.


referred to as a developmental discon- Acknowledgments Happe, F., and Frith, U. (2006). J. Autism Dev. Dis-
ord. 36, 5–25.
nection (Geschwind and Levitt, 2007).
Thus, one would expect to find subtle, The author thanks R. Cantor and B. Abrahams for
Jacquemont, M.L., Sanlaville, D., Redon, R.,
critical reading of the manuscript, G. Coppola for
widespread differences in many brain Raoul, O., Cormier-Daire, V., Lyonnet, S., Amiel,
the Figure, L. Hong for editorial assistance, and J., Le Merrer, M., Heron, D., de Blois, M.C., et al.
systems in subjects with ASD, even many colleagues for their collaborations, espe- (2006). J. Med. Genet. 43, 843–849.
those serving primary sensory func- cially the AGRE consortium and the AGP. We are
grateful to the families who participate in AGRE Jiang, Y.H., Sahoo, T., Michaelis, R.C., Bercovich,
tions, although these may not be the and funding from NIMH (RO1 MH64547, U54 D., Bressler, J., Kashork, C.D., Liu, Q., Shaffer,
direct cause of the core features of MH68172, R37 MH60233, P50 HD055784), Au- L.G., Schroer, R.J., Stockton, D.W., et al. (2004).
autism. Such abnormalities, however, tism Speaks, and the Simons Foundation. Am. J. Med. Genet. A. 131, 1–10.
may explain the differences in sensory Lupski, J.R. (2007). Nat. Genet. 39, S43–S47.
References
processing, motor function, and sen-
Marshall, C.R., Noor, A., Vincent, J.B., Lionel, A.C.,
sory-motor integration, in addition to Abrahams, B.S., and Geschwind, D.H. (2008). Nat. Feuk, L., Skaug, J., Shago, M., Moessner, R., Pin-
the more global processing differences Rev. Genet. 9, 341–355. Erratum: (2008). Nat. Rev. to, D., and Ren, Y. (2008). Am. J. Hum. Genet. 82,
that have been variably associated with Genet. 9, 493. 477–488.
ASD (e.g., Happe and Frith, 2006). Alarcón, M., Stone, J.L., Duvall, J.A., Abrahams, Morrow, E.M., Yoo, S.Y., Flavell, S.W., Kim, T.K.,
The concepts of focal versus dif- B.S., Sebat, J., Wigler, M., Nelson, S.F., Cantor, Lin, Y., Hill, R.S., Mukaddes, N.M., Balkhy, S.,
fuse circuit disruption are not mutually R.M., and Geschwind, D.H. (2008). Am. J. Hum. Gascon, G., Hashmi, A., et al. (2008). Science 321,
Genet. 82, 150–159. 218–223.
exclusive and both may cause different
forms of ASD. Moreover, any unifying Arking, D.E., Cutler, D.J., Brune, C.W., Teslovich, Reichenberg, A., Gross, R., Weiser, M., Bresnahan,
T.M., West, K., Ikeda, M., Rea, A., Guy, G., Lin, S., M., Silverman, J., Harlap, S., Rabinowitz, J., Shul-
framework for understanding autism will Cook, E.H.J., et al. (2008). Am. J. Hum. Genet. 82, man, C., Malaspina, D., Lubin, G., et al. (2006).
necessarily involve testing hypotheses in 160–164. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 1026–1032.
autism cases with many distinct known
Badner, J.A., and Gershon, E.S. (2002). Mol. Psy- Sebat, J., Lakshmi, B., Malhotra, D., Troge, J.,
etiologies. Now that we possess the chiatry 7, 56–66. Lese-Martin, C., Walsh, T., Yamrom, B., Yoon, S.,
tools to continue to identify the genes Krasnitz, A., Kendall, J., et al. (2007). Science 316,
Bakkaloglu, B., O’Roak, B.J., Louvi, A., Gupta, 445–449.
causing autism, the challenge is to inte-
A.R., Abelson, J.F., Morgan, T.M., Chawarska, K.,
grate these findings with the study of Klin, A., Ercan-Sencicek, G., Stillman, A.A., et al. Sebat, J., Lakshmi, B., Troge, J., Alexander, J.,
cellular physiology and brain anatomy (2008). Am. J. Hum. Genet. 82, 165–173. Young, J., Lundin, P., Maner, S., Massa, H., Walker,
M., Chi, M., et al. (2004). Science 305, 525–528.
and function to bridge the gap between Benayed, R., Gharani, N., Rossman, I., Mancuso,
genes and cognition. Given the role of V., Lazar, G., Kamdar, S., Bruse, S.E., Tischfield, Strauss, K.A., Puffenberger, E.G., Huentelman,
highly adapted language and social cog- S., Smith, B.J., Zimmerman, R.A., et al. (2005). M.J., Gottlieb, S., Dobrin, S.E., Parod, J.M.,
Am. J. Hum. Genet. 77, 851–868. Stephan, D.A., and Morton, D.H. (2006). N. Engl.
nition systems in autism, we also need to J. Med. 354, 1370–1377.
clearly consider the role of human-spe- Campbell, D.B., Sutcliffe, J.S., Ebert, P.J., Militer-
ni, R., Bravaccio, C., Trillo, S., Elia, M., Schneider, Szatmari, P., Paterson, A.D., Zwaigenbaum, L.,
cific cognitive specializations, carefully Roberts, W., Brian, J., Liu, X.Q., Vincent, J.B.,
C., Melmed, R., Sacco, R., et al. (2006). Proc. Natl.
integrating model system data with stud- Acad. Sci. USA 103, 16834–16839. Skaug, J.L., Thompson, A.P., Senman, L., et al.
ies in humans. From this perspective, (2007). Nat. Genet. 39, 319–328.
Cantor, R.M., Kono, N., Duvall, J.A., Alvarez-Re-
autism is paradigmatic of the challenge tuerto, A., Stone, J.L., Alarcón, M., Nelson, S.F., Veenstra-Vanderweele, J., Christian, S.L., and
facing those who wish to understand and Geschwind, D.H. (2005). Am. J. Hum. Genet. Cook, E.H., Jr. (2004). Annu. Rev. Genomics Hum.
diseases affecting higher cognition—the 76, 1050–1056. Genet. 5, 379–405.

challenge of integrating detailed molecu- Cantor, R.M., Yoon, J.L., Furr, J., and Lajonchere, Weiss, L.A., Shen, Y., Korn, J.M., Arking, D.E.,
lar knowledge with complex circuit func- C.M. (2007). Mol. Psychiatry 12, 419–421. Miller, D.T., Fossdal, R., Saemundsen, E., Stefans-
son, H., Ferreira, M.A.R., Green, T., et al. (2008). N.
tion in humans. As this challenge is met Geschwind, D.H., and Levitt, P. (2007). Curr. Opin. Engl. J. Med. 358, 667–675.
and our knowledge increases, leading to Neurobiol. 17, 103–111.
etiological understanding of the disor- Zhao, X., Leotta, A., Kustanovich, V., Lajonchere,
Geschwind, D.H., Sowinski, J., Lord, C., Iversen, C., Geschwind, D.H., Law, K., Law, P., Qiu, S.,
der, our concepts of disease boundaries P., Shestack, J., Jones, P., Ducat, L., and Spence, Lord, C., Sebat, J., et al. (2007). Proc. Natl. Acad.
are likely to change. S.J. (2001). Am. J. Hum. Genet. 69, 463–466. Sci. USA 104, 12831–12836.

Cell 135, October 31, 2008 ©2008 Elsevier Inc.  395

Anda mungkin juga menyukai